Skip to main content
Log in

The Effect of Interactional Fairness and Detection on Taxpayers’ Compliance Intentions

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although the role of fairness in tax compliance has been of increasing interest among the academic and professional tax communities, very little is known about the role of interactional fairness. Interactional fairness refers to the quality of the treatment provided to individuals from authority figures, such as tax authority representatives. We conduct an experiment using US taxpayers to examine the role of interactional fairness on tax compliance intentions, and how detection influences this relation. Taxpayers’ detection salience reflects their perceptions that they will be audited by the tax authority. Using insights from conditional cooperation theory, we predict and find that detection moderates the relation between interactional fairness and tax compliance intentions, such that the effect of interactional fairness on tax compliance intentions diminishes with higher detection. We discuss the implications of our results for tax policy.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Using dummy responses, the firm initially tested all web links to ensure they were working properly. We verified that the dummy data were accurately populated into the software. The firm then launched the data collection. The firm provided us with an update at the end of each day of the number of complete responses they had collected. The firm also recalibrated the sample at the end of each day so that our gender and age parameters were as accurate as possible.

  2. We checked our sample against U.S. census data (U.S. Census Bureau, Current Population Survey, 2014 Annual Social and Economic Supplement) that segmented the U.S. population according to income and education level. Our income segments matched those used in this population survey. Our sample was similarly weighted to that of the US population with two exceptions: (1) The income category below $50,000 was underweight (by about 8%) relative to the census data, and (2) our income category above $75,000 was weighted 6% more than the census data. Given that the population of the income category below $50,000 is the one least likely to pay tax, we believe that our sample is relatively representative of the broader population of US taxpayers.

  3. As discussed earlier, we chose a phone call because it is a common and widespread method of contacting the IRS. For example, in the period from January 1 through March 5, 2016, the IRS received 46.1 million telephone calls (https://www.treasury.gov/tigta/auditreports/2016reports/201640034fr.pdf, p. 16).

  4. Consistent with the tax compliance literature (Blanthorne and Kaplan 2008; Bobek and Hatfield 2003; Carnes and Englebrecht 1995; Sanders et al. 2008; Verboon and van Dijke 2007), we use compliance intentions as a proxy for taxpayers’ compliance.

  5. We also tested whether any demographic variables were associated with either independent variable or the interaction between the two independent variables. With the exception of age, the demographic variables were not associated with the independent variables. Age was significantly higher in the lower condition of detection expectations (M = 43.6, SD = 14.6) compared to the higher condition of detection expectations (M = 39.04, SD = 15.0), F(1203) = 4.80, p = .03). We do not include age in the model because it was not associated with the dependent variable, compliance intentions.

  6. Contrast coding represents a more powerful test than ANCOVA and is appropriate when there is a specific interaction based on theoretical predictions (Buckless and Ravenscroft 1990; Cohen et al. 2015; Rosenthal and Rosnow 1985).

References

  • Allingham, M. G., & Sandmo, A. (1972). Income tax evasion: A theoretical analysis. Journal of Public Economics, 1(3–4), 323–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Jackson, B., & McKee, M. (2009). Getting the word out: Enforcement information dissemination and compliance behavior. Journal of Public Economics, 93(3–4), 392–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Kirchler, E., Muehlbacher, S., Gangl, K., Hofmann, E., Kogler, C., et al. (2012). Rethinking the research paradigms for analyzing tax compliance behaviour. CESifo Forum, 13(2), 33–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., McClelland, G. H., & Schulze, W. D. (1992). Why do people pay taxes? Journal of Public Economics, 48(1), 21–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., & Torgler, B. (2011). Do ethics matter? Tax compliance and morality. Journal of Business Ethics, 101(4), 635–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni, J., Erard, B., & Feinstein, J. (1998). Tax compliance. Journal of Economic Literature, 36(2), 818–860.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. F. (1986). Interactional justice: Communication criteria of fairness. In R. J. Lewicki, B. H. Sheppard, & M. H. Bazerman (Eds.), Research on negotiations in organizations (Vol. 1, pp. 43–55). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanthorne, C., & Kaplan, S. (2008). An egocentric model of the relations among the opportunity to underreport, social norms, ethical beliefs, and underreporting behavior. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 33(7–8), 684–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blodgett, J., Hill, D., & Tax, S. (1997). The effects of distributive, procedural, and interactional justice on post compliant behavior. Journal of Retailing, 73(2), 185–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blodgett, J., & Tax, S. (1993). The effects of distributive and interactional justice on complainants’ patronage intentions and negative word-of-mouth intentions. Journal of Consumer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behavior, 6, 100–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobek, D., & Hatfield, R. (2003). An investigation of the theory of planned behavior and the role of moral obligation in taxpayers’ compliance. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 15, 13–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bobek, D., Roberts, R., & Sweeney, J. (2007). The social norms of tax compliance: Evidence from Australia, Singapore, and the United States. Journal of Business Ethics, 74(1), 49–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bordignon, M. (1993). A fairness approach to income tax evasion. Journal of Public Economics, 52(3), 345–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braithwaite, V. (2003). A new approach to tax compliance. In V. Braithwaite (Ed.), Taxing democracy: Understanding tax avoidance and evasion (pp. 1–11). Aldershot, UK: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buckless, F., & Ravenscroft, S. (1990). Contrast coding: A refinement of ANOVA in behavioral analysis. The Accounting Review, 65(4), 933–945.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carnes, G., & Englebrecht, T. (1995). An investigation of the effect of detection risk perceptions, penalty sanctions, and income visibility on tax compliance. Journal of the American Taxation Association, 17(1), 26–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, T. D., & Reimers, J. L. (2005). Unethical and fraudulent financial reporting: Applying the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 60(2), 115–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chebat, J., & Slusarczyk, W. (2005). How emotions mediation the effect of perceived injustice on loyalty in service recovery situations: An empirical analysis. Journal of Business Research, 58(5), 664–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, C., Harcourt, M., & Flynn, M. (2013). Clinical governance, performance appraisal and interactional and procedural fairness at a New Zealand public hospital. Journal of Business Ethics, 117(4), 667–678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Manzon, G., & Zamora, V. (2015). Contextual and individual dimensions of taxpayer decision making. Journal of Business Ethics, 126(4), 631–647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collie, T., Bradley, G., & Sparks, B. (2002). Fair process revisited: Differential effects of interactional and procedural justice in the presence of social comparison information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(6), 545–555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colquitt, J., Conlon, D., Wesson, M., Porter, C., & Ng, K. (2001). Justice at the millennium: A meta-analytic review of 25 years of organizational justice research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(3), 425–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cropanzano, R., Prehar, C., & Chen, P. (2002). Using social exchange theory to distinguish procedural from interactional justice. Group and Organizational Management, 27(3), 324–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feld, L. P., & Frey, B. S. (2007). Tax compliance as the result of a psychological contract: The role of incentives and responsive regulation. Law and Policy, 29(1), 102–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, J. L., Ellen, P. S., & Bearden, W. O. (2014). Procedural and distributive fairness: Determinants of overall price fairness. Journal of Business Ethics, 121(2), 217–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B., & Feld, L. (2002). Deterrence and morale in taxation: An empirical analysis. CESifo Working Paper no. 760, August.

  • Frey, B., & Torgler, B. (2007). Tax morale and conditional cooperation. Journal of Comparative Economics, 35(1), 136–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gangl, K., Muehlbacher, S., de Groot, M., Goslinga, S., Hofmann, E., Kogler, C., et al. (2013). “How can I help you?” Perceived service orientation of tax authorities and tax compliance. Public Finance Analysis, 69(4), 487–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gangl, K., Torgler, B., Kirchler, E., & Hofmann, E. (2014). Effects of supervision on tax compliance: Evidence from a field experiment in Austria. Economics Letters, 123, 378–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gemmell, N., & Ratto, M. (2012). Behavioral responses to taxpayer audits: Evidence from random taxpayer inquiries. National Tax Journal, 65(1), 33–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gobena, L., & Van Dijke, M. (2016). Power, justice, and trust: A moderated mediation analysis of tax compliance among Ethiopian business owners. Journal of Economic Psychology, 52(1), 24–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartner, M., Rechberger, S., Kirchler, E., & Schabmann, A. (2008). Procedural fairness and tax compliance. Economic Analysis & Policy, 38(1), 137–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, E., Hoelzl, E., & Kirchler, E. (2008). Voluntary tax compliance: Knowledge and evaluation of taxation, norms, fairness, and motivation to cooperate. Journal of Psychology, 216(4), 209–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, B., & Milliron, V. (1986). Taxpayers’ compliance research: Findings, problems, and prospects. Journal of Accounting Literature, 5, 125–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, M., & Meckling, W. (1976). Theory of the firm: Managerial behavior, agency costs and ownership structure. Journal of Financial Economics, 3(4), 305–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, C., Evans, J., & Moser, D. (2005). Economic and equity effects on tax reporting decisions. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 30(7/8), 609–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E., Hoelzl, E., & Wahl, I. (2008). Enforced versus voluntary compliance: The “slippery slope” framework. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29(2), 210–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E., Muehlbacher, S., Kastlunger, B., & Wahl, I. (2010). Why pay taxes? A review of tax compliance decisions. In J. Alm, J. Martinez-Vazquez, & B. Torgler (Eds.), Developing alternative frameworks for explaining tax compliance (pp. 15–31). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kogler, C., Mittone, L., & Kirchler, E. (2016). Delayed feedback on tax audits affects compliance and fairness perceptions. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 124, 81–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maciejovsky, B., Schwarzenberger, H., & Kirchler, E. (2012). Rationality versus emotions: The case of tax ethics and compliance. Journal of Business Ethics, 109(3), 339–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Margasak, L. (2011). IRS chief apologizes for employees’ rudeness. Available at: http://cnsnews.com/news/article/irs-chief-apologizes-employees-rudeness.

  • Masterson, S. S., Lewis-McClear, K., Goldman, B. M., & Taylor, M. S. (2000). Integrating justice and social exchange. The differing effects of fair procedures and treatment on work relationships. Academy of Management Journal, 43(4), 738–748.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molero, J., & Pujol, F. (2012). Walking inside the potential tax evader’s mind: Tax morale does matter. Journal of Business Ethics, 105(2), 151–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moser, D., Evans, J., & Kim, C. (1995). The effects of horizontal and exchange inequity on tax reporting decisions. The Accounting Review, 70(4), 619–634.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K. (2004). The role of trust in nurturing compliance: A study of accused tax avoiders. Law and Human Behavior, 28(2), 187–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K. (2005). Regulating more effectively: The relationship between procedural justice, legitimacy and tax non-compliance. Journal of Law and Society, 32(4), 562–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K. (2009). Procedural justice and affect intensity: Understanding reactions to regulatory authorities. Social Justice Research, 22(1), 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, R. (2011). The cost of tax abuse: A briefing paper on the cost of tax evasion worldwide. Tax Justice Network publication, November 2011.

  • National Taxpayer Advocate (2014). 2014 Annual report to congress. Online: http://taxpayeradvocate.irs.gov/Media/Default/Documents/2014-Annual-Report-to-Congress-Executive-Summary.pdf.

  • OECD (2010). Understanding and influencing taxpayers’ compliance behavior. Centre for Tax Policy and Administration, Paris. Available at http://www.oecd.org/tax/administration/46274793.pdf.

  • Primeaux, P. S. M., Karri, R., & Caldwell, C. (2003). Cultural insights to justice: A theoretical perspective through a subjective lens. Journal of Business Ethics, 46(2), 187–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Randall, D. M. (1989). Taking stock: Can the theory of reasoned action explain unethical conduct? Journal of Business Ethics, 8(11), 873–882.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, J. (2011). Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educational Research Review, 6, 135–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. (1983). Cognitive and physiological processes in fear and appeals in attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In B. L. Cacioppo & L. L. Petty (Eds.), Social psychophysiology: a source book (pp. 153–176). London: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. (1985). Contrast analysis: Focused comparisons in the analysis of variance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, D., Reckers, P., & Iyer, G. (2008). Influence of accountability and penalty awareness on tax compliance. Journal of the American Taxation Association, 30(1), 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention-behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. European Review of Social Psychology, 12(1), 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, D. (2011). Letter to the ways and means committee of congress. Available at http://democrats.waysandmeans.house.gov/media/pdf/112/Rep_Lewis_IRS_Letter.pdf.

  • Steiger, J. (2004). Beyond the F test: Effect size confidence intervals and tests of close fit in the analysis of variance and contrast analysis. Psychological Methods, 9(2), 164–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgler, B. (2002). Speaking to theorists and searching for facts: Tax morale and tax compliance in experiments. Journal of Economic Surveys, 16(5), 657–683.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijke, M., & Verboon, P. (2010). Trust in authorities as a boundary condition to procedural fairness effects on tax compliance. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(1), 80–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verboon, P., & Van Dijke, M. (2007). A self-interest analysis of justice and taxpayers’ compliance: How distributive justice moderates the effect of outcome favorability. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28(6), 704–727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verboon, P., & Van Dijke, M. (2011). When do severe sanctions enhance compliance? The role of procedural fairness. Journal of Economic Psychology, 32(1), 120–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verboon, P., & Van Dijke, M. (2012). The effect of perceived deterrence on compliance with authorities: The moderating influence of procedural justice. International Journal of Criminology and Sociology, 1, 151–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, K. (2012). Understanding taxpayer behavior—new opportunities for tax administration. The Economic and Social Review, 43(3), 451–475.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M. (2002). The impact of outcome orientation and justice concerns on taxpayers’ compliance: The role of taxpayers’ identity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(4), 629–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M. (2006). A letter from the Tax Office: Compliance effects of informational and interpersonal justice. Social Justice Research, 19(3), 345–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wuensch, K. (2016). Standardized effect size estimation: Why and how? Online: http://core.ecu.edu/psyc/wuenschk/StatHelp/Effect%20Size%20Estimation.pdf.

  • Zapata-Phelan, C., Colquitt, J., Scott, B., & Livingston, B. (2009). Procedural justice, interactional justice, and task performance: The mediating role of intrinsic motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108(1), 93–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zyglidopoulos, S., Fleming, P., & Rothenberg, S. (2009). Rationalization, overcompensation and the escalation of corruption in organizations. Journal of Business Ethics, 84(1), 65–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Funding was provided by Schulich CPA Research Alliance, Canadian Accounting Association.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Linda Thorne.

Appendices

Appendix: Experimental Instrument

Part 1: Basic Tax Scenario, Common to all Experimental Materials

Below is a story about a barber named Jason and his experiences with the IRS. Imagine that you are Jason. Please read it carefully, as you will be asked some follow-up questions.

Jason is a barber, and while he used to work in construction, he decided to open his own barber shop last year. He rented a small store on the main street of his home town and bought an antique barber chair on eBay for $2200. He did not accept credit cards or checks, so his customers paid him in cash only. He kept records of customers’ appointments and haircuts by writing on a calendar with a pencil.

Jason was preparing his own tax return shortly before the April 15th deadline. He understood that the total amount of cash received from customers was part of his business income. But, he had a question about how to treat the barber chair for tax purposes. He wasn’t sure whether he should deduct the entire cost, or only a portion.

Specific Scenario Information

Higher interactional fairness, Higher Detection

Jason had several barber friends who were audited last year by the IRS. They told him the IRS was devoting more time and effort to auditing cash-based businesses.

Jason decided to call the IRS to ask about the tax rules for deducting the barber chair. The IRS spokesperson was very polite and respectful, answered his question simply and thoroughly, and asked whether there was anything else she could help him with.

Higher Interactional Fairness, Lower Detection

Jason decided to call the IRS to ask about the tax rules for deducting the barber chair. The IRS spokesperson was very polite and respectful, answered his question simply and thoroughly, and asked whether there was anything else she could help him with.

Lower Interactional Fairness, Higher Detection

Jason had several barber friends who were audited last year by the IRS. They told him the IRS was devoting more time and effort to auditing cash-based businesses.

Jason decided to call the IRS to ask about the tax rules for deducting the barber chair. The IRS spokesperson was very rude and disrespectful, said that it was not their responsibility to answer his question, and immediately disconnected his call.

Lower Interactional Fairness, Lower Detection

Jason decided to call the IRS to ask about the tax rules for deducting the barber chair. The IRS spokesperson was very rude and disrespectful, said that it was not their responsibility to answer his question, and immediately disconnected his call.

Part 2: Questions

Dependent Variable

Please read the following statements and indicate your level of agreement by clicking on the appropriate response, where 1 = strongly agree, and 7 = strongly disagree.

  1. 1.

    Jason will not declare all the cash to the IRS.

  2. 2.

    Jason would be tempted to not report all of his cash receipts on his tax return.

  3. 3.

    Jason is unlikely to report all his cash earnings to the IRS.

  4. 4.

    Under the circumstances, Jason might not report all of his cash earnings on his tax return.

Manipulation checks

Interactional Fairness

  1. 1.

    Jason was treated well when he phoned the IRS (1 = strongly agree; 7 = strongly disagree).

Detection

  1. 1.

    Jason expects to be audited by the IRS (1 = strongly agree; 7 = strongly disagree).

Demographic Questions

Please answer the following demographic questions.

  1. 1.

    Your gender: male female

  2. 2.

    Have you ever had a problem with the IRS? yes no

  3. 3.

    Your present age in years: __________

  4. 4.

    The number of years of your full-time work experience: __________.

  5. 5.

    Who usually prepares your tax return?

    • I do

    • My spouse/partner

    • Paid preparer

    • Other

  6. 6.

    Please indicate your highest level of education completed:

    • High School

    • Community College diploma

    • Undergraduate degree

    • Graduate degree

    • Other

  7. 7.

    How would you categorize your political beliefs?

    • Very conservative

    • Moderately conservative

    • Slightly conservative

    • Middle of political spectrum

    • Slightly liberal

    • Moderately liberal

    • Very liberal

  8. 8.

    Please indicate your approximate annual income:

    • Less than $25,000

    • Between $25,000 and $50,000

    • Between $50,001 and $75,000

    • Between $75,001 and $100,000

    • ≥$100,000

    • Prefer not to answer

________________________________________

Thank you for your participation in this study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Farrar, J., Kaplan, S.E. & Thorne, L. The Effect of Interactional Fairness and Detection on Taxpayers’ Compliance Intentions. J Bus Ethics 154, 167–180 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-017-3458-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-017-3458-x

Keywords

Navigation