Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 09 September 2022
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Psychometric properties and factorial invariance of the Farsi version of the Stress Mindset Measure

  • Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran

The Stress Mindset Measure consists of eight items to assess whether individuals hold a stress-is-enhancing or a stress-is-debilitating mindset. The current research is a cross-sectional study and aimed to investigate the factor structure, internal consistency reliability, and construct and convergent validity of the Farsi version of the Stress Mindset Measure (SMM). Prior to conducting the study, forward and backward translations of the SMM were performed. Using the convenience sampling method, we recruited 400 none-clinical sample (161 men and 239 women; aged 18 to 69). We utilized SPSS version 24, Amos, and Mplus 7.1 software to analyze the data. Results revealed satisfactory reliability and validity indexes for the Farsi version of the Stress Mindset Measure. The internal consistency of the Farsi version of the Stress Mindset Measure was in the excellent range (α = 0.87). The results of the confirmatory factorial analysis revealed two factors of the Stress Mindset Measure instead of the single factor suggested by the previous studies (fitness indices for the two-factor model were RMSEA = 0.78, CFI = 0.96, and TLI = 0.94). Moreover, we found that the stress-is-debilitating mindset is positively associated with stress (r = 0.233), depression (r = 0.163), and anxiety (r = 0.197). However, this mindset has been found to have no significant relationship with cognitive strategies of emotion regulation and life satisfaction. Also, findings showed no significant correlation between the stress-is-enhancing mindsets and the other variables. The results of this study suggest that the Farsi SMM has proper psychometric properties to assess stress mindsets.

Introduction

Stress is defined as an experienced tension when individuals perceive that the demands from external events are beyond their coping capacity (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984; Lovallo, 2015). Numerous studies portraying the detrimental effects of stress have consolidated negative cultural narratives around stress, proposing that stress must be reduced or removed (Crum et al., 2020). Over the years, stress has been cited as causing cardiovascular diseases (Juster et al., 2010), brain aging (Jefferson et al., 2010), and cognitive impairments (Schwabe and Wolf, 2010). However, the new line of research in the science of stress has shed light on the positive outcomes of stress by introducing the concept of stress mindsets (Keller et al., 2012; Crum et al., 2013; Nabi et al., 2013). Mindsets are lenses that filter and categorize the information people receive every day. Mindsets determine how individuals experience, understand and respond to the surrounding stimuli (Dweck, 2013). Stress mindset, a recently introduced concept, refers to the belief about whether stress is enhancing or debilitating for cognitive, emotional and performance outcomes. This concept shifts our attention to the fundamental role of the belief and attitude toward the effects of stress on various aspects of our wellbeing. Correlational studies and randomized controlled trials on stress mindsets have demonstrated that the stress-is-enhancing mindset-believing that stress increases health, vitality, learning, growth, and performance- is linked to reduced symptoms of anxiety and depression, improved self-reported health and energy levels, greater life satisfaction, and performance at work (Crum et al., 2013, 2017). On the other side of the continuum, there is the stress-is-debilitating mindset, which holds the belief that stress negatively affects performance, health, and wellbeing. Stress-is-debilitating mindset is more prevalent in individuals (Clark, 2003; Kinman and Jones, 2005) since the mass media constantly underpins the negative ramifications of stress (Cohen et al., 2007). Specifically, research studies have revealed that the extent to which people believe that stress is debilitating is positively correlated with the rate of mortality and morbidity (Keller et al., 2012; Nabi et al., 2013). Conversely, further studies have displayed constructive consequences of stress (Podsakoff et al., 2007). For instance, it has been shown that introducing a stress-is-enhancing mindset improves physiological functioning (Jamieson et al., 2010, 2013), as well as escalating work performance and self-reported health (Crum et al., 2013).

One of the implications of this recent notion toward stress is that individuals can be placed on the spectrum of stress mindset with the stress-is-enhancing mindset on one side and the stress-is-debilitating on the other side. Further research studies have manipulated the stress mindset with the mission of increasing the extent to which people adhere to a stress-is-enhancing mindset (Crum et al., 2013, 2017). In their pioneering study, Crum et al. (2017) found that activating a stress-is-enhancing mindset increases dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate secretion, cognitive flexibility, positive affect, and attention toward positive stimuli. One of the crucial implications of this study is that the stress mindset can be manipulated through straightforward interventions. These interventions may be applicable to non-clinical issues such as managing stress during a pandemic (Hagger et al., 2020). Also, Crum et al. (2013) have elucidated that stress is not always enhancing, but it can be utilized to be enhancing. This notion is in line with the theory of mindsets, describing mindsets as individuals’ beliefs about fundamental attributes such as intelligence and personality- whether the person considers them to be fixed or malleable (Dweck et al., 1995; Dweck and Yeager, 2019). Studies have revealed that when people change their mindsets from fixed to growth, they experience improvements in their functions and achievements (Yeager et al., 2019).

Whether we want to manipulate the stress mindset or conduct a correlational study, we first need to determine the individuals’ position on the stress mindset spectrum. To this end, we need to use the Stress Mindset Measure (SMM). This measure is used to determine the subjective meaning people ascribe to stress and decide how stress influences their performance, health, and wellbeing (Crum et al., 2013). The SMM consists of eight items and asks the participants to show their agreement or disagreement with sentences about the effects of stress on learning and growth, vitality, performance, health, and productivity (Crum et al., 2013). Extensive studies have validated and utilized the SMM in various populations, such as college students (Crum et al., 2017; Goyer et al., 2018), firm employees (Crum et al., 2013, 2018), and Navy SEALs (Smith et al., 2020). Crum et al. (2013) created two versions of the measure in the validation study of SMM. The first version consisted of beliefs about the general nature of stress (SMM-G), while the second one consisted of stress when a specific stressor was present (SMM-S). These two versions were shown to be internally consistent (Cronbach’s alpha for the SMM-G was 0.86 and for the SMM-S was 0.80) and the confirmatory factor analyses confirmed the single structure of SMM.

Studies have shown that mindsets can lead us to a better understanding of the effects of stress on physical and mental health (Crum et al., 2017). Therefore, validating the measures that assess the stress mindsets is of critical importance. Although previous studies support the psychometric properties of the SMM in Western countries, the generalization of these results to countries with different cultures, such as Iran, can be problematic. This concern is salient in studying stress, given that the process that triggers and perpetuates stress must be evaluated from a cultural perspective (Han et al., 2022). Also, stress is experienced, described, and labeled in various ways in different cultures (Moheb and Ram, 2010; Shokri et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2021). Thus, it is vital to investigate the psychometric properties of the SMM in Iran. We designed this study to investigate the psychometric properties and factorial invariance of the SMM in Iranian culture.

In the present study, we aimed to provide a fluent Farsi translation of SMM and study the psychometric properties of the measure in the Iranian population. To adapt the SMM to the Iranian context and study the reliability and validity of the measure for assessing the stress mindsets, we translated the measure into Farsi, hired 400 none-clinical participants and analyzed the data for inter-item correlations, confirmatory factor analysis, internal consistency reliability, and factorial invariance.

Materials and methods

Procedure

The current research was a cross-sectional study. To determine the sample size, we based our work on research studies suggesting having at least a 10:1 ratio of participants to an item for factor analysis (Everitt, 1975; Costello and Osborne, 2005). Given that the SMM has 8 items, the minimum sample size for our study was estimated to be 80. Considering that the overall number of participants was 400, it can be concluded that the ultimate sample size was appropriate.

We recruited participants via advertisements of social networks and provided information about the study. Data was gathered online, using Porsline, an Iranian platform for creating online questionnaires and conducting studies in the field of social sciences.

Participants

Participants responded to the SMM questions after giving consent and answering some demographic questions. The inclusion criteria were: (a) at least 18 years of age and older, (b) being able to speak and read in Farsi, and (c) no history of severe mental and physical disorders. The exclusion criteria were: (a) answering the questions incompletely.

Translation

All SMM items were translated into Farsi using the standard back translation technique (Brislin, 1970). Specifically, the first and the second authors translated the SMM into Farsi from the original English version. In the next step, two independent translators translated the SMM back into English. Before using the translated measure in the research procedure, we sent the translation files to the mind and body lab at Stanford University, directed by Alia Crum, the creator of the SMM. After being confirmed and published on the mind and body lab website, we used the Farsi translation in the study.

Measures

In the present research, we used three other measures to investigate the criterion and concurrent validity of the Farsi version of the SMM. For this purpose, we used the following measures:

1. Stress Mindset Measure (SMM; Crum et al., 2013). In this measure, eight items are presented to the participants and they are asked to show their agreement or disagreement to them on a five-point Likert scale (from 0 = strongly disagree to 4 = strongly agree). Four items assess the stress-is-enhancing mindsets (e.g., Experiencing stress facilitates my learning and growth) and the four others assess the stress-is-debilitating mindset (e.g., The effects of stress are negative and should be avoided). Cronbach’s alpha for this measure is 0.87.

2. Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al., 1985; Farsi version: Bayani et al., 2007). This scale uses five items (e.g., In most ways, my life is close to my ideals) and a seven-point Likert scale (from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree) to assess the reported level of satisfaction in life. The reported Cronbach’s alpha for the Farsi version is 0.83. In this study, we found Cronbach’s alpha of 0.826 for the scale.

3. Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21; Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995; Farsi version: Samani and Joukar, 2007). In this scale, the participants report the presence of depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms in the last week (e.g., I was aware of dryness in my mouth) on a Likert scale (from 0 = did not apply to me at all to 3 = applied to me very much or most of the times). In the study conducted by Samani and Joukar (2007), high internal consistency levels were found for the scales (α = 0.81, α = 0.74, and α = 0.78 respectively). We found Cronbach’s alpha of 0.845 for stress, 0.813 for anxiety, and 0.837, for depression.

4. Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross and John, 2003; Farsi version: Qasempour et al., 2012). This 10-item self-report questionnaire assesses the use of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression as common strategies to alter emotions. Participants respond to each item (e.g., I control my emotions by not expressing them) using a seven-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree). Our results revealed Cronbach’s alpha of 0.706 for reappraisal and 0.740 for expressive suppression strategies.

Confirmatory factor analysis was applied to assess the structure of the SMM. We used maximum likelihood estimation for assessing the parameters of the assumed model. We investigated the fitness of the model by the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TCI), Goodness of Fit Index (GFI), Expected Cross-Validation Index (ECVI), Normed Fit Index (NFI), and Incremental Fit Index (IFI). We measured factorial invariance of the SMM between men and women by multigroup confirmatory factor analysis (Kline, 2015). Also, internal consistency was evaluated by Cronbach’s alpha coefficient and convergent validity with other scales was assessed by Pearson’s correlation.

Results

This study was conducted by hiring 400 healthy individuals (aged 18 to 69). 239 of the participants were women (59.8%) and 161 were men (40.3%). The mean age of the participants was 31.89 (SD = 10.80).

Inter-item correlations

First, we examined the inter-item correlations among the eight SMM items. After reversing the negative items, the findings indicated that the inter-item correlations ranged from 0.31 to 0.62 (see Table 1). According to de Vaus (2004), the inter-item correlations between 0.30 and 0.70 were considered acceptable. Results reveal proper item homogeneity and SMM items measure the same concept.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Stress Mindset Measure inter-item correlations.

Confirmatory factor analysis

Confirmatory factor analysis was conducted by Amos version 24 to examine the construct validity of the measure. According to the recommended model designed by Crum et al. (2013), in which all the eight items loaded on a single stress mindset factor, a first-order single-factor model was created. Next, we examined a first-order two-factor model based on the two elements of the SMM (see Figure 1). Four items (q2, q4, q6, and q8) loaded on a factor that evaluated stress-is-enhancing mindset. The other four items (q1, q3, q5, and q7) loaded on a factor that measures the stress-is-debilitating mindset. We used various indexes to assess the fitness of the models. First, we analyzed the Chi-square ratio. The Chi-square ratios lower than three indicate the goodness of fit of the model (Kline, 2010). Also, we examined other indexes such as the expected cross-validation index (ECVI; Schreiber et al., 2006); Tucker-Lewis index (TLI; Tucker and Lewis, 1973), the goodness of fit index (GFI; Joreskog and Sorbom, 1984), the comparative fit index (CFI; Bentler, 1990), and the root means square error of approximation. Based on previous studies, RMSEA values below 0.08 indicate a good model fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999). Further, CFI, GFI, and TLI values greater than 0.90 showed acceptable model fit, whereas values higher than 0.95 display great model fit. It should be noted that ECVI is mainly used to compare various models and the smaller values indicate better model fit.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The two-factor solution model for the Farsi version of the SMM.

Our findings revealed that the Chi-square ratio was higher than three for both models due to the big sample size. For the single-factor model, CFI, GFI, TLI, NFI, and IFI values were greater than 0.90, which indicates a good model fit. Also, CFI, GFI, and IFI values for the two-factor model were 0.96, indicating a great model fit. Given that for the two-factor model, CFI, TLI, GFI, and NFI values were higher than 0.90, and RMSEA value was less than 0.08, and ECVI values were smaller than the first-factor model. It can be concluded that the fit indices for the two-factor model are better than the single-factor model (see Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Fit indices in confirmatory factor analysis for a single-factor and a two-factor.

Internal consistency reliability

We calculated the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient to evaluate the internal consistency reliability of the two factors and the total score of the SMM. The total alpha score for the SMM was 0.87, which indicates high internal consistency reliability. Also, the alpha coefficient for the stress-is-enhancing mindset was 0.81 and for the stress-is-debilitating mindset was 0.78 indicating great internal consistency.

Factorial invariance

To investigate the factorial invariance of the SMM, we employed multi-group confirmatory factor analysis between men and women. We used three models of invariance for the analysis. The configural model considers the structure and pattern of the factors constant. In the metric model, factor loadings between groups are considered equivalent as well as the structures. As it is apparent in Table 3, Chi-square is not significant when comparing the metric model with the configural model. Also, the ΔCFI value is lower than the cut point, which is 0.01, indicating metric factorial invariance between men and women. In the scalar model, the intercepts between two groups are considered equivalent, as well as the structure and factor loadings. Chi-square not being significant and the small values of ΔCFI in the scalar model compared to the metric model shows the scalar factorial invariance in men and women (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Factorial invariance across gender for the SMM model.

Convergent validity

In the final step, we tested the correlation of the SMM items with the pertinent positive and negative concepts (see Table 4). Results show that the stress-is-debilitating mindset is positively associated with stress (r = 0.233), depression (r = 0.163), and anxiety (r = 0.197). However, this mindset has been found to have no significant relationship with cognitive strategies of emotion regulation and life satisfaction. Also, findings displayed no significant correlation between the stress-is-enhancing mindsets and the other variables.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Bivariate correlations between DAS, ERQ, and SWLS with the SMM subscales.

Discussion

The current research aimed to examine the psychometric properties and factorial invariance of the Farsi version of the SMM. The results depicted that the Farsi translation of the SMM conveys the concepts that the creators of the measure intended. However, our findings showed a different structure of the SMM in Iran. Contrary to the single stress mindset factor that Crum et al. (2013) proposed, we identified two factors. These factors indicated individuals’ beliefs about the effects of stress, whether stress-is-enhancing or stress-is-debilitating. In other words, we found that these two mindsets are independent rather than the constituents of a universal stress mindset. One explanation for this could be the reversed scoring of the stress-is-debilitating items. Studies have revealed that utilizing positive items as well as reversed items creates secondary sources of variance and this hinders the unidimensionality of the scale (Suárez Álvarez et al., 2018). In this vein, the results of a study investigating the psychometric properties of the SMM in a Greek sample have also identified two factors instead of a single stress mindset factor (Karampas et al., 2020).

The first factor, “Enhancing Stress,” includes items that capture the belief that stress has enhanced consequences for various stress-related outcomes such as performance and productivity, health and wellbeing, and learning and growth. It includes four items with significant factor loadings and high internal consistency. The second factor, “Debilitating Stress,” reflects the belief that stress has debilitating consequences for those outcomes. All four items had significant factor loadings on their underlying constructs and satisfactory internal consistency. The results showed a high correlation between Enhancing and Debilitating Stress, with coefficients of 0.87. So our results revealed that stress mindset is a variable with two distinct categories that influence the stress response.

Also, we found adequate configural, metric, and scalar invariance of the instrument in the analysis of factorial invariance. These results show that the Farsi Stress Mindset Measure evaluates the stress mindset with the exact structure and meaningfully across men and women and the total sample.

Moreover, findings revealed that the stress-is-debilitating mindset is positively associated with stress, depression, and anxiety, meaning that people who hold a stress-is-enhancing mindset are more likely to suffer from the symptoms of these disorders. More specifically, the stress-is-debilitating mindset had the strongest correlation with the stress symptoms. Stress-is-enhancing mindset, on the other hand, was not associated with the symptoms of mental disorders. These results are in accordance with the prior studies revealing that individuals with a stress-is-debilitating mindset (not a stress-is-enhancing mindset) are at a higher risk of experiencing mental health concerns (Keech et al., 2018; Huebschmann and Sheets, 2020).

This study had a few limitations. First, because the data collection procedure took place in the university context and participants were selected from the university students’ population, the findings will only indicate the stress mindset of a restricted portion of the Iranian population. Therefore, conducting a similar study using a more inclusive sample is recommended. Second, we did not ask questions about the ethnicity and cultural background of the participants. Although all participants were Iranian, their ethnicities were not specified. Given that there are various ethnicities and linguistic backgrounds coexist in Iran, the lack of these data could be considered a limitation. Investigating the psychometric properties of the SMM in other cultures and languages in Iran is recommended for future studies.

Based on our results, we can consider the SMM an appropriate tool to measure stress mindsets. The Farsi version of the SMM could be applied to assess the stress mindset in both research and clinical realms.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the results of the present study indicate that the Farsi version of the SMM is a valid and reliable tool to measure the stress mindset in the Iranian population. Also, our findings corroborate the notion that the stress mindset can determine the psychological symptoms that individuals experience when they are stressed.

Data availability statement

The data and materials that support the findings of this study are openly available in figshare at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.19362326.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the University of Tehran, Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

YT and YD performed the translation, material preparation, and data collection. HK analyzed the data and collaborated in writing the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the study conception and design, read, and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Bayani, A. A., Kouchaki, A. M., and Goudarzi, H. (2007). Validity and reliability of the scale of life satisfaction. Iran. J. Psychol. 3, 259–265.

Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol. Bull. 107:238. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 1, 185–216. doi: 10.1177/135910457000100301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, A. M. (2003). ‘It’s like an explosion in your life…‘: Lay perspectives on stress and myocardial infarction. J. Clin. Nurs. 12, 544–553. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2702.2003.00740.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., and Miller, G. E. (2007). Psychological stress and disease. JAMA 298, 1685–1687.

Google Scholar

Costello, A. B., and Osborne, J. (2005). Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 10:7. doi: 10.7275/jyj1-4868

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crum, A. J., Akinola, M., Martin, A., and Fath, S. (2017). The role of stress mindset in shaping cognitive, emotional, and physiological responses to challenging and threatening stress. Anxiety Stress Coping 30, 379–395. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2016.1275585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crum, A. J., Akinola, M., Turnwald, B. P., Kaptchuk, T. J., and Hall, K. T. (2018). Catechol-O-Methyltransferase moderates effect of stress mindset on affect and cognition. PLoS One 13:e0195883. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0195883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crum, A. J., Jamieson, J. P., and Akinola, M. (2020). Optimizing stress: An integrated intervention for regulating stress responses. Emotion 20:120. doi: 10.1037/emo0000670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crum, A. J., Salovey, P., and Achor, S. (2013). Rethinking stress: The role of mindsets in determining the stress response. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 104:716. doi: 10.1037/a0031201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vaus, D. A. (2004). “Structured questionnaires and interviews,” in Handbook of research methods for nursing and health science, 2nd Edn, (Lonon: Pearson Education).

Google Scholar

Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dweck, C. S. (2013). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Dweck, C. S., and Yeager, D. S. (2019). Mindsets: A view from two eras. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 14, 481–496. doi: 10.1177/1745691618804166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C. Y., and Hong, Y. Y. (1995). Implicit theories and their role in judgments and reactions: A word from two perspectives. Psychol. Inq. 6, 267–285. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0604_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everitt, B. S. (1975). Multivariate analysis: The need for data, and other problems. Br. J. Psychiatry 126, 237–240. doi: 10.1192/bjp.126.3.237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goyer, J. P., Akinola, M., Grunberg, R., and Crum, A. J. (2018). Evaluation of a stress mindset intervention to improve performance and wellbeing in underrepresented minority college students at a selective institution. Manuscript in preparation.

Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., and John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S., Keech, J. J., and Hamilton, K. (2020). Managing stress during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic and beyond: Reappraisal and mindset approaches. Stress Health 36, 396–401. doi: 10.1002/smi.2969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, J. Y., Lee, H., Ohtsubo, Y., and Masuda, T. (2022). Culture and stress coping: Cultural variations in the endorsement of primary and secondary control coping for daily stress across European Canadians, East Asian Canadians, and the Japanese. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 64, 141–155. doi: 10.1111/jpr.12406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huebschmann, N. A., and Sheets, E. S. (2020). The right mindset: Stress mindset moderates the association between perceived stress and depressive symptoms. Anxiety Stress Coping 33, 248–255. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2020.1736900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., and Nock, M. K. (2013). Improving acute stress responses: The power of reappraisal. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 22, 51–56. doi: 10.1177/0963721412461500

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., Blackstock, E., and Schmader, T. (2010). Turning the knots in your stomach into bows: Reappraising arousal improves performance on the GRE. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 208–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jefferson, A. L., Himali, J. J., Beiser, A. S., Au, R., Massaro, J. M., Seshadri, S., et al. (2010). Cardiac index is associated with brain aging: The Framingham Heart Study. Circulation 122, 690–697. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.109.905091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joreskog, K. G., and Sorbom, D. (1984). LISRELVI. Analysis of linear structural relationships by the method of maximum likelihood. Mooresville, NC: Scientific Software.

Google Scholar

Juster, R. P., McEwen, B. S., and Lupien, S. J. (2010). Allostatic load biomarkers of chronic stress and impact on health and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 2–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karampas, K., Pezirkianidis, C., and Stalikas, A. (2020). Psychometric properties of the Stress Mindset Measure (SMM) in a Greek sample. Psychology 11:1185. doi: 10.4236/psych.2020.118079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keech, J. J., Hagger, M. S., O’Callaghan, F. V., and Hamilton, K. (2018). The influence of university students’ stress mindsets on health and performance outcomes. Ann. Behav. Med. 52, 1046–1059. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, A., Litzelman, K., Wisk, L. E., Maddox, T., Cheng, E. R., Creswell, P. D., et al. (2012). Does the perception that stress affects health matter? The association with health and mortality. Health Psychol. 31:677. doi: 10.1037/a0026743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinman, G., and Jones, F. (2005). Lay representations of workplace stress: What do people really mean when they say they are stressed? Work Stress 19, 101–120. doi: 10.1080/02678370500144831

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice for structural equation modelling, 3rd Eds Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.

Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Lee, H., Masuda, T., Ishii, K., Yasuda, Y., and Ohtsubo, Y. (2021). Cultural differences in the perception of daily stress between European Canadian and Japanese undergraduate students. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 48:01461672211070360. doi: 10.1177/01461672211070360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovallo, W. R. (2015). Stress and health: Biological and psychological interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, doi: 10.4135/9781452233543

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, P. F., and Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 335–343. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moheb, N., and Ram, U. (2010). Cross-cultural study of stress and anger. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 5, 1765–1769. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.07.361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nabi, H., Kivimäki, M., Batty, G. D., Shipley, M. J., Britton, A., Brunner, E. J., et al. (2013). Increased risk of coronary heart disease among individuals reporting adverse impact of stress on their health: The Whitehall II prospective cohort study. Eur. Heart J. 34, 2697–2705. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/eht216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsakoff, N. P., LePine, J. A., and LePine, M. A. (2007). Differential challenge stressor-hindrance stressor relationships with job attitudes, turnover intentions, turnover, and withdrawal behavior: A meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 92:438. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.2.438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qasempour, A., Ilbeigi, R., and Hassanzadeh, Z. (2012). “Psychometric characteristics of Grass and Jan’s emotional regulation questionnaire in an Iranian sample,” in Proceedings of the 6th Gilan University Student Mental Health Conference, Gilan, 722–724.

Google Scholar

Samani, S., and Joukar, B. (2007). A study on the reliability and validity of the short form of the depression anxiety stress scale (DASS-21). J. Soc. Sci. Hum. 26, 65–77.

Google Scholar

Schreiber, J. B., Nora, A., Stage, F. K., Barlow, E. A., and King, J. (2006). Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis results: A review. J. Educ. Res. 99, 323–338. doi: 10.3200/JOER.99.6.323-338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwabe, L., and Wolf, O. T. (2010). Learning under stress impairs memory formation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 93, 183–188. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shokri, O., Farahani, M. N., Kormi Nouri, R., and Moradi, A. R. (2012). Perceived social support and academic stress: The roles of gender and cultural differences. Dev. Psychol. 9, 143–156.

Google Scholar

Smith, E. N., Young, M. D., and Crum, A. J. (2020). Stress, mindsets, and success in Navy SEALs special warfare training. Front. Psychol. 10:2962. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suárez Álvarez, J., Pedrosa, I., Lozano, L. M., García Cueto, E., Cuesta Izquierdo, M., and Muñiz Fernández, J. (2018). Using reversed items in Likert scales: A questionable practice. Psicothema 30, 149–158. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2018.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker, L. R., and Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika 38, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/BF02291170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeager, D. S., Hanselman, P., Walton, G. M., Murray, J. S., Crosnoe, R., Muller, C., et al. (2019). A national experiment reveals where a growth mindset improves achievement. Nature 573, 364–369. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1466-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stress mindset, Stress Mindset Measure, psychometric properties, factorial invariance, validity, reliability

Citation: Tedadi Y, Daryani Y and Karsazi H (2022) Psychometric properties and factorial invariance of the Farsi version of the Stress Mindset Measure. Front. Psychol. 13:945673. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.945673

Received: 16 May 2022; Accepted: 25 August 2022;
Published: 09 September 2022.

Edited by:

Noureddin Nakhostin Ansari, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Iran

Reviewed by:

Wanich Suksatan, Chulabhorn Royal Academy, Thailand
Reza Ghanei Gheshlagh, Kurdistan University of Medical Sciences, Iran

Copyright © 2022 Tedadi, Daryani and Karsazi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yaser Tedadi, mahvid.bb@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.