Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 03 December 2021
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
Volume 15 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.760569

Real-Time Neuropsychological Testing Protocol for Left Temporal Brain Tumor Surgery: A Technical Note and Case Report

Barbara Tomasino1* Ilaria Guarracino1 Tamara Ius2 Marta Maieron3 Miran Skrap2
  • 1Scientific Institute, IRCCS E. Medea, Dipartimento/Unità Operativa Pasian di Prato, Udine, Italy
  • 2SOC Neurochirurgia, Azienda Sanitaria Universitaria Friuli Centrale ASU FC, Udine, Italy
  • 3Fisica Sanitaria, Azienda Sanitaria Universitaria Friuli Centrale ASU FC, Udine, Italy

Background: The risk of surgery in eloquent areas is related to neuropsychological dysfunctions. Maximizing the extent of resection increases the overall survival. The onco-functional balance is mandatory when surgery involves cognitive areas, and maximal information on the cognitive status of patients during awake surgery is needed. This can be achieved using direct cortical stimulation mapping and, in addition to this, a neuropsychological monitoring technique called real-time neuropsychological testing (RTNT). The RTNT includes testing protocols based on the area where the surgery is performed. We reported on tests used for left temporal lobe surgery and our RTNT decision tree.

Case Report: We reported our RTNT experience with a 25-year-old right-handed man with 13 years of schooling. He reported daily partial seizures. MRI revealed the presence of a low-grade glioma involving the temporo-insular cortex. The neuropsychological status presurgery which was within the normal range was combined with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) information. Awake surgery plus RTNT was performed. Direct electrical stimulation during object naming elicited a motor speech arrest. Resection was continuously accompanied by the RTNT. The RTNT provided enriched information to the surgeon. Performance never dropped. A slight decrement in accuracy emerged for pseudoword repetition, short-term memory and working memory, phonological processing, and verbal comprehension. Total resection was performed, and the histological examination confirmed the nature of the lesion. Immediate postsurgery performance was within the normal range as it was the fMRI and DTI assessment.

Conclusion: The RTNT provides essential information that can be used online, during surgery, for clinical aims to provide the surgeon with useful feedback on the cognitive status of patients.

Introduction

Resections in eloquent areas entail the risk of altering the cognitive capacities of patients (Sanai and Berger, 2008). Awake surgery is performed to preserve neuropsychological functions (Ojemann et al., 1989; Ojemann, 1993; Duffau et al., 2003, 2005).

Assessment of cognitive functioning is highly recommended as the neuropsychological profile is an important outcome (Ng et al., 2020). Neuropsychological deficits could arise because of tumor growth or surgery and may affect the quality of life. For instance, Ng et al. (2020) compared pre- and postsurgery cognitive assessments in a consecutive series of 47 patients with (incidental) low-grade glioma (LGG) who underwent awake surgery. Of note, 72.3% of the patients presented stable cognitive functioning, 14.9% of the patients improved cognitive functioning, and 12.8% of the patients showed slight impairments.

Sparing neuropsychological functions during awake surgery is crucial. However, only language or motor functions are routinely mapped. A review on the neuropsychological tests used during awake brain surgery (Ruis, 2018) reported that cognitive functions (i.e., memory, calculation, and emotions) were assessed in a minority of cases.

To test the neuropsychological functions during awake surgery, direct electrical stimulation (DES) mapping is performed (Berger and Ojemann, 1992; Berger, 1994, 1996; Duffau et al., 2000, 2005), while the patient carries out a cognitive task. DES mapping minimizes the risk of permanent postoperative deficits while maximizing the quality of resection, especially in infiltrating tumors such as LGGs where the larger the resection, the longer the survival of a patient (Duffau et al., 2003; Keles et al., 2004; Sanai and Berger, 2008, 2011; Sanai et al., 2010; Ius et al., 2012). The surgeon determines where DES evokes an interference response and imports this spatial information into a 3D MRI image to reproduce a somewhat personalized non-resectability map to guide surgery at cortical and subcortical levels. DES mapping is repeated intra-resections at intervals by pausing excision.

The most frequently used task for surgery in the left Sylvian and peri-Sylvian areas is naming (or counting; Duffau et al., 2000, 2002, 2003; Brennan et al., 2007; Tonn, 2007; Chang et al., 2017). DES mapping at cortical or subcortical portions of the brain induces an interference with task performance. For example, if the patient is carrying out a counting task, DES in Broca’s area may elicit speech arrest. As another example, during a naming task, DES mapping delivered in temporal areas may trigger anomia or phonological and semantic paraphasia. Sometimes, semantic processing can be tested by means of the Pyramids and Palm Trees Test (Talacchi et al., 2013).

Other tests for the left temporal lobe include repetition (Moritz-Gasser and Duffau, 2013; Sierpowska et al., 2017; Leonard et al., 2019). DES to the left peri-Sylvian network causes interference with word repetition with phonological errors, perceptual deficits, and speech arrest (Leonard et al., 2019). Dissociations between naming and pseudoword repetition can be observed: DES to the arcuate fasciculus causes interference with pseudoword repetition (Sierpowska et al., 2017), while DES to the posterior part of the superior and middle temporal gyri induces anomias with preserved word repetition (Moritz-Gasser and Duffau, 2013). These data suggest that it is worth using a multiple testing approach with more than one test.

There are occasional reports of DES during a reading task (Roux et al., 2004, 2012; Gil-Robles et al., 2013; Motomura et al., 2014; Zemmoura et al., 2015; Hirshorn et al., 2016). A study used a somewhat different approach and administered the reading test during resection and not during DES (Tomasino et al., 2020). A decrease in performance (vs. presurgery data) was found in 19/49 patients, and their x, y, and z brain coordinates, where resection causes a decrement, were distributed on the posterior inferior and middle temporal gyrus to the temporoparietal areas and the precentral gyrus (Tomasino et al., 2020).

There are reports on DES during writing (Roux et al., 2003, 2009, 2014; Lubrano et al., 2004; Motomura et al., 2014) and less data on a picture description task to test syntax (Chang et al., 2018).

In addition to DES mapping, surgeons may obtain further information from neuropsychological monitoring providing feedback on the cognitive status of patients during resection. This (Skrap et al., 2016) can be performed using real-time neuropsychological testing (RTNT). The RTNT is listed among the neuropsychological techniques (Tomasino and Rumiati, 2020) available for neuropsychologists as clinical tools. Contrary to DES, the RTNT is performed without pausing excision, as it is administered from the beginning of resection until hemostasis. Upon the request of a surgeon, the RTNT can be temporarily paused to perform DES. An RTNT protocol consists of a battery of neuropsychological tasks tapping the area under resection, e.g., for insular lobe, see Tomasino et al. (2018). It is individually selected for each patient based on the lesion localization, presurgical functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)/diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) results, and presurgical neuropsychological status. Each RTNT task includes 10–15 items (e.g., pictures, sentences, words, and numbers to be repeated) and is repeated several times, by changing items, in order to test several cognitive functions in 1–2 min. Thus, the RTNT can provide continuous feedback on cognitive functions in real time in order to monitor the consequence of each surgical act. A decrease in performance might warrant pausing resection, after searching for confirmation of the functionality of the anatomical site using DES. If RTNT runs are performed correctly, the surgeon resumes resection. Finally, a temporary decrease in performance, which could be related to mechanical manipulation or fatigue of a patient, deserves in-depth investigation. During this time, a “stop-and-go” approach (Sala et al., 2007) is used by the surgeon.

In this case report, we presented our RTNT protocol for resections in the left temporal lobe and describe our multimodal approach combining neuropsychology, fMRI, DTI, and RTNT plus DES. By using the RTNT approach, we aimed to obtain a complete overview of the neuropsychological functions of a patient supported by the craniotomy site and to determine whether—and if so, how—the neuropsychological status evolves during resection. Second, we expected to confirm that this method can be applied in a short resection time.

Materials and Methods

Procedure

We first performed the presurgical neuropsychological assessment followed by fMRI and DTI examinations presurgery. The patient underwent awake surgery plus RTNT, immediate postsurgery neuropsychological assessment 1 week after surgery, and, after 4 months, neuropsychological testing and fMRI follow-up.

Real-Time Neuropsychological Testing Decision Tree

Following the initial neurosurgical consultation and MRI examination, patients with certain features (i.e., brain tumor involving the eloquent cortex) undergo fMRI and DTI along with presurgical neuropsychological assessment. If fMRI and DTI measurements indicate that the brain tumor is infiltrating or rearranging the functional tissue and the clinical characteristics of a patient and cognitive performance are within the normal range on the majority of the tests administered, and provided the patient is not feeling particularly anxious/depressed, awake surgery plus RTNT is discussed as an option (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Decision tree for awake surgery plus RTNT procedure. In the case presented here, we found that the patient—male, young (25 year old), right-handed—had a low-grade glioma involving the temporo-insular cortex. The fMRI and DTI examination indicate that the brain tumor is infiltrating the functional tissue. The neuropsychological assessment showed that the cognitive performance of the patient is within the normal range on the majority of the tests administered, and provided the patient is not feeling particularly anxious/depressed, awake surgery plus RTNT were selected.

Presurgery Neuropsychological Assessment

Extensive neuropsychological assessment was performed on the same day as fMRI and DTI examinations. Neuropsychological assessment covered different cognitive domains, and language skills were specifically tested if the brain tumor was localized in the left hemisphere.

The cognitive functions of a patient were tested by administering a neuropsychological battery involving general functioning as well as language processing (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Neuropsychological status of the patient: presurgery, immediately postsurgery (imm. post), and at follow-up.

Raw scores (RS) for each task were converted into correct scores (CS) for age, education, and sex and then converted into equivalent scores (ES). An ES of 0 indicates pathological performance, PE of 1 borderline performance, and a score of 2–4 indicates normal performance. The reference cutoff value was used for tests in which RS could not be converted into ESs (see Table 1).

Presurgery Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Diffusion Tensor Imaging

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Tasks

Language fMRI tasks included object naming, verb naming, word and pseudoword reading aloud, and tongue movements. Tongue-movement localizer was performed to map the tongue motor-related areas involved in speech that could be taken as a reference for the planning and for the intra-surgery neuronavigation. As far as language mapping is concerned, multiple fMRI language related tasks were used to increase evidence and to obtain confirmation of the fMRI cluster more involved in/or close to the lesion.

In the silent object naming task, each block (N = 4, 15 s each) included seven trials (2,430 ms each). Stimuli were selected from the battery for the analysis of language disorders (Miceli et al., 1994). In the baseline condition (N = 5), a fixation cross (15 s) was presented between blocks.

In the silent verb naming task, each block (N = 4, 15 s each) included seven trials (2,430 ms each). Stimuli were selected from the battery for the analysis of language disorders (Miceli et al., 1994). In the baseline condition (N = 5), a fixation cross (15 s) was presented between blocks.

In the tongue localizer task, each block (N = 4, 15 s each) instructed the patient to make continuous circular tongue movements with closed mouth. In the baseline condition (N = 5), a fixation cross (15 s) was presented between blocks.

In the reading task, each block (N = 11, 15 s each) included 10 trials (1,500 ms each). Eight blocks included words, and three blocks pseudowords. Stimuli were selected from the battery for the analysis of language disorders (Miceli et al., 1994). In the baseline condition (N = 12), a fixation cross (15 s) was presented between blocks.

The Presentation® software (Version 9.9, Neurobehavioral Systems Inc., CA, United States) was used for stimuli presentation and their synchronization with the MR scanner. Patients viewed the stimuli via VisuaStim Goggles (Resonance Technologies, Northrige, United States). Prior to acquisitions, participants practiced the tasks outside the scanner.

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Diffusion Tensor Imaging Data Acquisition and Analyses

The MRI was performed 6–10 days prior to craniotomy. A Philips Achieva 3-T (Best, Netherlands) whole-body scanner was used to acquire DTI, a SENSE-Head-8 channel head coil was used to acquire anatomical and functional images, and a custom-built head restrainer was used to minimize head movements.

Functional images were obtained using a single-shot gradient echo, echoplanar imaging (EPI) sequence. EPI volumes (N = 54) contained 34 axial slices (repetition time [TR] = 2,500 ms, echo time [TE] = 35 ms, field of view [FOV] = 230 mm, matrix: 128 × 128; slice thickness of 3 mm with no gaps, 90° flip angle, voxel size: 1.79 mm × 1.79 mm × 3.3 mm) and were preceded by four dummy images so that the MR scanner could reach a steady state.

Diffusion tensor data were acquired using an axial diffusion-weighted, single-shot, spin-echoplanar imaging sequence covering the whole brain (TR = 8,800 ms, TE = 74 ms, bandwidth = 1,287 Hz/pixel, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 224 cm × 224 cm; 70 contiguous axial slices, 1.6-mm slice thickness; matrix size = 224 × 224 voxels). Two b values were used; seven images at 0 s/mm2 (no diffusion weighting) and 64 non-coplanar images at 1,000 s/mm2 (diffusion weighting b value) were acquired. Gradient directions were uniformly distributed on a sphere.

High-resolution T2-weighted and post-gadolinium contrast T1-weighted anatomical MR images were acquired for the intra-operatory stereotactic surgical navigation system. To this purpose, a T1-weighted 3D magnetization-prepared, rapid acquisition gradient-echo fast field echo (T1W_3D_TFE SENSE) pulse sequence (TR = 8,200 ms, TE = 376 ms, FOV = 240,000 mm, 190 sagittal slices of 1-mm thickness, flip angle = 8°, voxel size: 1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm) and a T3-weighted 3D magnetization-prepared, rapid acquisition gradient-echo fast field echo (T2W_3D_TFE SENSE) pulse sequence (TR = 2,500 ms, TE = 35 ms, FOV = 240,000 mm, 190 sagittal slices of 1-mm thickness, flip angle = 90°, voxel size: 1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm) were acquired.

All calculations were performed on UNIX workstations (Ubuntu 8.04 LTS, i386, http://www.ubuntu.com/) using FSL (FMRIB Software Library version 6.0, Oxford, United Kingdom). Dummy images were discarded prior to further image processing. For fMRI data, standard preprocessing and single subject statistics were calculated. DTI data were analyzed using three-dimensional tract reconstructions from DTIStudio (Johns Hopkins, Baltimore, MD, United States) (version 3.0.3) software (Jiang et al., 2006).

Awake Surgery

Intra-Surgery Real-Time Neuropsychological Testing Protocol for the Left Temporo-Insula Area

We administered the RTNT protocol for left temporal resections (see Figure 2). Importantly, the same tasks that were used during fMRI examination were also used during RTNT. Obviously, the fMRI examination has time constraints; therefore, tasks were selected according to lesion localization.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Alternation of DES mapping and RTNT together with the RTNT protocol for left temporal resections. The first run of tasks is reported as an example. Each task includes 10 items.

Some operational definitions are listed as follows:

• “RTNT runs” refers to a complete sequence of tests; once an RTNT run is completed, a second RTNT run is administered by the neuropsychologist, and so on, depending on the time needed for resection.

• “RTNT test” is a specific test, e.g., naming or reading. It appears once every RTNT run.

• “RTNT items” refers to the number of pictures, or words, or sentences (typically 10) included in each RTNT test.

Direct cortical stimulation mapping was performed at the beginning and repeated if needed (see Figure 2 and Table 2). Resection was then followed and was continuously paired with RTNT.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. RTNT performance in Case A.

Immediate Postsurgery Assessment

The same neuropsychological evaluation performed presurgery was performed again 1 week after surgery.

Follow-Up

Patients were routinely retested 4/5 months postsurgery. Patients perform the same neuropsychological tests and fMRI and DTI measurements as those administered presurgery.

Results

Presurgery Neuropsychological Assessment

Patient A is a 25-year-old right-handed man with 13 years of schooling. He is native Italian, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision; he has no history of psychiatric disease or drug abuse.

He reported partial seizures that occurred every day and lasted about 1 min. MRI revealed a LGG (which was confirmed by the postsurgery histological examination evidencing a glial neoplasm). The neuroradiological examination reported that the lesion involved the left fronto-temporo-insular cortex (hyperintense in T2-weighted MRI image, with no contrast-enhancing signs). During the neurosurgical consultation, a presurgery neuropsychological assessment was requested together with an fMRI and DTI examination.

As evidenced by the neuropsychological assessment, the neuropsychological status of the patient was good in almost every cognitive domain (see Table 1). He only performed below average on the visual and auditory lexical decision test.

Patient A was calm, collaborative, and did not feel anxious as revealed by the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, a measure of trait and state anxiety (Spielberger, 1989).

Presurgery Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Diffusion Tensor Imaging Examination

The fMRI examination showed the following:

• The premotor part of the tongue representation is activated mainly contralesionally, at the level of the upper part of the lesion within the premotor cortex.

• Object naming-related activation is found in the left basal temporal area, posterior to, and not affected by, the lesion; activation in the lower premotor cortex is located anterior to the lesion. An additional fMRI cluster was found in the contralesional hemisphere.

• Verb naming-related activation showed similar results as compared to object naming. In addition, the left supramarginal gyrus activation is posterior to the lesion and not contiguous to it.

• Activation in Broca’s area related to reading was localized just in front of the lesion, and a part of the fMRI cluster was found medially. In addition, posterior temporal activation was found in the back of the lesion, bilaterally, and bilateral activation in the Rolandic operculum was found just in front of the lesion (see Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Pre- (left panel) and post (right panel)-surgery fMRI maps for tongue movements (A), object naming (B), verb naming (C), and word/pseudoword reading (D) tasks. Activations are superimposed on the T2-weighted MRI axial slices of the patient.

DTI measurements showed the following:

• The inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus run medially to the lesion and the superior longitudinal fasciculus run externally to it (see Figure 4).

• The number of streamlines of the patient associated with each fascicle and the fractional anisotropy (FA) values were compared with our data of healthy controls (N = 25). We employed independent t-tests that were modified for small sample sizes, using SINGLIMS.EXE (Crawford and Garthwaite, 2002). SINGLIMS tests whether the score of an individual is significantly different from a control or normative sample and provides a point estimate of the abnormality of scores, i.e., it estimates the percentage of the population that would obtain a lower score. It then provides 95% confidence limits on this percentage. The analysis revealed that the FA of the patient of the left superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) and left inferior fronto-occpital fasciculus (IFOF) was significantly lower than the control group. In the right hemisphere, the FA of the patient for the corticospinal tract and IFOF was significantly higher than in controls. As to streamlines, we found that the number of streamlines of the patient for the left corticospinal tract was significantly lower than controls, while it was significantly higher for the right corticospinal tract (see Figure 4 and Table 3).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Pre- (left panel) and post (right panel)-surgery DTI reconstructions of the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (A), superior longitudinal fasciculus (B), and corticospinal tract (C). Plots (D) represent mean fractional anisotropy (FA) and the number of streamlines for the patient and healthy controls.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparisons of the fractional anisotropy of the patient and number of streamlines vs. healthy controls (N = 25).

Awake Surgery

Initial DES mapping was performed during a counting task and an object naming task, at 2.5 mA. Seven series of counting from 1 to 10 were administered. Negative mapping (i.e., DES did not elicit a response) was obtained. DES during object naming elicited motor speech arrest (i.e., with visible orbicularis oris muscle contraction) for three times.

Resection was then followed and was continuously paired with RTNT. Assessment went smoothly, with performance always on a high level (see Table 2). A slight decrement in accuracy was observed for pseudoword repetition, short-term memory, and working memory, for phonological processing and verbal comprehension (Token test). Object naming performed during DES confirmed the initial positive site reported above, where DES elicited a motor speech arrest. No other positive DES sites were found during white matter stimulation. Errors in pseudoword repetition consisted of consonant substitutions (dilanciario in place of bilanciario, froledario in place of froledano, and gufico in place of gupico), or regularizations (e.g., comando in place of comango and compito in place of pontico). At the phonological processing level, the patient matched a word with a wrong phonological target (e.g., maglia/shirt in place of vaglia/money order). The initial short-term memory span of Patient A was 6 and fell to 3 at the last RTNT run. Lexical-decision performance was considered stable since some errors made presurgery were also made during the RTNT. Finally, on the verbal comprehension task, the patient indicated the wrong token in response to the verbal request of the neuropsychologist twice out of 10 RTNT items. In the second half of the RTNT, the patient appeared tired at times and fell asleep. The patient completed four RTNT runs, where the object naming task was repeated two additional times and the word-reading task one additional time, and resection ended. A short break was made after the fifth repetition of object naming. We have previously reported (Skrap et al., 2016) that during the tumor removal, temporary interruption of the patient performance during the testing may occur. We defined this transitory decrement as a reversible neuropsychological deficit (Skrap et al., 2016). We speculated that oscillation in performance, with worsening and recovering accuracy in response within a few minutes. Due to the manipulation of the tissue, similar oscillation could be observed when some surgical traction is applied close to the corticospinal tract or in the medulla. In that case motor and somatosensorial evoked potentials may be temporarily altered.

Total resection was performed. Histological examination confirmed the presence of a glial neoplasm.

Immediate Postsurgery Neuropsychological Examination

The immediate postsurgery performance of Patient A was within the normal range like his presurgery assessment. A non-ceiling performance on the lexical decision test, both on auditory and visual versions, was observed which was in keeping with his presurgery assessment.

Neuropsychological Follow-Up

The performance of Patient A at the follow-up of 5 months after surgery was impaired only on auditory and visual lexical decision. All other tests were within the normal range. These data confirmed the result that there was no change in the performance of the patient pre-postsurgery and at follow-up. We can speculate that this result could be due to different factors. First, Plasticity could be a first reason. Indeed, the fMRI pre- and post-surgery maps show that a contra-lesion activation emerged postsurgery for language tasks. Second, intra-surgery neuropsychological monitoring through the RTNT, could early detect cognitive decrements and prevent deficit. Finally, the pre-surgical fMRI maps showed that the tissue within the hyperintense signal in T2- weighted MRI image was almost not activated, thus it might be not functional. Activations were mainly found at the anterior and posterior lesion margin, and these areas were exposed during craniotomy and were preserved.

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Diffusion Tensor Imaging Follow-Up

The fMRI of Patient A follow-up examination revealed the following:

• Tongue movements elicited a similar activation map as presurgery, with bilateral distribution (presurgery activation was higher in the contra-lesion hemisphere).

• Object naming elicited an activation at the level of Broca’s area anterior to the resection cavity as compared to presurgery.

• Verb naming triggered a wider activation in the premotor cortex and supramarginal gyrus/parietal lobe.

• Reading elicited a similar activation map as presurgery.

The DTI analysis showed the following:

• All tracts were reconstructed normally, as in the presurgery analysis.

• The number of streamlines of the patient associated with each fascicle and the FA values were compared to our data of healthy controls (N = 25). The analysis revealed that FA of the left SLF was significantly lower than controls, while the FA of the right corticospinal tract and of the right IFOF was significantly higher than controls (Figure 4).

Discussion

We presented the RTNT protocol for resections in the left temporal lobe and a case report. We chose to present a single case rather than our experience with the entire series of temporal patients in order to detail the entire RTNT protocol and performance, the procedure, fMRI images, and DTI results.

Our experience suggests that DES remains the gold standard in awake surgery; nonetheless, the surgeon can use the RTNT technique to increase the amount and type of available information about the cognitive status of the patient.

Studies investigating the postoperative cognitive status of patients often report deficits in functions that were not directly assessed during DES, since DES is mainly matched to object naming or counting tasks. In this specific patient, too, DES was performed during object naming and showed a positive speech arrest site. The RTNT added considerable information. This RTNT protocol for temporal resections includes tests that are most commonly used in awake surgery (e.g., see introduction mentioning naming, reading, repetition). In addition, it includes monitoring of short-term memory and working memory, phonological processing, lexical decision, and verbal comprehension (Token test) as well as verb naming.

The same tests were then assessed postsurgery. This is possible since RTNT takes advantage of resection time as the time of test presentation. In this way, surgery time is not extended; rather it is optimized, thanks to the online feedback provided to the surgeon, which enables him to proceed in a more confident manner, when RTNT demonstrates that the patient is responding proficiently to testing.

In the case report illustrated, our patient showed slight decrements in accuracy for pseudoword repetition, short-term memory and working memory, phonological processing, and verbal comprehension (Token test). His postsurgery testing performance was within the normal range. Based on our experience with RTNT, we believe that the lower performance on short-term memory and working memory may be due to fatigue, since immediate postsurgery performance was perfect. The patient was otherwise highly collaborative. In addition, his initial level of vigilance was high while in the final part of the RTNT he was tired and fell asleep. One disadvantage of the RTNT is that it presupposes levels of collaboration and flexibility, which can affect neuropsychological scores such as, in this case example, short-term memory and working memory. It is likely that the slight decrease in the Token test, too, was due to the decrease in short-term memory performance (Pisoni et al., 2019). The warning errors that we considered with much attention during surgery are phonological errors. The patient made some phonologic errors, both in production and in perception. Such errors occurred toward the end of resection.

Our multimodal approach combined RTNT, fMRI, and DTI together with this method. Combining this approach with DES presents some advantages in terms of extent of resection. In a previous study (Ius et al., 2019) using the multimodal approach, we reported a median extent of resection of 98%, compared with a median extent of resection of 83% with DES alone.

Preoperative mapping with fMRI and DTI provides essential, yet insufficient, information. In our opinion, it provides insight during surgery, but only neuropsychological testing can provide accurate levels of information about the cognitive status of the patient. The important aspect is the consistency between instruments and tests. Tongue movement, object naming, verb naming, word reading, and pseudoword reading were used in fMRI. The same tests were used intra-surgery. For example, a mistake made in the presurgery evaluation (e.g., patient saying “to marry” in response to a figure showing a shining ring shine instead of the verb “to shine”) and repeated during the intra-surgery examination as well is considered the baseline of the patient. The response will not be considered an error as it would happen in a formal neuropsychological assessment. In our example, some lexical decision errors were made both presurgery and during RTNT.

The postoperative assessment provides the information such as whether the patient activates or not the same areas as presurgically, whether performance on neuropsychological tests is the same as presurgically, as in our case report. Interestingly, the fMRI assessment also tells us if there is any plasticity or rearrangement pre- or postsurgery in glioma patients (Cargnelutti et al., 2020), showing, for example, that the postsurgery fMRI map for object naming shows an activation in Broca’s area that was not observed presurgery.

Postsurgery DTI assessment and fiber reconstruction indicates whether white matter pathways are preserved, and the results are interpreted in the light of the intra-surgery RTNT performance and according to which white matter pathway was detected during resection. In our case example, two data emerge. First, FA of the left IFOF was significantly lower pre-operatively compared with controls, while FA of the right IFOF was higher than controls both pre- and postoperatively, a sign of plasticity or pre-operative rearrangement. FA of the SLF was lower than controls presurgery and in the normal range postsurgery. Based on pre-operative data, follow-up values should never be viewed as absolute and isolated values but should be compared with pre-operative values. In addition, we are aware that we should be cautious in interpreting the DTI results above, as DTI measurements are sensitive to the presence of “free water” (Weninger et al., 2020). The presence of the tumor itself, or, postsurgery, the presence of the edema could temporarily modify the DTI measurements.

Taken together, the RTNT is highly informative, especially if combined with multimodality data. It does not require further effort. In neurosurgeries where DES is performed, the RTNT does not require additional materials or devices other than the same PC screen used to present DES stimuli. Finally, the RTNT provides essential information that can be used online, during surgery, for clinical aims to provide the surgeon with useful feedback. In addition, RTNT data can be used offline, for research purposes, by listening to patient performance recordings to detect error types and run error analysis or to correlate RTNT data with MRI data according to a multimodal approach.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by CEUR fvg. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MS and BT designed the research. BT, IG, MM, and TI performed the research. BT and MM analyzed the data. BT, MS, and IG wrote the manuscript. All authors edited the manuscript and revised the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

BT is supported by the Italian Ministry of Health (Ricerca Corrente).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the medical imaging staff.

References

Basso, A., Capitani, E., and Laiacona, M. (1987). Raven’s coloured progressive matrices: normative values on 305 adult normal controls. Funct. Neurol. 2, 189–194.

Google Scholar

Berger, M. S. (1994). Lesions in functional (“eloquent”) cortex and subcortical white matter. Clin. Neurosurg. 41, 444–463.

Google Scholar

Berger, M. S. (1996). Minimalism through intraoperative functional mapping. Clin. Neurosurg. 43, 324–337.

Google Scholar

Berger, M. S., and Ojemann, G. A. (1992). Intraoperative brain mapping techniques in neuro-oncology. Stereotactic Funct. Neurosurg. 58, 153–161. doi: 10.1159/000098989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brennan, N. M. P., Whalen, S., Branco, D. D., O’shea, J. P., Norton, I. H., and Golby, A. J. (2007). Object naming is a more sensitive measure of speech localization than number counting: converging evidence from direct cortical stimulation and fMRI. Neuroimage 37, S100–S108. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cargnelutti, E., Ius, T., Skrap, M., and Tomasino, B. (2020). What do we know about pre- and postoperative plasticity in patients with glioma? a review of neuroimaging and intraoperative mapping studies. NeuroImage. Clin. 28:102435. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, E. F., Kurteff, G., and Wilson, S. M. (2018). Selective interference with syntactic encoding during sentence production by direct electrocortical stimulation of the inferiorfrontal gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 411–442. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, W., Pei, Y., Wei, K., Chen, M., Yeh, H., Jaw, F., et al. (2017). Outcomes of a novel naming test applied in intraoperative language mapping for awake brain surgery: a preliminary study. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Med. 10, 10453–10462.

Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., and Garthwaite, P. H. (2002). Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia 40, 1196–1208. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00224-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Renzi, E., and Faglioni, P. (1978). Normative data and screening power of a shortened version of the Token Test. Cortex 14, 41–49. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(78)80006-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Renzi, E., Motti, F., and Nichelli, P. (1980). Imitating gestures. a quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia. Arch. Neurol. 37, 6–10. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1980.00500500036003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffau, H., Capelle, L., Denvil, D., Sichez, N., Gatignol, P., Taillandier, L., et al. (2003). Usefulness of intraoperative electrical subcortical mapping during surgery for low-grade gliomas located within eloquent brain regions: functional results in a consecutive series of 103 patients. J. Neurosurg. 98, 764–778. doi: 10.3171/jns.2003.98.4.0764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffau, H., Capelle, L., Sichez, J. P., Bitar, A., Faillot, T., Arthuis, F., et al. (2000). Surgery of the central nervous system: value of preoperative functional brain mapping by direct electric stimulation. Presse Med. 29, 421–422.

Google Scholar

Duffau, H., Capelle, L., Sichez, N., Denvil, D., Lopes, M., Sichez, J. P., et al. (2002). Intraoperative mapping of the subcortical language pathways using direct stimulations. an anatomo-functional study. Brain 125, 199–214. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffau, H., Lopes, M., Arthuis, F., Bitar, A., Sichez, J. P., Van, E. R., et al. (2005). Contribution of intraoperative electrical stimulations in surgery of low grade gliomas: a comparative study between two series without (1985-96) and with (1996-2003) functional mapping in the same institution. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76, 845–851. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.048520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gamboz, N., Coluccia, E., Iavarone, A., and Brandimonte, M. A. (2009). Normative data for the Pyramids and Palm Trees Test in the elderly Italian population. Neurol. Sci. 30, 453–458. doi: 10.1007/s10072-009-0130-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gil-Robles, S., Carvallo, A., Jimenez, M. M., Gomez, C. A., Martinez, R., Ruiz-Ocana, C., et al. (2013). Double dissociation between visual recognition and picture naming: a study of the visual language connectivity using tractography and brain stimulation. Neurosurgery 72, 678–686. doi: 10.1227/NEU.0b013e318282a361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirshorn, E. A., Li, Y., Ward, M. J., Richardson, R. M., Fiez, J. A., and Ghuman, A. S. (2016). Decoding and disrupting left midfusiform gyrus activity during word reading. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 113, 8162–8167. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1604126113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ius, T., Cesselli, D., Isola, M., Pauletto, G., Tomasino, B., D’Auria, S., et al. (2019). Incidental low-grade gliomas: single-institution management based on clinical, surgical, and molecular data. Neurosurgery 86, 391–399. doi: 10.1093/neuros/nyz114

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ius, T., Isola, M., Budai, R., Pauletto, G., Tomasino, B., Fadiga, L., et al. (2012). Low-grade glioma surgery in eloquent areas: volumetric analysis of extent of resection and its impact on overall survival. a single-institution experience in 190 patients: clinical article. J. Neurosurg. 117, 1039–1052. doi: 10.3171/2012.8.JNS12393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, H., van Zijl, P. C., Kim, J., Pearlson, G. D., and Mori, S. (2006). DtiStudio: resource program for diffusion tensor computation and fiber bundle tracking. Comput. Methods Programs Biomed. 81, 106–116. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2005.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keles, G. E., Lundin, D. A., Lamborn, K. R., Chang, E. F., Ojemann, G., and Berger, M. S. (2004). Intraoperative subcortical stimulation mapping for hemispherical perirolandic gliomas located within or adjacent to the descending motor pathways: evaluation of morbidity and assessment of functional outcome in 294 patients. J. Neurosurg. 100, 369–375. doi: 10.3171/jns.2004.100.3.0369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, M. K., Cai, R., Babiak, M. C., Ren, A., and Chang, E. F. (2019). The peri-Sylvian cortical network underlying single word repetition revealed by electrocortical stimulation and direct neural recordings. Brain Lang. 193, 58–72. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubrano, V., Roux, F. E., and Démonet, J. F. (2004). Writing-specific sites in frontal areas: a corticalstimulation study. J. Neurosurg. 101, 787–798. doi: 10.3171/jns.2004.101.5.0787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luzzatti, C., Laiacona, M., Allamano, N., De Tanti, A., Inzaghi, M. G., and Lorenzi, L. (1994). Un test per la diagnosi dei deficit di scrittura: principi di costruzione e dati normativi. Ric. Psicol. 18, 137–160.

Google Scholar

Miceli, G., Laudanna, A., Burani, C., and Capasso, R. (1994). Batteria per l’analisi dei Deficit Afasici: BADA [BADA: A Battery for the Assessment of Aphasic Disorders.]. Rome: CEPSAG.

Google Scholar

Monaco, M., Costa, A., Caltagirone, C., and Carlesimo, G. A. (2015). Erratum to: forward and backward span for verbal and visuo-spatial data: standardization and normative data from an Italian adult population. Neurol. Sci. 36, 345–347. doi: 10.1007/s10072-014-2019-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz-Gasser, S., and Duffau, H. (2013). The anatomo-functional connectivity of word repetition: insights provided by awake brain tumor surgery. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:405. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motomura, K., Fujii, M., Maesawa, S., Kuramitsu, S., Natsume, A., and Wakabayashi, T. (2014). Association of dorsal inferior frontooccipital fasciculus fibers in the deep parietal lobe with both reading and writing processes: a brain mapping study. J. Neurosurg. 121, 142–148. doi: 10.3171/2014.2.JNS131234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, S., Herbet, G., Lemaitre, A. L., Cochereau, J., Moritz-Gasser, S., and Duffau, H. (2020). Neuropsychological assessments before and after awake surgery for incidental low-grade gliomas. J. Neurosurg. Online ahead of print. doi: 10.3171/2020.7.JNS201507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novelli, G., Papagno, C., Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Vallar, G., and Cappa, S. F. (1996). Tre test clinici di ricerca e produzione lessicale. Taratura su soggetti normali. Arch. Psicol. Neurol. Psichiatr. 47, 477–505.

Google Scholar

Ojemann, G., Ojemann, J., Lettich, E., and Berger, M. (1989). Cortical language localization in left, dominant hemisphere. an electrical stimulation mapping investigation in 117 patients. J. Neurosurg. 71, 316–326.

Google Scholar

Ojemann, G. A. (1993). Functional mapping of cortical language areas in adults. intraoperative approaches. Adv. Neurol. 63, 155–163.

Google Scholar

Pisoni, A., Mattavelli, G., Casarotti, A., Comi, A., Riva, M., Bello, L., et al. (2019). The neural correlates of auditory-verbal short-term memory: a voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping study on 103 patients after glioma removal. Brain Structure Funct. 224, 2199–2211. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01902-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ripamonti, E., Aggujaro, S., Molteni, F., Zonca, G., Frustaci, M., and Luzzatti, C. (2014). The anatomical foundations of acquired reading disorders: a neuropsychological verification of the dual-route model of reading. Brain Lang. 134, 44–67. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ripamonti, E., Lucchelli, F., Lazzatti, G., Martini, E., and Luzzatti, C. (2016). Reading impairment in neurodegenerative diseases: a multiple single-case study. Aphasiology 31, 519–541.

Google Scholar

Roux, F. E., Boetto, S., Sacko, O., Chollet, F., and Tremoulet, M. (2003). Writing, calculating, and finger recognition in the region of the angular gyrus: a cortical stimulation study of Gerstmann syndrome. J. Neurosurg. 99, 716–727. doi: 10.3171/jns.2003.99.4.0716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roux, F. E., Dufor, O., Giussani, C., Wamain, Y., Draper, L., Longcamp, M., et al. (2009). The graphemic/motor frontal area Exner’s area revisited. Ann. Neurol. 66, 537–545. doi: 10.1002/ana.21804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roux, F. E., Durand, J. B., Jucla, M., Réhault, E., Reddy, M., and Démonet, J. F. (2012). Segregation of lexical and sub-lexical reading processes in the left perisylvian cortex. PLoS One 7:e50665. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roux, F. E., Durand, J. B., Rehault, E., Planton, S., Draper, L., and Demonet, J. F. (2014). The neural basis for writing from dictation in the temporoparietal cortex. Cortex 50, 64–75. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roux, F. E., Lubrano, V., Lauwers-Cances, V., Tremoulet, M., Mascott, C. R., and Demonet, J. F. (2004). Intra-operative mapping of cortical areas involved in reading in mono- and bilingual patients. Brain 127, 1796–1810. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruis, C. (2018). Monitoring cognition during awake brain surgery in adults: a systematic review. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 40, 1081–1104.

Google Scholar

Sala, F., Bricolo, A., Faccioli, F., Lanteri, P., and Gerosa, M. (2007). Surgery for intramedullary spinal cord tumors: the role of intraoperative (neurophysiological) monitoring. Eur. Spine J. 16, (Suppl. 2), S130–S139.

Google Scholar

Sanai, N., and Berger, M. S. (2008). Glioma extent of resection and its impact on patient outcome. Neurosurgery 62, 753–766. doi: 10.1227/01.neu.0000318159.21731.cf

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanai, N., and Berger, M. S. (2011). Extent of resection influences outcomes for patients with gliomas. Rev. Neurol. (Paris) 167, 648–654. doi: 10.1016/j.neurol.2011.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanai, N., Polley, M. Y., and Berger, M. S. (2010). Insular glioma resection: assessment of patient morbidity, survival, and tumor progression. J. Neurosurg. 112, 1–9. doi: 10.3171/2009.6.jns0952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sierpowska, J., Gabarrós, A., Fernandez-Coello, A., Camins, À, Castañer, S., Juncadella, M., et al. (2017). Words are not enough: nonword repetition as an indicator of arcuate fasciculus integrity during brain tumor resection. J. Neurosurg. 126, 435–445. doi: 10.3171/2016.2.jns151592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skrap, M., Marin, D., Ius, T., Fabbro, F., and Tomasino, B. (2016). Brain mapping: a novel intraoperative neuropsychological approach. J. Neurosurg. 125, 877–887. doi: 10.3171/2015.10.jns15740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D. (1989). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Bibliography, 2nd Edn. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Spinnler, M., and Tognoni, G. (1987). Standardizzazione e taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici. Italian J. Neurol. Sci. 6, (Suppl. 8), 1–120.

Google Scholar

Talacchi, A., Casartelli, M., Capasso, R., and Miceli, G. (2013). Awake surgery between art and science. Part II: language and cognitive mapping. Funct. Neurol. 28:223. doi: 10.11138/FNeur/2013.28.3.223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasino, B., Ius, T., Skrap, M., and Luzzatti, C. (2020). Phonological and surface dyslexia in individuals with brain tumors: performance pre-, intra-, immediately post-surgery and at follow-up. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 5015–5031. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasino, B., Marin, D., Ius, T., and Skrap, M. (2018). “Neuropsychology in insular lesions prior-during and after brain surgery,” in Island of Reil (Insula) in the Human Brain: Anatomical, Functional, Clinical and Surgical Aspects, eds M. Turgut, C. Yurtta, and S. Tubbs (Berlin: Springer).

Google Scholar

Tomasino, B., and Rumiati, R. I. (2020). “Methods in neuropsychology,” in Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience, 2nd Edn, ed. Sergio Della Sala (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 246–254. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-809324-5.23928-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tonn, J. (2007). Awake craniotomy for monitoring of language function: benefits and limits. Acta Neurochir. 149, 1197–1118. doi: 10.1007/s00701-007-1368-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toraldo, A., Cattani, B., Zonca, G., Saletta, P., and Luzzatti, C. (2006). Reading disorders in a language with shallow orthography: a multiple single-case study in Italian. Aphasiology 20, 823–850. doi: 10.1080/02687030600738838

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weninger, L., Na, C. H., Jütten, K., and Merhof, D. (2020). Analyzing the effects of free water modeling by deep learning on diffusion MRI structural connectivity estimates in glioma patients. PLoS One 15:e0239475. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0239475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zemmoura, I., Herbet, G., Moritz-Gasser, S., and Duffau, H. (2015). New insights into the neural network mediating reading processes provided by cortico-subcortical electrical mapping. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 2215–2230. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: awake surgery, brain mapping, glioma, neuropsychology, plasticity

Citation: Tomasino B, Guarracino I, Ius T, Maieron M and Skrap M (2021) Real-Time Neuropsychological Testing Protocol for Left Temporal Brain Tumor Surgery: A Technical Note and Case Report. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:760569. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.760569

Received: 18 August 2021; Accepted: 10 November 2021;
Published: 03 December 2021.

Edited by:

Carlo Semenza, University of Padua, Italy

Reviewed by:

Sara Cruz, Lusíada University of Porto, Portugal
Antonino F. Germano’, University of Messina, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Tomasino, Guarracino, Ius, Maieron and Skrap. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Barbara Tomasino, Barbara.tomasino@lanostrafamiglia.it

Download