Event Abstract

Mirror neurons, action understanding and social interaction: implications for educational neuroscience

  • 1 Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, United Kingdom
  • 2 University of Oxford, United Kingdom

Mirror neurons are cells which fire to both the observation and execution of actions (di Pellegrino et al., 1992). Originally found in premotor area F5 of the macaque monkey, they have subsequently been found in additional brain areas including the inferior parietal lobule and dorsal premotor cortex (Fogassi et al., 2005; Tkach et al., 2007). In humans, converging neuroscientific evidence for the presence of mirror neurons (Fadiga et al., 1995; Iacoboni et al., 1999; Kilner et al., 2009) has led some researchers to claim that mirror neurons are involved in a process known as ‘action understanding’ (Binkofski & Buccino, 2006). However, what is meant by the term ‘action understanding’ is disputed (Cook et al., 2014; Hickok, 2009), and this has left considerable confusion in the literature, regarding both what ‘action understanding’ refers to, and whether mirror neurons are involved in such a process. Below, we outline our concerns regarding these two points; and emphasise the importance of clarifying not only the evidence for mirror neuron involvement in ‘action understanding’, but also the definition of ’action understanding’, before proposing that either mirror neurons or ‘action understanding’ should be targets for educational neuroscience interventions (Zhu et al., 2011). ‘Action understanding’ has been defined as the ability to identify others’ actions, infer the goal of their actions, and extract the intention underlying their actions (Gallese et al., 1996). This definition suggests that ‘action understanding’ can be separated into three distinct processes: identifying the configuration of body parts involved in an action; identifying its goal; and identifying the actor’s intention. However, these three processes differ in terms of the amount of generalization required across action features. More specifically, identifying an action requires discrimination of the perceptual features (such as the type of grip used: e.g. precision or whole-hand), based on the configural relationship between different body parts. Identifying goals and intentions, however, requires generalization across the perceptual features of observed actions. This is because a goal (e.g. “to grasp”) or intention (e.g. “in order to drink”) can be achieved using a range of different grip types; and, crucially, the same grip type can be used to achieve a number of different goals and intentions. Since there is not a one-to-one correspondence between body part configurations, goals, and intentions (e.g. Jacob & Jeannerod, 2005), the same pattern of mirror neuronal firing cannot simultaneously represent the other’s action, their goal, and their intention. Although this has been acknowledged in the literature (Kilner, 2011), researchers investigating the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying ‘action understanding’ typically focus on only one of the above processes (e.g. Fogassi et al., 2005; Spunt et al., 2011; Umiltà et al., 2001); but problematically, all three processes are referred to as ‘action understanding’. The non-specific use of the term ‘action understanding’ has led to contradictory results regarding whether mirror neurons (or, in human studies, brain areas thought to contain mirror neurons) are involved in ‘action understanding’. For example, some researchers have claimed that brain regions containing mirror neurons are involved in identifying others’ intentions (Michael et al., 2014). Michael et al. found that disruption to the premotor cortex by continuous theta burst stimulation impaired accuracy on a task involving the analysis of the actor’s intention. In this task, participants had to combine information on the configuration of a hand with contextual information in order to deduce the actor’s intention. However, closer inspection of the results showed that accuracy on a purely perceptual task, in which the participant matched the configuration of a hand depicted in a video to a hand depicted in an image, was impaired to the same extent as the intention task. If mirror neurons are involved in identifying intentions, then stimulation should have reduced accuracy on the task involving the analysis of intentions to a greater extent than the purely perceptual task. Therefore, the results only provide convincing evidence for the role of mirror neurons in the identification of the perceptual features of actions (see Catmur, 2014). Similarly, other researchers have claimed to have found evidence that mirror neurons are able to distinguish between different goals (Hafri et al., 2017). However, other evidence has demonstrated that mirror neuron brain areas encode different exemplars of actions based on their perceptual features (Nicholson et al., 2017), suggesting that mirror neuron areas may only appear to encode the goals of observed actions when those goals are perceptually distinct. Moreover, generalization across the perceptual features of observed actions appears to occur in other, non-motor, brain areas, such as the lateral occipitotemporal cortex (Wurm et al., 2016; Wurm & Lingnau, 2015) or the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (Spunt & Adolphs, 2014; Spunt & Lieberman, 2013), which are not thought to contain mirror neurons. This summary of the literature suggests that there is some evidence to support mirror neuron involvement in identification of the configuration of body parts involved in an observed action, but not in identifying the goal or the intention of the action. However, since the current definition of ‘action understanding’ does not distinguish between these processes, whether this evidence supports mirror neuron involvement in ‘action understanding’ clearly depends on which particular sub-definition of this term is adopted. The possible involvement of mirror neurons in ‘action understanding’ is particularly important for educational neuroscience. Both the psychological process implied by the term ‘action understanding’, and its potential neural underpinnings in terms of mirror neurons, have the potential to be targeted in educational interventions: most notably, for those with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). ASD is a neurodevelopmental disorder involving deficits in social communication and social interaction, and restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior (DSM-5). Perhaps the most striking social difficulty in ASD, a deficit in theory of mind (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985), has been associated – at least theoretically – with the concept of ‘action understanding’ (Koster-Hale & Saxe, 2013). Certain researchers have therefore proposed that mirror neurons are a possible neural substrate for these social abilities (Gallese, 2003), with impairments in mirror neuron functioning playing a role in the social deficits seen in ASD (e.g. Khalil et al., 2018; Oberman et al., 2005). However, the link between the term 'action understanding' and social abilities such as theory of mind is theoretical rather than empirical. Moreover, as discussed above, the term itself refers to three distinct processes, all of which are likely to have different associations with specific social abilities. For example, ASD has been associated with difficulties in identifying the intentions underlying observed actions but not with difficulties in identifying the actions themselves (Boria et al., 2009). If, as proposed above, mirror neuron involvement in ‘action understanding’ is limited to the identification of the configuration of body parts in an observed action, and not in the identification of the goal or intention underlying that action, then these data are not consistent with the proposed association between mirror neuron dysfunction and the social symptoms of ASD. Despite the unsubstantiated nature of the links between mirror neurons, ‘action understanding’, and social ability, educational interventions targeted at altering mirror neuron firing have been designed in an attempt to improve the social abilities of individuals with ASD. One such intervention is neurofeedback therapy, which uses operant conditioning to alter brain responses. Neurofeedback interventions targeting mirror neuron function have focused on the mu rhythm, an 8–12 Hz electroencephalographic frequency band recorded from sensorimotor cortex that is proposed to index mirror neuron activity (e.g. Hari, 2006). While some researchers have claimed success with this intervention (Pineda et al., 2014), at the meta-analytical level there is no support for its effectiveness (Holtmann et al., 2011). Indeed, debate is ongoing regarding whether neurofeedback therapy does target mu activity (Hobson & Bishop, 2017); whether mu activity is associated with mirror neurons (Hobson & Bishop, 2016); and whether mirror neurons are indeed linked to deficits in social abilities (Enticott et al., 2013). In this context, therefore, we argue that the first step in establishing whether educational neuroscience interventions should target mirror neuron function should be to determine the role of mirror neurons in each of the three processes currently subsumed under the term ‘action understanding’. The next step is to determine the association between each of those processes and social abilities, including theory of mind. Unless mirror neurons are involved in a process that contributes to such social abilities, educational interventions targeting mirror neuron activation, such as neurofeedback therapy, are unlikely to be productive. Therefore, until a clearer definition of ‘action understanding’ is agreed, and until mirror neuron involvement in such a process is clarified, researchers and clinicians should be wary of educational interventions targeting such factors.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Leverhulme Trust [grant number PLP-2015-019] awarded to C.C. G.B. is funded by the Baily Thomas Charitable Trust.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M., Frith, U., 1985. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21, 7–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8 Binkofski, F., Buccino, G., 2006. The role of ventral premotor cortex in action execution and action understanding. J. Physiol. Paris 99, 396–405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.005 Boria, S., Fabbri-destro, M., Cattaneo, L., Sparaci, L., Sinigaglia, C., Cossu, G., Rizzolatti, G., 2009. Intention understanding in autism. PLoS One 4, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0005596 Catmur, C., 2014. Unconvincing support for the role of mirror neurons in “action understanding”: commentary on Michael et al. (2014). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 553. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2805 Cook, R., Bird, G., Catmur, C., Press, C., Heyes, C., 2014. Mirror neurons: from origin to function. Behav. Brain Sci. 37, 177–92. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13000903 di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G., 1992. Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Exp. Brain Res. 91, 176–180. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00230027 Enticott, P.G., Kennedy, H.A., Rinehart, N.J., Bradshaw, J.L., Tonge, B.J., Daskalakis, Z.J., Fitzgerald, P.B., 2013. Interpersonal motor resonance in autism spectrum disorder: evidence against a global “mirror system” deficit. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00218 Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., Rizzolatti, G., 1995. Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. J.Neurophysiol. 73, 2608–2611. https://doi.org/10.1.1.299.4524 Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P.F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., Rizzolatti, G., 2005. Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science (80-. ). 308, 662–667. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1106138 Gallese, V., 2003. The roots of empathy: The shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology 36, 171–180. https://doi.org/10.1159/000072786 Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Rizzolatti, G., 1996. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119, 593–609. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.2.593 Hafri, A., Trueswell, J.C., Epstein, R.A., 2017. Neural representations of observed actions generalize across static and dynamic visual input. J. Neurosci. 37, 3056–3071. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2496-16.2017 Hari, R., 2006. Action – perception connection and the cortical mu rhythm. Prog. Brain Res. 159. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(06)59017-X Hickok, G., 2009. Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1229–1243. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21189 Hobson, H.M., Bishop, D.V.M., 2017. The interpretation of mu suppression as an index of mirror neuron activity: past, present and future. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4, 1–22. Hobson, H.M., Bishop, D.V.M., 2016. Mu suppression - A good measure of the human mirror neuron system? Cortex 82, 290–310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.019 Holtmann, M., Steiner, S., Hohmann, S., Poustka, L., Banaschewski, T., Bölte, S., 2011. Neurofeedback in autism spectrum disorders. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 53, 986–993. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04043.x Iacoboni, M., Woods, P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J.C., Rizzolatti, G., 1999. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science (80-. ). 286, 2526–2528. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.286.5449.2526 Jacob, P., Jeannerod, M., 2005. The motor theory of social cognition: A critique. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 21–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.003 Khalil, R., Tindle, R., Boraud, T., Moustafa, A.A., Karim, A.A., 2018. Social decision making in autism: On the impact of mirror neurons, motor control, and imitative behaviors. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 24, 669–676. https://doi.org/10.1111/cns.13001 Kilner, J.M., 2011. More than one pathway to action understanding. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 352–357. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.06.005 Kilner, J.M., Neal, A., Weiskopf, N., Friston, K.J., Frith, C.D., 2009. Evidence of mirror neurons in human inferior frontal gyrus. J. Neurosci. 29, 10153–10159. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2668-09.2009 Koster-Hale, J., Saxe, R., 2013. Theory of Mind : A Neural Prediction Problem. Neuron 79, 836–848. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.020.Theory Michael, J., Sandberg, K., Skewes, J., Wolf, T., Blicher, J., Overgaard, M., Frith, C.D., 2014. Continuous theta-burst stimulation demonstrates a causal role of premotor homunculus in action understanding. Psychol Sci 25, 963–972. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613520608 Nicholson, T., Roser, M., Bach, P., 2017. Understanding the goals of everyday instrumental actions is primarily linked to object, not motor-kinematic, information: Evidence from fMRI. PLoS One 12, 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169700 Oberman, L.M., Hubbard, E.M., Mccleery, J.P., Altschuler, E.L., Ramachandran, V.S., Pineda, J.A., 2005. EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 190–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.014 Pineda, J.A., Friedrich, E.V.C., Lamarca, K., 2014. Neurorehabilitation of social dysfunctions: a model-based neurofeedback approach for low and high-functioning autism. Front. Neuroeng. 7, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneng.2014.00029 Spunt, R.P., Adolphs, R., 2014. Validating the why/how contrast for functional MRI studies of theory of mind. Neuroimage. 99, 301–311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.023. Spunt, R.P., Lieberman, M.D., 2013. The busy social brain: evidence for automaticity and control in the neural systems supporting social cognition and action understanding. Psychol. Sci. 24, 80–86. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612450884 Spunt, R.P., Satpute, A.B., Lieberman, M.D., 2011. Identifying the what, why, and how of an observed action: an fMRI study of mentalizing and mechanizing during action observation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 63–74. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21446 Tkach, D., Reimer, J., Hatsopoulos, N.G., 2007. Congruent activity during action and action observation in motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 27, 13241–13250. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2895-07.2007 Umiltà, M.A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C., Rizzolatti, G., 2001. I know what you are doing: A neurophysiological study. Neuron 31, 155–165. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00337-3 Wurm, M.F., Ariani, G., Greenlee, M.W., Lingnau, A., 2016. Decoding concrete and abstract action representations during explicit and implicit conceptual processing. Cereb. Cortex 26, 3390–3401. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv169 Wurm, M.F., Lingnau, A., 2015. Decoding actions at different levels of abstraction. J. Neurosci. 35, 7727–7735. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0188-15. Zhu, H., Sun, Y., Zeng, J., Sun, H., 2011. Mirror neural training induced by virtual reality in brain – computer interfaces may provide a promising approach for the autism therapy. Med. Hypotheses 76, 646–647. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2011.01.022

Keywords: Mirror Neurons, Action Understanding, Autism (ASD), Social cognition (SC), Education

Conference: 4th International Conference on Educational Neuroscience, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates, 10 Mar - 11 Mar, 2019.

Presentation Type: Oral Presentation (invited speakers only)

Topic: Educational Neuroscience

Citation: Thompson EL, Bird G and Catmur C (2019). Mirror neurons, action understanding and social interaction: implications for educational neuroscience. Conference Abstract: 4th International Conference on Educational Neuroscience. doi: 10.3389/conf.fnhum.2019.229.00012

Copyright: The abstracts in this collection have not been subject to any Frontiers peer review or checks, and are not endorsed by Frontiers. They are made available through the Frontiers publishing platform as a service to conference organizers and presenters.

The copyright in the individual abstracts is owned by the author of each abstract or his/her employer unless otherwise stated.

Each abstract, as well as the collection of abstracts, are published under a Creative Commons CC-BY 4.0 (attribution) licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) and may thus be reproduced, translated, adapted and be the subject of derivative works provided the authors and Frontiers are attributed.

For Frontiers’ terms and conditions please see https://www.frontiersin.org/legal/terms-and-conditions.

Received: 08 Feb 2019; Published Online: 27 Sep 2019.

* Correspondence: Miss. Emma L Thompson, Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, England, SE5 8AF, United Kingdom, emma.l.thompson@kcl.ac.uk