Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 03 November 2021
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognition and Music Performance View all 11 articles

Effect of Bow Camber and Mass Distribution on Violinists' Preferences and Performance

  • 1Univ Lyon, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Univ Gustave Eiffel, LBMC UMR_T9406, Lyon, France
  • 2Laboratoire de Simulation et de Modélisation du Mouvement, École de Kinésiologie et des Sciences de l'Activité Physique, Faculté de Médecine, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
  • 3Wilder & Davis, Montréal, QC, Canada
  • 4Research Center, Sainte-Justine Hospital, Montréal, QC, Canada

Little is known about how bow mechanical characteristics objectively and quantitatively influence violinists' preferences and performance. Hypothesizing that the bow shape (i.e., camber) and mass distribution modifications would alter both violinists' appreciations of a bow and objective assessments of their performance, we recruited 10 professional violinists to play their own violin using 18 versions of a single bow, modified by combining three cambers and six mass distributions, in random order. A musical phrase, composed for this study, was played legato and spiccato at three octaves and two tempi. Each violinist scored all 18 bows. Then, experts assessed the recorded performances according to criteria inspired by basic musical analysis. Finally, 12 audio-descriptors were calculated on the same note from each trial, to objectivise potential acoustic differences. Statistical analysis (ANOVA) reveals that bow camber impacted the violinists' appreciations (p < 0.05), and that heavier bow tips gave lower scores for spiccato playing (p < 0.05). The expert evaluations reveal that playing with a lighter bow (tip or frog), or with a bow whose camber's maximum curvature is close to the frog, had a positive impact on some violinists' performance (NS to p < 0.001). The “camber-participant” interaction had significant effects on the violinists' appreciations (p < 0.01 to p < 0.001), on the expert's evaluation and on almost all the audio-descriptors (NS to p < 0.001). While trends were identified, multiple camber-participant interactions suggest that bow makers should provide a variety of cambers to satisfy different violinists.

Introduction

Violin bows' shape and materials have changed little since the end of the eighteenth century. However, trade restrictions have made the wood used in bow sticks, Pernambuco (Paubrasilia echinata, also termed brazilwood and derived mainly from Brazilian forests), less available and more expensive (Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species of Wild Fauna Flora, 2017). This increasing rarity raises the question of what makes a “good” pernambuco bow, if satisfactory alternatives are to be developed: both the point of view of violinists, through the assessment of their preferences, and of listeners, through the assessment of violinists' musical performance should be considered.

The scientific interest for string vibrations is ancient, as links between a monochord vibration and sound pitches are well known since the Antiquity (Abromont and de Montalembert, 2001), and is still relevant, as can be seen for instance with the studies of the non-steady parts of bowing, i.e., the attack and release of the sound also known as transients (Badeau, 2005; Castellengo et al., 2015). In further work, the relationships between the string vibration and the bow have been studied (Schoonderwaldt et al., 2007; Schoonderwaldt and Altenmüller, 2009). With the development of new technological possibilities appeared a growing interest for the upper limb biomechanics during the bowing (Schoonderwaldt and Altenmüller, 2011; Kelleher et al., 2013), as well as the description of the motion of the bow itself, which remains a technological challenge (Provenzale et al., 2021).

Most studies investigating violinists' preferences have focused on the violin itself (Saitis et al., 2012, 2015; Fritz and Dubois, 2015). Instrumentalists' rankings of violins have been shown to be reproducible (Saitis et al., 2012), and the more precise the musical task, the more the instrumentalists agree on their assessments of a violin. Violinists also discriminate better between violins, in terms of richness of sound, when playing rather than listening to them (Saitis et al., 2015). Such studies set the example in experimental design to assess instrumentalists' preferences. However, no study has addressed violin bow preferences. A single study (Caussé et al., 2001) explored potential connections between verbal descriptors used by instrumentalists and the static (measured) or dynamic (estimated by finite element modeling) mechanical properties of the bow. Their preliminary findings showed no evidence of a link. Other studies on the violin bow addressed its modal and acoustical responses (Ravina et al., 2008) and its spectral content (Schoonderwaldt and Altenmüller, 2009), but without considering how these might relate to violinists' preferences (Woodhouse, 2014). There is clearly a gap in our knowledge concerning the relationship between bow mechanical characteristics and preferences.

Determining whether a musical performance is satisfactory inevitably involves human perception. Although human evaluations are subjective by nature—i.e. dependent on personal musical culture, place and time—their reproducibility has been documented: music student grades have been reported to show an intra-rater correlation coefficient of 0.87–0.99 (Franzén, 1969; Beazley, 1981) and an inter-rater correlation of 0.95–0.95 (Beazley, 1981). Evaluations by experts were found to be most reproducible if the judges were experienced and judged from recordings rather than from real-time performances (Salvador, 2010). Furthermore, musicians were found to be more accurate than non-musicians in their perceptions (McAdams et al., 1999; Fritz et al., 2008).

A complementary approach to performance rating involves calculating audio-descriptors, which are numerical indicators that account for particular dimensions of a sound. The calculation is based either on sound energy envelopes or on frequency spectrums, using either the Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) or the harmonic model (Table 1). Some parameters are temporal, describing the variation of one parameter across time, while others are global, describing an entire sound (Peeters et al., 2011). Audio-descriptors are used in several ways in the literature. The approach taken by music information research, for example to automatically classify instruments, distinguishes between different sounds by including as many descriptors as possible, even those not relevant in terms of human perception. This approach has been found effective in this context, correctly classifying more than 90% of the instrumental sounds tested (Siedenburg et al., 2016). Another approach, used in musical psychology, assesses the dissimilarity between sounds using a small number of audio-descriptors whose relevance in terms of human perception has been shown (Table 1; Peeters et al., 2011). For the violin, research has focused on the correlation of audio-descriptors with verbal descriptors (Fritz et al., 2012; Saitis et al., 2012). As for bow assessment based on audio-descriptors, the impact of different bowing parameters, as speed and pressure, has been assessed, but without examining bow mechanical characteristics themselves. Schoonderwaldt and Altenmüller (2009) showed that the spectral centroid (Table 1) increases mainly with the pressure exerted by the bow, especially when played close to the bridge. Moreover, the fundamental frequency of the note decreases when bow pressure is high, especially when played far from the bridge. Conversely, fundamental frequency increases when a string is played rapidly near the bridge (Schoonderwaldt and Altenmüller, 2009). To date, audio-descriptors have not been used to assess the sound modifications generated by different bows.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Main audio-descriptors as calculated in Peeters et al. (2011), variables transformed in our study, and camber-participant interaction effect on audio-descriptors.

The main objective of this study was to investigate the influence of bow mechanical characteristics, specifically its camber and mass distribution, on instrumentalists' preferences and on their musical performance. In keeping with the above literature, we chose to evaluate musical performance on the basis of both expert assessment and audio-descriptors.

Method

Data Collection

Ten professional musicians (9 women and 1 man, aged 27.7 ± 7.6 years), who had been playing the violin for 21.0 ± 8.5 years were recruited to participate in this study. The protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Montreal (17-018-CERES-D). The violinists played their own personal violin, while a single bow (mass: 62 g; natural camber, i.e., bow shape: camber 1) was used, modified into 18 camber/mass distribution configurations.

A bow maker (coauthor EG) modified the bow behind a curtain, changing its camber and adding mass randomly in two steps [see the Supplementary Material and the video (S2M Lab, 2017) minute 0:42]. First, one of the three cambers was randomly selected. After being heated (see the Supplementary Material), the bow was bent to match one of three defined patterns. Then masses were randomly added at the tip (0, 1 or 2 g, i.e., 1.6 or 3.2% of the bow weight) and/or at the frog (0 or 2 g), using sticky gums. All camber and mass combinations were tested, i.e., 18 bow conditions for each violinist, except for one of the cambers (camber 1) for one of the violinists, due to lack of time. For 6 out of the 10 participants, one of the 18 conditions, randomly chosen for each camber, was repeated in immediate succession. The bow maker pretended to modify the bow as usual, ensuring that the violinist did not know the two conditions were identical.

The piece of music (Figure 1), especially composed to explore different facets of the bowing technique while keeping a certain degree of ecological validity, consisted of two sections, each involving a different type of articulation: first legato, then spiccato. Both were played over three octaves. The entire piece of music was played at two tempi, 60 beats per minute or 120 (an electronic metronome was used), in random order, for a total of 12 musical sections. The sound was recorded with a Zoom Q3HD recorder (condenser microphone, XY, 120° angle), set at a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz. The recorder was placed on the lectern, facing the violinist. The lectern position relative to the performer was kept relatively constant during recordings by floor markings.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The piece of music, consisting of two sections (spiccato, then legato), both played at three octaves. The C# analyzed using audio-descriptors is highlighted in yellow.

Evaluation

After each trial, the participants gave their overall appreciation of the bow, rating it from 0 to 10, as well as an appreciation specifically for playing legato and spiccato. No instructions were given regarding rating criteria.

From the recordings, two-step randomized by violinist and then by audio file, a first expert evaluated musical performance (coauthor TA, a composer). She searched for four types of defects, using a method inspired by musical analysis techniques: timbre errors (squeaking, wrong string played, unexpected bounced bowstroke), lack of pitch accuracy, lack of rhythm precision and articulation errors. She defined the defects in writing and via sound samples. Penalty scores (timbre score, pitch score, rhythm score and articulation score) were calculated for each bow configuration for each participant by incrementing them (+1) whenever a musical section contained at least one of the targeted defects. An overall penalty score was also calculated for the whole piece of music and for all the sections related to each musical parameter (types of articulation, octave and tempo). Penalty scores were then fitted to a scale ranging from 0 (flawless performance) to 10 (all musical sections were flawed). Then, 30 audio files (three per participant) were randomly selected. They were assessed again 1 month later by the same expert and a second expert (coauthor MB, a violinist and the composer of this piece of music) to assess grading repeatability, using coefficients of variation.

After averaging the two voices of the stereo recording, audio-descriptors that seemed the most relevant in terms of human perception were calculated using Matlab's Timbre Toolbox (Peeters et al., 2011). To avoid redundancy, we choose the descriptors which were the less linked to each other, i.e., that had a median rank correlation lower than 0.8 in Peeters et al. (2011) (see Table 1). As recommended by Peeters et al. (2011), the temporal audio-descriptors were characterized by their median value. Audio-descriptors were calculated on the C#3 (expected frequency of 277 Hz) spiccato tempo 60 (see Figure 1). This note was manually extracted using Audacity (Audacity Team, 2018).

Statistics

To assess the repeatability of violinists' preferences, differences between their assessments of the two identical bow trials were calculated. Then, after checking the normality of these differences, the coefficient of variation between the first and second trial was calculated. The coefficient of determination (R2) of an analyse of variance (ANOVA) was used to assess the percentage of variance in preferences due to the difference between two repeated tests.

The influence of bow characteristics was assessed using the bow cambers, the mass added at the tip and the mass added at the frog as explanatory variables. Dependent variables were the violinists' preferences, the expert's penalty score and the audio-descriptors. The distribution of each variable was checked graphically to ensure its normality and perform any necessary transformations (Table 1). Homoscedasticity among subgroups was tested using a Levene test. After verification of these conditions of applicability, one-factor ANOVAs were performed. Due to high inter-participant variability, the factors were all re-tested with a two-factor ANOVA including a participant effect. More complex models were developed using this approach, i.e., by progressively adding the most significant variables (bottom-up approach).

Pearson's linear correlation coefficients were calculated among the violinists' different appreciations (overall, legato and spiccato playing) and among penalty scores and audio-descriptors, after normalizing each parameter by violinist (same mean and standard deviation for each) and graphically verifying the shape of correlations on scatter plots.

Results

Violinists' Preferences

The mass at the tip had a significant effect on appreciations of spiccato playing, and the camber had a significant effect on all appreciations. Since there was a major participant effect (R2 = 0.31–0.37, p < 0.001, vs. R2 = 0.2 at most, not significant, for the explanatory variables), it was taken into account simultaneously through two-factor ANOVA. Specifically, added mass at the tip of the bow reduced ratings for the spiccato on average (5.8, 6.5 and 6.6/10, for 2, 1 and 0 g addition, respectively, p < 0.05). Camber 1 was always the lowest rated (overall ratings of 6.2, 6.9 and 6.7/10, for cambers 1, 2 and 3, respectively), except by one violinist. However, there were differences in effect size and in preferences between cambers 2 and 3, generally explained more by a camber-participant interaction (p < 0.01 to p < 0.001) than by the camber itself (p < 0.05) (Figure 2). In the three-factor model, there was no interaction between violinists, added mass and camber.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Violinists' bow appreciations—average overall appreciation according to camber (1, 2 or 3) and violinist. Lines connect the values for each individual violinist.

In terms of repeatability of the violinists' appreciations, the mean of the absolute value of the difference between the two identical bow trials was 1.4/10 for the overall and legato assessments and 1.6/10 for the spiccato assessment. Half the participants raised their rating on average in the second trial, while the other half lowered their rating (Figure 3). The coefficient of variation between the values given for the first and second trials was 0.26 for overall assessment, 0.33 for spiccato, 0.27 for legato. The percentage of variance explained by difference between the two identical bow trials was 23% for the overall assessment, 30% for the legato assessment and 17% for the spiccato assessment.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Differences in violinists' general appreciations for the two tests performed under identical bow conditions (trial 1–trial 2) for six participants (3, 4 and 7–10).

After normalization by violinist, links between overall ranking and appreciations for spiccato and legato playing were respectively r = 0.84 and 0.87. Correlation between the latter two appreciations was r = 0.62.

Experts' Penalty Ratings

The rating agreement between evaluations 1 month apart was very good (81.4%), indicating low intra-expert variability. The consistency of ratings between experts was good (70.1%), indicating reasonable inter-expert variability. Once again, all penalty ratings showed a major participant effect (R2 = 0.34–0.78, p < 0.001, vs. at most R2 = 0.2, NS, for the explanatory variables). After taking into account the participant effect, the main results were that a mass of 0 g at the tip had a positive effect on timbre ratings (5.6 vs. 6.3 and 6.0/10 for a 1 and 2 g mass, respectively, p < 0.05), and that a mass of 0 g at the frog had a positive effect on playing at high pitches (two last lines of the piece of music, Figure 1), depending on the violinist (mean: 4.1 vs. 4.2/10, p < 0.05).

Camber had an influence at low pitches and in fast passages (p < 0.05), in both of which cases camber 3 obtained the lowest penalty ratings (mean: 3.2 vs. 3.5 and 3.6/10 for camber 3, 1 and 2, respectively at low pitch and 3.4 vs. 3.8 and 3.7/10, respectively in fast passages). There were camber-participant interactions for rhythmic precision, for respect of articulation, at middle-range pitches (p < 0.05) and for overall ratings (p < 0.001).

After normalization by violinist, no links between timbre, pitch, rhythm and articulation penalty scores were found, nor between mutually exclusive musical parameter penalty scores (for example, between penalty scores for each of the three octaves).

Audio-Descriptors

For the audio-descriptors that did not fit a normal distribution, variables were transformed to ensure normality (Table 1). For each audio-descriptor, there was a major participant effect (R2 = 0.18–0.78, p < 0.001, vs. at most a non-significant R2 = 0.2 for the explanatory variables). When the participant effect was taken into account, camber-participant interaction had a statistically significant effect on almost all audio-descriptors, whereas mass additions had no effect (Table 1).

After normalization by violinist, multiple links were found between audio-descriptors, computed two by two.

Discussion

The main objective of this study was to investigate the influence of bow mechanical characteristics on both instrumentalists' preferences and their musical performance. As hypothesized, bow modifications altered the violinists' appreciations, especially camber changes. Concerning musical performance, significant “camber-participant” interactions were found for each audio-descriptor. The experts' evaluations revealed that a camber with maximum curvature closest to the frog (Camber 3) had positive effects on playing, while added mass at the tip or frog had negative effects.

Acoustic response can differ across bows (Ravina et al., 2008). Specifically, we found that camber has a major impact, much more pronounced than bow mass distribution, on many sound parameters and on violinists' appreciations. The camber with maximum curvature closest to the tip (Camber 1) scored lowest, except with one violinist. Differences in violinists' preferences regarding the two other cambers, i.e., camber-participant interaction, could be explained by differences in the types of bow they habitually use, the ones with cambers most similar to theirs being possibly preferred. Documenting the characteristics of the bows generally used by the instrumentalists involved in experimental studies might therefore be worthwhile. On the other hand, these differing preferences could also reflect musicians' personal aesthetic aims. Actually, overall quality rating and preference rating are known to be poorly correlated (Wollman et al., 2014), likely suggesting different expectations regarding musical performance. In the end, since our violinists' appreciations and experts' evaluations often depended on the violinist (camber-participant interaction), a good general principle for bow makers would be to provide bows with different cambers (affecting overall bow stick design) to satisfy diverse violinists.

Throughout the history of bow making, a balance has been sought between bow length, mass and “nervosity,” i.e., its ability to respond quickly to the violinist gesture (Bachmann et al., 2001), which seems likely to refer to its dynamic properties. In the present study, the mass added at the tip and frog affected both the violinists' appreciations of the bow and their performance. At the tip, added mass had a negative impact both on instrumentalists' ratings for spiccato playing and on the frequency of timbre flaws as evaluated by the expert. A deleterious effect on the spiccato was expected: the bow “bounces” less well with more weight at the tip, since the center of mass may have shifted and/or the bow may be more difficult to control. Adding 2 g to the frog also had a negative impact, i.e., higher expert penalty ratings, with some players producing more defects in the treble. Bow mass and nervosity are linked to the nature of the materials used and to the geometry of the bow (Bachmann et al., 2001). Our data, together with the new prospect of using carbon fibers to make lighter bows, encourage reconsideration of the total weight of bows, as well as their mass balance and their dynamic behavior. This field of research is complex, as the bow material and geometry also impact the risk for bow fatigue failure (Bachmann et al., 2001). The expectations of different musicians have to be accounted, as highlighted above, and the musical patterns used for assessing the bow quality can also be adjusted to specific musical repertoires (Koechlin, 1956; Penesco, 1986).

While the present protocol was designed in a comprehensive way, taking into account instrumentalists' preferences, experts' evaluations and effects on sound, some limitations remain. In terms of expert evaluation, the reproducibility, even though it is reasonable (consistency of 81.4 and 70.1% for intra- and inter-expert assessments), could be improved. As for audio-descriptors, calculations could be extended to include more musical notes (Tomezzoli et al., 2019). Furthermore, given the multiple links found between the audio-descriptors used here, their number could be reduced. Finally, the highly significant inter-participant effect found here reflects not only the effect of inter-individual variations between participants but also differences in the violin and sound recording. Studies with larger samples of instrumentalists will need to be performed before these findings can be generalized to the entire population of violinists.

Author's Note

The results reported in the present article were partly presented at the 44th congress of the French Biomechanics Society (see Tomezzoli et al., 2019). With respect to the conference paper, we have detailed the Methods section to allow replication studies and have included experts' evaluations of violinists' performance in the dataset.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The study involving human participants was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Montreal (17-018-CERES-D). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

AT, BM, MB, and SD contributed to conception and design of the study. BM, EG, MB, and SD contributed to the acquisition and analysis or interpretation of data for the work. AT performed the statistical analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

EG was employed by Wilder & Davis.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

This study was carried out within the framework of the Associated International Laboratory EVASYM.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.769831/full#supplementary-material

References

Abromont, C., and de Montalembert, E. (2001). Guide de la théorie de la musique. Paris: Fayard H. Lemoine.

Audacity Team (2018). Audacity (2.2.2) (Computer Software). Boston: Free Software Foundation. Availability online at: http://audacity.sourcefOrge.net/

Bachmann, W., Seletsky, R., Boyden, D., Liivoja-Lorius, J., Walls, P., and Cooke, P. (2001). “Bow,” in Grove Music Online, eds. S. Sadie and G. Grove (Oxford: Oxford University Press). doi: 10.1093/gmo/9781561592630.article.03753

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badeau, R. (2005). Méthodes à haute résolution pour l'estimation et le suivi de sinusoïdes modulées. Application aux signaux de musique. Dissertation Thesis. Paris: Télécom ParisTech. Available online at: https://pastel.archives-ouvertes.fr/tel-00009321 (accessed October 6, 2021).

Beazley, H. V. (1981). Development and validation of a music education competency test. J. Res. Music Educ. 29, 5–10. doi: 10.2307/3344674

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellengo, M., Liénard, J.-S., and Bloch, G. (2015). Écoute musicale et acoustique: avec 420 sons et leurs sonagrammes décryptés. Paris: Eyrolles.

Caussé, R., Maigret, J. P., Dichtel, C., and Bensoam, J. (2001). “Study of violin bow quality,” in International Symposium on Musical Acoustics.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species of Wild Fauna and Flora (2017). Available online at: https://cites.org/eng/disc/text.php#II (accessed September 23, 2018).

Franzén, B. (1969). Entrance test validity. J. Res. Music Educ. 17, 62–68. doi: 10.2307/3344187

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritz, C., Blackwell, A. F., Cross, I., Woodhouse, J., and Moore, B. C. J. (2012). Exploring violin sound quality: investigating English timbre descriptors and correlating resynthesized acoustical modifications with perceptual properties. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 131, 783–794. doi: 10.1121/1.3651790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritz, C., Cross, I., Moore, B., and Woodhouse, J. (2008). Perceptual thresholds for detecting modifications applied to the acoustical properties of a violin. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 122, 3640–3650. doi: 10.1121/1.2799475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritz, C., and Dubois, D. (2015). Perceptual evaluation of musical instruments: state of the art and methodology. Acta Acust. United Acust. 101, 369–381. doi: 10.3813/AAA.918833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelleher, L. K., Campbell, K. R., and Dickey, J. P. (2013). Biomechanical research on bowed string musicians: a scoping study. Med. Probl. Perform Art 28, 212–218.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Koechlin, C. (1956). Traité de l'orchestration. Paris: M. Eschig.

McAdams, S., Beauchamp, J. W., and Meneguzzi, S. (1999). Discrimination of musical instrument sounds resynthesized with simplified spectrotemporal parameters. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 105(2 Pt 1), 882–897. doi: 10.1121/1.426277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peeters, G., Giordano, B. L., Susini, P., Misdariis, N., and McAdams, S. (2011). The Timbre Toolbox: extracting audio descriptors from musical signals. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 130, 2902–2916. doi: 10.1121/1.3642604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penesco, A. (1986). Les Instruments du quatuor: technique et interprétation. Paris: la Flûte de Pan.

Provenzale, C., Di Stefano, N., Noccaro, A., and Taffoni, F. (2021). Assessing the bowing technique in violin beginners using MIMU and optical proximity sensors: a feasibility study. Sensors 21:5817. doi: 10.3390/s21175817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravina, E., Silvestri, P., and Airenti, A. (2008). Experimental modal analysis of bow. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 123:3659

S2M Lab (2017). L'Archet et son effet sur la biomécanique. Available online at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=0-NHGJz6vTw (accessed October 08, 2021).

Saitis, C., Giordano, B., Fritz, C., and Scavone, G. (2012). Perceptual evaluation of violins: a quantitative analysis of preference judgments by experienced players. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 132, 4002–4012. doi: 10.1121/1.4765081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saitis, C., Scavone, G., Fritz, C., and Giordano, B. (2015). Effect of task constraints on the perceptual evaluation of violins. Acta Acust. Unit. Acust. 101, 382–393. doi: 10.3813/AAA.918834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salvador, K. (2010). How can elementary teachers measure singing voice achievement? a critical review of assessments, 1994–2009. Appl. Res. Music Educ. 29, 40–47. doi: 10.1177/8755123310378454

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoonderwaldt, E., and Altenmüller, E. (2009). The violinist's sound palette: spectral centroid, pitch flattening and anomalous low frequencies. Acta Acust. United Acust. 95, 901–914. doi: 10.3813/AAA.918221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoonderwaldt, E., and Altenmüller, E. (2011). “Mastering the violin: motor learning in complex bowing skills,” in Proceedings of the International Symposium on Performance Science, 649–654.

Schoonderwaldt, E., Guettler, K., and Askenfelt, A. (2007). “Schelleng in retrospect—a systematic study of bow force limits for bowed violin strings,” in International Symposium on Musical Acoustics, Barcelona, Spain

Siedenburg, K., Fujinaga, I., and McAdams, S. (2016). A comparison of approaches to timbre descriptors in music information retrieval and music psychology. J. New Music Res. 45, 27–41. doi: 10.1080/09298215.2015.1132737

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomezzoli, A., Michaud, B., Begon, M., and Duprey, S. (2019). Bow camber and mass distribution: violinist's preferences and sound descriptors. Comput. Methods Biomech. Biomed. Eng. 22, S240–S242. doi: 10.1080/10255842.2020.1714255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wollman, I., Fritz, C., Poitevineau, J., and McAdams, S. (2014). Investigating the role of auditory and tactile modalities in violin quality evaluation. PloS ONE 9:e112552. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodhouse, J. (2014). The acoustics of the violin: a review. Rep. Prog. Phys. 77:115901. doi: 10.1088/0034-4885/77/11/115901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: violin, bow, preference, musical analysis, acoustic

Citation: Tomezzoli A, Michaud B, Gagné E, Begon M and Duprey S (2021) Effect of Bow Camber and Mass Distribution on Violinists' Preferences and Performance. Front. Psychol. 12:769831. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.769831

Received: 02 September 2021; Accepted: 12 October 2021;
Published: 03 November 2021.

Edited by:

Gary Edward McPherson, The University of Melbourne, Australia

Reviewed by:

Nicola Di Stefano, National Research Council (CNR), Italy
Caitlyn Trevor, University of Zurich, Switzerland

Copyright © 2021 Tomezzoli, Michaud, Gagné, Begon and Duprey. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sonia Duprey, sonia.duprey@univ-lyon1.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.