Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 23 August 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Reading and Writing Skills: Cognitive, Emotional, Creative, and Digital Approaches View all 41 articles

Contribution of oral narrative textual competence and spelling skills to written narrative textual competence in bilingual language-minority children and monolingual peers

  • Department of Education, Languages, Intercultures, Literatures and Psychology (Psychology Section), University of Florence, Florence, Italy

This study investigates the developmental pattern and relationships between oral narrative textual skills, spelling, and written narrative textual skills in monolingual and bilingual language-minority (BLM) children, L1-Chinese and L2-Italian. The aims were to investigate in monolingual and BLM children: (1) the developmental patterns of oral and writing skills across primary school years; (2) the pattern of relationships (direct and mediated) between oral narrative textual competence, spelling skills, and written narrative textual competence with age and socio-economic status (SES) taken under control. In total, 141 primary school children from grades 2 to 5 in Central Italy (44% BLM, 56% monolinguals) aged between 7 and 11 years (M-age = 8.59, SD = 1.13; 41% girls, 59% boys) obtained scores for oral and written narrative textual competence, spelling accuracy in dictation, and written texts. One-way ANOVA and ANOVA with robust method (Welch test) analyses and Bonferroni’s correction showed that BLM children had poorer spelling skills in dictation and written narrative textual competence (i.e., text structure) than their monolingual peers. After preliminary correlation analysis, the results of hierarchical regression showed that the relationship between oral and written narrative textual competence is completely mediated by spelling accuracy in BLM children. These results suggest that adequate performance in written narrative textual competence depends on adequate spelling accuracy in writing stories. The Sobel test verified the power of this mediation. In monolinguals, the strongest predictor of written narrative textual competence is oral narrative textual competence. This relation is stronger in older children whose spelling skills are automatized. The identified pattern of relationships shows a complex network of oral and written processes. The scarce spelling skills characterizing BLM children may explain why spelling skills determine a low written narrative textual level. Scarce spelling skills absorb cognitive resources, hindering high-level cognitive processes that regulate narrative production. In monolinguals, the medium of writing does not impact narrative textual competence. Children’s oral narrative textual competence easily transfers into their written narrative productions. These findings have implications for the assessment and instruction of literacy skills in young BLM children and their monolingual peers.

Introduction

An exponential growing number of students in school classrooms worldwide are bilingual language-minority (BLM) children. These children experience the most intensive and sustained exposure to their second language (L2) when they begin formal education because they come from homes where the language background is different from the societal language (e.g., Hoff et al., 2021). Previous studies indicate that BLM children have fewer emergent orthographic knowledge in kindergarten (Incognito et al., 2021) and writing skills in school years (Goodrich et al., 2016) with detrimental effects on achievement and school outcomes. A serious disadvantage also for online communication in written form, a widespread practice (Alarcón et al., 2020). Theory and evidence indicate that writing narrative production skills are closely linked to the development of oral and spelling skills. Therefore, this study aimed to examine the developmental pattern and (direct and mediated) relations of L2 oral and writing skills among school-age children, including BLM children and their monolingual peers. Previous research shows the development of spelling and written narrative textual competence skills in monolingual, whereas oral narrative textual competence skills are less explored, especially across primary schools. Little is known about the developmental pattern and relations between oral and writing skills in BLM children acquiring two structurally and morphologically distinct languages. To our knowledge, this is one of the few attempts to study different skills across the primary school years in the profile of BLM children acquiring L1-Chinese and L2-Italian.

Impact of oral language and spelling skills on writing skills

In this study, we refer to a model of writing development that comprehends important distal and proximal factors. Models of writing development, based on English, such as the “simple view of writing,” describe how proximal lower-level transcription skills (e.g., spelling skills), distal higher-level processes (e.g., planning, translating, and reviewing), and general cognitive skills (e.g., working memory, reading, attention) contribute to producing written texts. The updated model of writing, the so-called “not-so-simple view,” includes executive functions (e.g., attention, planning, reviewing) as core, describing how these components evolve and interact with transcription skills (handwriting or typing and spelling) in text generation. Consistent with previous models, mainly derived from opaque orthographies, beginning writers are mainly engaged in managing “lower-level processes,” such as proximal spelling skills (Hayes, 2012). Researchers agree that this step is particularly challenging for novice writers who are engaged in translating their ideas into text (Graham and Harris, 2000) by observing the grammatical and spelling rules of the language system (Graham, 2006). Recently, oral language and reading skills have been incorporated into these models as distal factors that contribute to the production of written text (e.g., Kim and Park, 2019). The recent “Direct and Indirect Effects model of Writing” (DIEW; Kim and Schatschneider, 2017; Kim, 2020) further demonstrates that both oral text generation and spelling skills are necessary to support writing quality. In novice writers, difficulties in spelling skills may hinder writing narrative textual processes and interfere with the transfer of oral narrative textual competence to written narrative textual productions. It is important to consider that the impact of spelling skills on writing texts may vary according to the characteristics of orthographies. Most evidence derived from studies typically focuses on English, where children’s spelling skills are challenged by an opaque language system with a strong inconsistency between its phoneme and grapheme correspondences. This research emphasizes the importance of investigating writing development in relation to children’s linguistic and socio-economic backgrounds (e.g., Incognito et al., 2021). We still know only very little about the role of spelling skills in writing texts in school-age BLM children acquiring Italian as L2. Italian is defined as a transparent or “shallow” language because it shows consistent sound-sign matching, similar to Finnish, German, and Spanish. Research indicates that for Italian monolingual children, the consistency between sound and sign matching sustains them in advancing quickly to writing sentences, and by the end of the first grade, children are able to write short texts (Bigozzi et al., 2016a). Further studies in Italian monolingual children (Bigozzi and Vettori, 2016) indicate that only in the case of a significant disadvantage in spelling skills do first graders’ oral narrative textual competence cease to sustain written narrative textual competence. Little is known about the pattern of (direct and mediated) relationships between oral narrative textual competence, spelling skills, and written narrative textual competence in children acquiring transparent Italian orthography through primary school years.

Writing a story in L2

In studying children’s written narrative textual competence, it is interesting to distinguish which factors are sustained in the early or late phase of learning to write in primary school, because the characteristics of orthography may exert a different role on novice or more expert writers and monolingual or BLM children. Previous longitudinal studies on the transition from preschool to formalized education (e.g., Pinto et al., 2015, 2016) indicate that Italian preschool children’s oral narrative textual competence influences their later written narrative competence in first and second grades via a mediational effect of orthographic competence. Instead, there has been little research on how L2 spelling skills contribute to L2 written narrative textual competence in BLM children acquiring two orthographically distant languages: L1-Chinese (non-alphabet) and L2-Italian (alphabet and transparent orthography). The limited L2-Italian language input in the family context can negatively impact the acquisition of adequate L2 spelling skills (Roch and Hržica, 2020). Prior research indicates that BLM children use word knowledge in their L1 when writing in their second language (L2) (e.g., code-switching between two languages) (e.g., Gort, 2001). This adaptive strategy sometimes conducts to a “negative transfer” across languages (e.g., Howard et al., 2012), leading children to commit L1-influenced spelling errors when writing in the societal language (e.g., “l” instead of “r”). The Italian language as a transparent orthography might reduce cognitive demands on young writers in spelling, but less is known about the relationship between spelling and narrative texts when writers must manage the demand of the morphosyntactic complexity of this language.

Aim and hypothesis

In this study, we examine the contribution of oral language and spelling skills to written narrative textual competence in a specific population of school-aged BLM children acquiring distant structural and morphological languages, L1-Chinese (non-alphabet) and L2-Italian (alphabet and transparent orthography), and their monolingual peers. Previous research has mainly focused on the transition from preschool to formalized education (e.g., Pinto et al., 2009, 2012; Bigozzi and Vettori, 2016). In this study, we focus on the important period of primary school years when children from grades 2 to 5 are expected to have almost automatized orthographic competence in Italian. Previous results have confirmed the usefulness of adopting both dictation and spontaneous spelling of words to assess children’s orthographic skills (Bigozzi et al., 2016a). In this study, two measures of spelling skills were adopted to gain a more complete picture of the involvement of spelling skills in the process of writing text by using both a measure in the process of writing text and an external measure of the level of proficiency in a dictation task.

This study contributes to closing the aforementioned gaps with the following aims.

First, we aimed to investigate the developmental patterns of BLM and monolingual children’s oral narrative textual competence, spelling skills, and written narrative textual competence across primary school years. We expect an improvement in children’s capacity to tell and write stories and in their spelling accuracy across primary school years. Specifically, we are interested in providing a better understanding of this improvement in the outcome measures of children in primary school by verifying floor or ceiling effects: whether there is rapid development or only gradual improvements during that period and whether there are any developmental plateaus.

Second, we aimed to examine the patterns of direct and mediated relationships between oral narrative textual competence, spelling skills, and written narrative textual competence in school-aged BLM children and their monolingual peers. We expect oral narrative textual competence and spelling skills to play a significant predictive role in written narrative textual competence. Specifically, we expect that the contribution of spelling skills is influenced by the level of proficiency, which is expected to be lower in novice writers and BLM children in comparison with monolinguals. Regarding monolingual children, we expect spelling skills to have a less significant influence on their written narrative textual competence, increasing with primary school grades, considering that most children show an adequate mastering of spelling skills in the Italian transparent orthography since the early years.

Materials and methods

Participants

A total of 141 primary school children aged 7–11 years living in Italy (M-age = 8.59, SD = 1.13; 83 girls and 58 boys) participated in this study as follows: 79 children (56%) were monolinguals, Italian-as-L1 children, exposed to the societal language both at home and school, and 62 children (44%) were Chinese-speaking language-minority bilingual children exposed to an L1 other than Italian in the family context. All Chinese children are born in Italy to parents who are both Chinese. In this study, the great proportion of Chinese as children’s minority language is related to the fact that they belong to a wide area in central Italy with a long history of Chinese immigrants. Given that, in Italy, children usually go to the public school closest to where they live, and classrooms saw a significant presence of Chinese bilingual language minorities. Children attending different primary school classrooms were included in this study: second grade N = 30 (monolingual = 14; BLM = 16); third grade N = 35 (monolingual = 20; BLM = 15); fourth grade N = 43 (monolingual = 25; BLM = 18); and fifth grade N = 33 (monolingual = 20; BLM = 13). Children with any known special educational needs or impairments/disorders were excluded to avoid any additional difficulties that could potentially affect their performance. School authorities, parents, and children provided consent to participate in the study. Background information about home language characteristics and sociocultural-economic status, defined as parents’ educational level (i.e., International Standard Classification of Education; ISCED-11; UNESCO Institute for Statistics, 2012), were collected using a parental questionnaire attached to the informed consent sheet. In Italy, first-grade teachers focus primarily on the spelling component of writing, whereas second-grade teachers focus on the textual properties of writing because second graders are expected to have finalized the acquisition of orthography (National Indications of the Italian Ministry of Education, hereafter MIUR, 2012). The children’s ages range from 7 to 10 years, corresponding to the second to fifth grades in school.

Measures

Narrative tasks: Oral and written narrative textual competence

Two narrative tasks were adopted to measure children’s oral and written narrative textual competence. In a collective session in the classroom during school, the children individually performed a written narrative task. Each child wrote an invented story. Furthermore, in an individual session in a quiet room next to the classroom, children individually performed an oral narrative task. Each child was asked to tell a story they invented. Each story told was transcribed and codified. With respect to the aims of this study, each written and oral narrative was codified for the index of text structure. To identify the level of text structure in children’s written and oral stories, in line with previous research (e.g., Spinillo and Pinto, 1994; Pinto et al., 2020), the presence/absence of the 8 elements which characterize the narrative genre (e.g., title, opening, setting, description of character/s, problem, central event, resolution of the problem, and story ending) were identified to assign the corresponding score on a five-point scale as follows:

Score 1 – no narrative: Simple description or list of events, objects, or facts;

Score 2 – sketch narrative: Opening, setting, character(s), conclusion or opening, the sketch of the problem, and resolution;

Score 3 – incomplete narrative: Opening, character(s), problem, and resolution;

Score 4 – essential narrative: Opening, character(s), problem, central event, and resolution;

Score 5 – complete narrative: Title, opening, character(s), setting, problem, central event, resolution, and narrative closing.

The rate of agreement between the judges was 99%; cases of disagreement were resolved through discussion. In summary, each child in this study obtained a score for

• “oral narrative textual competence,” derived from the score of structure in oral texts;

• “written narrative textual competence,” derived from the score of structure in written texts.

A dictation task and a written narrative task: Spelling skills

To assess children’s spelling skills, a paper-and-pencil text dictation was performed individually by children in a collective session in the classroom during school time. The dictation task was taken (BVSCO) from the “Battery for the Evaluation of Writing and Orthographic Competence in Primary School” standardized for the Italian population. The children listened to a recorded text, and each child had to write down the text. To measure children’s spelling skills in the two tasks (dictation and written narrative), the orthographic errors were identified based on the classification of the orthographic errors by Pinto et al. (2012) which covers the entire variability of orthographic errors that children may commit in the Italian language. As indicated in previous research (see e.g., Bigozzi et al., 2017), it is important to gain a comprehensive score of orthographic error including both the cases in which the pronunciation of the target word is preserved despite the spelling violation (e.g., “anno” [year] instead of “hanno” [they have]), and both the cases in which the pronunciation of the target word is changed due to a spelling violation (“mecrato” instead of “mercato”). All phonemes are possible sources of non-homophone errors such that spelling errors of this type result in phonetically implausible words. Writing accuracy was determined by the total number of orthographic errors (see, Pinto et al., 2012), which were counted as many times as the error occurred.

Sociocultural-economic status

A parental questionnaire attached to the informed consent form was used to collect information about the children’s family sociocultural-economic status (SES). For this study, the index of ISCED level was considered as follows: a score from “ISCED 1 – Primary education” till “ISCED 6 – bachelor’s or master’s degree” was given for the educational level of fathers and mothers. Children’s SES scores were calculated based on the higher ISCED level between parents. The measure consists of the number of years of father and mother’s education (see also, Haman et al., 2017).

Data analysis

First of all, we grouped the participants as follows: the youngest were the students attending second and third grades (total N = 65; monolingual = 34, BLM = 31); the oldest were the students attending fourth and fifth grades (total N = 76; monolingual = 45, BLM = 31). This division stems from the fact that second graders and third graders have similar spelling skills, in fact, it is at the end of the third grade that spelling proficiency is considered acquired. The first graders are not part of the sample because they are too immature in terms of learning. By third grade, they have acquired spelling skills, and in fourth and fifth-grades, children master spelling.

Before analyzing the data, according to Tabachnick and Fidell’s (2013) recommendation, the presence of univariate outliers in the oral and written narrative skills scores was checked. No outliers are observed. Preliminarily, the homogeneity of variances was checked by Levene’s test. To investigate the oral narrative, spelling, and written narrative text production skills in L2-Italian shown by BLM (L1-Chinese and L2-Italian) and their monolingual Italian-speaking peers, one-way ANOVA was used. In the case of non-homogeneity of variances, ANOVA with robust methods was used (Welch’s Test). Moreover, we used Bonferroni’s correction to control the family-wise error rate (αnew = αold/n). Correlation analyses were then performed to investigate the relationships between variables as preliminary analyses for regression assumptions. Finally, we used hierarchical regression analysis to verify the increment in variation accounted for by the addition of predictors over a set of models (Figure 1). This is generally assessed by testing the change in R-square from one model to the next. If, after the inclusion of predictors at a given step, the R-square change is significantly greater than zero, we infer that the predictors added at that step offer incremental predictive power. The R-square change (increment) from Model 1 to Model 2 is computed as ΔR2 = Model 2 R2 – Model 1 R2. The R-square change (increment) from Model 2 to Model 3 is computed as ΔR2 = Model 3 R2 – Model 2 R2 (Darlington and Hayes, 2017).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hierarchical regression models.

To calculate the probability, if any, we used the Sobel test when the indirect effect of an independent variable on a dependent variable through a mediating variable is significant. Many studies rely on mediating models, and identifying whether a mediating variable significantly mediates the influence of an independent variable on a dependent variable is critical when assessing the value of such models.

Results

Before analyzing the data, based on the parents’ educational level, the sample was distributed as follows: 1.4% primary school, 35.5% middle school, 25.5% 3-year professional qualification, 32.6% high school, 1.4% another higher education qualification other than a high-school diploma (conservatory, arts), and 3.5% master’s degree. Preliminary descriptive statistics and comparative analyses between monolinguals and BLM (one-way ANOVA and ANOVA with the robust method – Welch’s test) for the main variables are shown in Table 1. Using Bonferroni’s correction, the new alpha level is 0.01. The results show that statistically significant differences were found between monolingual and BLM children in spelling inaccuracy in dictation and written narrative skills. Specifically, BLM children exhibited a significantly higher number of errors than monolingual children. Regarding written narrative skills, BLM children performed statistically worse than their monolingual peers.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive analyses and one-way ANOVA and ANOVA with robust methods results.

Table 2 shows descriptive statistics and comparison analyses between younger and older children in monolinguals and BLM (one-way ANOVA and ANOVA with the robust method – Welch’s test). The results show that both in monolingual and BLM, performance in written narrative skills and orthographic accuracy improve with age. However, there is no significant increase in oral narrative skills. Using the Bonferroni Correction, the new alpha level is 0.01.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive analyses and one-way ANOVA and ANOVA with robust methods results for monolingual and BLM children at school level.

Correlation analyses were conducted to determine the relationships between the variables. Scores on written narrative skills were associated with the age of participants, oral narrative skills, and spelling accuracy in narration and dictation. Table 3 presents the results.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlation analyses.

Hierarchical regression analysis was performed to determine whether performance in oral narrative skills and spelling accuracy in narrative and dictation improved participants’ prediction of their written narrative skills beyond that provided by age and parents’ level of education. These predictors were used in the equation because of their statistically significant correlations with written narrative skills. Table 4 shows the standardized regression coefficients (β), R2, and change R2 (ΔR2) for monolingual children.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Stepwise regression for monolingual children (dependent variable: written narrative skills).

In Model 1, age and parents’ level of education accounted for significant variations in written narrative skills (R-square = 0.19, F(2,52) = 6.20, p = 0.004). In Model 2, age, parents’ level of education, and oral narrative skills accounted for significant variations in written narrative skills (R-square = 0.32, F(3,51) = 8.18, p < 0.001). The change in R-square from Model 1 to Model 2 was 0.13, reflecting a significant increase in the explained variation [F(1,51) = 9.99, p < 0.01]. In Model 3, the predictors accounted for significant variation in written narrative skills (R-square = 0.36, F(5,49) = 5.57, p < 0.001). The change in R-square from Model 2 to Model 3 was 0.04, which does not reflect a significant increase in explained variation [F(2,49) = 1.44, p = 0.247]. These results show that the strongest predictor of written narrative skills in monolingual children is oral narrative skills. Moreover, the age of participants as a control variable is presumably contributing to independent variance, as suggested by previous correlational analyses. This is explained by the fact that the beta value decreased and the p-value increased for age, in Model 3.

Table 5 contains the standardized regression coefficients (β), R2, and change R2 (ΔR2) for BLM children. In Model 1, age and parents’ level of education accounted for significant variation in written narrative skills (R-square = 0.30, F(2,30) = 5.19, p = 0.004). In Model 2, age, parent’s level of education, and oral narrative skills accounted for significant variations in written narrative skills (R-square = 0.39, F(3,29) = 6.23, p = 0.002). The change in R-square from Model 1 to Model 2 was 0.09, reflecting a significant increase in explained variation [F(1,29) = 4.24, p < 0.05]. In Model 3, the predictors accounted for significant variations in written narrative skills, R-square = 0.54, F(5,27) = 3.71, p < 0.001. The change in R-square from Model 2 to Model 3 was 0.15, reflecting a significant increase in explained variation, F(2,27) = 4.39, p < 0.05. These results suggest that only spelling accuracy in written narrative texts is the best predictor of written narrative skills in BLM children; therefore, good performance in written narrative skills depends on good spelling accuracy in writing stories. Specifically, the model in step 3 improves significantly. In addition, in step 2, the oral narrative skills were significant, whereas, in step 3, they lost significance. This suggests that in the relationship between oral and written narrative skills, narrative orthographic accuracy completely mediates this relationship. We used the Sobel test to test the power of this mediation. The results indicate that there is a total mediation of narrative orthographic accuracy performance in the relationship between oral and written narrative skills (Sobel test statistic = 2.23, p < 0.05; Figure 2). Figure 2 explains the mediational model, and beta values and significances are shown for each distinct relationship.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Stepwise regression for BLM children (dependent variable: written narrative skills).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mediational analysis model for BLM children. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

Discussion

This study provides results on developmental patterns and relationships between oral narrative textual competence, spelling skills, and written narrative textual competence in two groups of school-age children, bilingual language-minority children, and their monolingual peers. The cross-sectional research design of this study provides results on the change and stability of the outcome measures throughout the primary school years.

As expected, oral narrative textual competence, spelling skills, and written narrative textual competence were found to develop throughout the primary school years in both language groups, BLM children and their monolingual peers, although some developmental trends were different within writing and oral domains and between language groups. From the comparison between young and older monolingual primary school children, a growth trend in oral narrative textual competence was observed. As presumably, older monolingual children produced oral stories with more structural information (i.e., story grammar units) than did the younger. This result shed light on a growth trend less documented in the Italian population and they are in line with previous studies on narrative development in diverse languages (Mäkinen et al., 2020). However, it is important to note that when considering BLM children’s oral narrative textual competence, the results show a developmental trend from the younger to older BLM primary school children with only minor improvement from the score around 2 (sketch narrative) to the score around 3 (incomplete narrative). Although a trend of improvement in oral narrative competence was observed both in bilingual and monolingual, the results did not reach significance, and this is somewhat surprising since narrative structure generally tends to improve significantly with age. The finding that the narrative structure in the BLM’s oral narratives reported only an improving trend, which was not statistically significant, is consistent with the fact that children’s oral narrative skills are poorly supported by teaching in primary school. Indeed, primary school focuses mainly on reading and writing skills, which, although related to the oral domain, are not yet able to influence the oral domain. In primary school years, children are scarcely engaged in telling oral narratives as for reading or listening to stories to the advantage of the teaching of writing (see e.g., Ministry of Education, University and Research MIUR, 2012).

Turning to spell skills, consistent growth in spelling accuracy occurred from younger to older primary school BLM and monolingual children. This is in line with previous studies on Italian children that document an age-related decline in errors, such as errors of omission, inversion, and non-phonological type together in dictation and written stories. This result can be confirmed for a specific population of BLM children acquiring the phoneme–grapheme correspondence, which is at the base of orthographic competence, in two languages with large differences. When taking a closer look at written narrative textual competence, performance was in the expected direction, with increasing primary school grades associated with improved performance in both language groups, BLM and their monolingual peers. Specifically, in monolingual children, the narrative structure in the younger group was around 2, indicating “sketch stories,” instead in the older group was a little above 4 moving toward an “essential narrative” with opening, character(s), problem, central event, and resolution. These results show increasing complexity in the mental model of the story, in line with previous studies (Pinto et al., 2015). In the younger BLM children group, the narrative structure was around 1, indicating a simple “description or list” of events, objects, or facts, whereas, in the older BLM children group, it was around 3, indicating an “incomplete story” with a conventional opening, character(s), problem, and resolution.

The two language groups (i.e., BLM and monolingual) display the same changes in oral and written domains across primary school years and are united by the school experience which focuses on reading and writing instruction while leaving aside oral practice. Alongside the similarities in growth, the comparison of oral narrative textual skills, spelling, and written narrative textual skills between monolinguals and BLMs shows that BLMs show quite equal oral narrative skills to monolinguals setting around the mean-level of 2 “sketch story” and 3 “incomplete story.” For what concerns the comparison of spelling skills, BLM children’s level of acquisition of sound-sign correspondence was significantly lower than those of their monolingual peers in the dictation task. As reported in the literature, a disadvantage at the orthographic level in BLM children has been observed since preschool when BLM children have reported lower levels of notational awareness than their monolingual peers (Incognito et al., 2021). Notational awareness refers to preschoolers’ conceptual knowledge of the writing system assessed by the “invented spelling task” requiring the ability to process forms of writing similar to conventional spelling. The invented spelling task, administered by the researcher, consists of asking the child to write a few words in order to assess, through various indicators, to what extent to which the child is able to produce signs more or less similar to letters and to read following with her/his finger what the child has written, in an attempt to make each sign correspond more or less correctly to a sound. Notational awareness is also assessed through the ability of the child to vary the number of signs in relation to the variation of the number of sounds in the word (Bigozzi et al., 2016a). Notational awareness represents a significant predictor of later reading and writing skills of primary school children in the Italian language system (Bigozzi et al., 2016b). In addition to the evidence that clarifies that children’s L2 exposure is strongly associated with their L2 language skills (e.g., Hoff, 2018), our results that BLM children were inferior in spelling skills in comparison to monolingual peers allow us to consider the influence of the characteristics of orthography, given that they were acquiring two distant structural and morphological languages, L1-Chinese (non-alphabet) and L2-Italian (alphabet and transparent orthography). While BLM children’s oral narratives are preserved, the obstacle of orthographic coding absorbs cognitive resources to the construction of a rich and detailed mental model of a story, aggravating the functioning of working memory for planning the text (Berninger and Swanson, 1994; Hayes, 2012). As previous writing models showed, children’s automatization of spelling skills enables them to free up memory and executive function resources to be devoted to higher-level processes of writing (e.g., generation of ideas, planning, and revision). This laborious search for words and correct transcription persists in older BLMs making the arduous circulation of knowledge between oral and written. These results of the comparisons between BLM vs. monolinguals corroborate the understanding of how important it is to master spelling so that it does not represent an obstacle to more creative and ideational processes such as textual writing, as already demonstrated in previous studies in other age groups (e.g., Pinto et al., 2015; Bigozzi and Vettori, 2016).

To successfully capture the pattern of the direct and mediated contributions of oral narrative textual competence and spelling skills to written narrative textual competence, we consider the results of BLM and monolingual primary school children. As predicted, spelling skills solely contributed to written narrative textual competence in BLM children, whose L2 spelling skills explained the most significant high proportion of the variance in written performance, even absorbing the influence of oral narrative textual skills in the statistical model. These results extend previous evidence about the relationship between oral language skills and sentence generation skills via spelling in the early stages of learning to write for English-speaking children to BLM children acquiring Italian as their L2. In our sample, the results show that the group of younger BLM children exhibit significant difficulties in L2 spelling skills in comparison to monolingual peers. Italian monolingual children, as highlighted in the literature, rapidly master spelling skills and begin to be more proficient in expressing their ideas and transferring their mental models into written texts. In this way, the medium of writing did not impact narrative textual competence, and oral narrative textual competence can easily transfer and influence textual writing production. It is important to consider monolinguals’ proficiency in spelling skills and related lexical knowledge in relation to their constant exposure to the societal language and the several text-hearing opportunities in the societal language since the early years. In addition, it is important to consider that, in primary school years, the teaching of writing is primarily focused on promoting the acquisition of orthographic accuracy in developing writers who are still facing lower-level transcription skills (spelling and handwriting) to progressively free working memory resources in favor of higher-level cognitive processes (Berninger et al., 1992; McCutchen, 1996; Graham and Harris, 2000) and writing strategies (planning, revising, and monitoring) (Arias-Gundín et al., 2021). Instead, BLM children show sustained L2 spelling difficulties. Such difficulties are a strong obstacle for writing texts in L2 to explain the larger proportion of variance and nullify the age effect. The BLM’s spelling difficulties in L2 negatively influenced their written narrative textual competence. When writing texts, children engage in high-level cognitive processes, such as planning, translating, and reviewing, while composing text (Berninger and Swanson, 1994; Hayes, 2012). The lack of automatization of L2 spelling skills in BLM children limits the cognitive resources devoted to high-level processes. Previous results (Vettori et al., 2022) on primary school BLM children show that BLM children fall significantly behind their monolingual peers in textual structure and lexical skills. This result can be linked to limited L2 vocabulary input in BLM children beyond school. It is interesting to consider the detrimental impact of low L2 spelling skills on school-age BLM children’s written narrative skills in relation to the different characteristics of Italian (L2 for BLM) and Chinese (L1 for BLM) languages. Italian is a transparent alphabetic language with complex morphology, whereas Chinese is a non-alphabetic language. BLM children are a heterogeneous group with regard to their L1 and L2 proficiency (Lonigan et al., 2018) and home literacy practices and experiences (Peets et al., 2022). As suggested by Wong (2017), children with L1 different from the societal language may show delayed development of their awareness of the adequate narrative structure qualities specific to the target language (Wong, 2017). Otherwise, they may not have the necessary linguistic resources to convey meaning appropriately (Kang, 2012).

In conclusion, our results confirm the importance of exploring direct and mediated relations between oral narrative textual, spelling skills, and written narrative textual competence (Pinto et al., 2015) and extend previous results focused on the transition between kindergarten to second grade by investigating primary school years. Our results underline the importance of considering how different writing systems pose different challenges for developing writers, especially when considering BLM children who receive limited L2 language input beyond school. Studying how oral and written narrative textual competence relate to each other via the mediating role of spelling skills will yield important implications for educational settings, where knowledge can be employed to design interventions that most effectively sustain L2 written narrative skills in BLM children speaking L1-Chinese at home. The results for monolingual school-age children highlight the need to support oral language, which impacts the quality of children’s narrative writing skills. It is also important to note that adequate mastering of spelling skills in the transparent Italian orthography no longer serves as a good discriminator of writing narrative skills in monolinguals.

Limitations and future research

Our results form an important basis for future longitudinal research to gain a more comprehensive understanding of the relations between BLM and monolingual peers’ oral narrative, spelling, and written narrative skills. In a future study, a more fine-grained analysis accounting for the type of spelling and morphological errors would further inform the challenges posed by the characteristics of the Italian language to BLM children. In this study, the written narrative skills of children have been assessed using narrative text in L2. Future research can explore more in-depth BLM children’s narrative skills in both L1 and L2 to examine these relationships linked to reading and writing habits and motivation (Camacho et al., 2021; De Sixte et al., 2021). Future studies should examine a more heterogeneous language background of BLM children and compare the results from a cross-linguistic perspective. In addition, further research is required to verify the stability of the model detected in this study in BLM children with different language backgrounds.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the University of Florence. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.946142/full#supplementary-material

References

Alarcón, R., del Pilar Jiménez, E., and de Vicente-Yagüe, M. I. (2020). Development and validation of the DIGIGLO, a tool for assessing the digital competence of educators. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 51, 2407–2421. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12919

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arias-Gundín, O., Real, S., Rijlaarsdam, G., and López, P. (2021). Validation of the writing strategies questionnaire in the context of primary education: A multidimensional measurement model. Front. Psychol. 12:700770. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.700770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berninger, V. W., and Swanson, H. L. (1994). “Modifying Hayes and Flower’s model of skilled writing to explain beginning and developing writing,” in Children’s writing: Toward a process theory of the development of skilled writing, ed. E. Butterfield (Greenwich, CT: JAI Press), 57–81.

Google Scholar

Berninger, V., Yates, C., Cartwright, A., Rutberg, J., Remy, E., and Abbott, R. (1992). Lower-level developmental skills in beginning writing. Read. Writ. 4, 257–280. doi: 10.1007/BF01027151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigozzi, L., and Vettori, G. (2016). To tell a story, to write it: Developmental patterns of narrative skills from preschool to first grade. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 31, 461–477. doi: 10.1007/s10212-015-0273-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigozzi, L., Tarchi, C., and Pinto, G. (2016a). Spelling across tasks and levels of language in a transparent orthography. PLoS One 11:e0163033. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigozzi, L., Tarchi, C., Caudek, C., and Pinto, G. (2016b). Predicting reading and spelling disorders: A 4-year prospective cohort study. Front. Psychol. 7:337. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigozzi, L., Tarchi, C., and Pinto, G. (2017). Consistency and stability of Italian children’s spelling in dictation versus composition assessments. Read. Writ. Q. 33, 109–122. doi: 10.1080/10573569.2015.1102111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camacho, A., Alves, R. A., De Smedt, F., Van Keer, H., and Boscolo, P. (2021). Relations among motivation, behaviour, and performance in writing: A multiple-group structural equation modeling study. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 91, 1456–1480. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darlington, R. B., and Hayes, A. F. (2017). Regression analysis and linear models. New York, NY: Guilford, 603–611.

Google Scholar

De Sixte, R., Fajardo, I., Mañá, A., Jáñez, Á, Ramos, M., García-Serrano, M., et al. (2021). Beyond the educational context: Relevance of intrinsic reading motivation during COVID-19 confinement in Spain. Front. Psychol. 12:703251. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.703251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodrich, J. M., Farrington, A. L., and Lonigan, C. J. (2016). Relations between early reading and writing skills among Spanish-speaking language minority children. Read. Writ. 29, 297–319. doi: 10.1007/s11145-015-9594-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gort, M. (2001). On the threshold of biliteracy: Bilingual writing processes of English-dominant and Spanish-dominant first graders in a two-way bilingual education program. Boston, MA: Boston University.

Google Scholar

Graham, S. (2006). “Writing,” in Handbook of educational psychology, eds P. Alexander and P. Winne (Mahwah, NJ: ER), 457–478.

Google Scholar

Graham, S., and Harris, K. R. (2000). The role of self-regulation and transcription skills in writing and writing development. Educ. Psychol. 35, 3–12. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3501_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haman, E., Wodniecka, Z., Marecka, M., Szewczyk, J., Białecka-Pikul, M., Otwinowska, A., et al. (2017). How does L1 and L2 exposure impact L1 performance in bilingual children? Evidence from polish-english migrants to the United Kingdom. Front. Psychol. 8:1444. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, J. R. (2012). Modeling and remodeling writing. Writ. Commun. 29, 369–388. doi: 10.1177/0741088312451260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoff, E. (2018). Bilingual development in children of immigrant families. Child Dev. Perspect. 12, 80–86. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoff, E., Tulloch, M. K., and Core, C. (2021). Profiles of minority-majority language proficiency in 5-year-olds. Child Dev. 92, 1801–1816. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, E. R., Green, J. D., and Arteagoitia, I. (2012). Can yu rid guat ay rot? A developmental investigation of cross-linguistic spelling errors among Spanish–English bilingual students. Biling. Res. J. 35, 164–178. doi: 10.1080/15235882.2012.703637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Incognito, O., Bigozzi, L., Vettori, G., and Pinto, G. (2021). Efficacy of two school-based interventions on notational ability of bilingual preschoolers: A group-randomized trial study. Front. Psychol. 12:686285. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.686285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, J. Y. (2012). Do bilingual children possess better phonological awareness? Investigation of Korean monolingual and Korean-English bilingual children. Read. Writ. 25, 411–431. doi: 10.1007/s11145-010-9277-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. S. G. (2020). Structural relations of language and cognitive skills, and topic knowledge to written composition: A test of the direct and indirect effects model of writing. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 90, 910–932. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. S. G., and Park, S. H. (2019). Unpacking pathways using the direct and indirect effects model of writing (DIEW) and the contributions of higher order cognitive skills to writing. Read. Writ. 32, 1319–1343. doi: 10.1007/s11145-018-9913-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. S. G., and Schatschneider, C. (2017). Expanding the developmental models of writing: A direct and indirect effects model of developmental writing (DIEW). J. Educ. Psychol. 109:35. doi: 10.1037/edu0000129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lonigan, C. J., Goodrich, J. M., and Farver, J. M. (2018). Identifying differences in early literacy skills across subgroups of language-minority children: A latent profile analysis. Dev. Psychol. 54:631. doi: 10.1037/dev0000477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mäkinen, L., Gabbatore, I., Loukusa, S., Kunnari, S., and Schneider, P. (2020). A comparison of picture-based narratives by Finnish, Italian and English-speaking children. Early Educ. Dev. 31, 395–410. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2019.1666446

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 8, 299–325. doi: 10.1007/BF01464076

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MIUR (2012). Indicazioni nazionali per il curricolo della scuola dell’infanzia del primo ciclo di istruzione. (National indications for the curriculum of the nursery school and the first cycle of education). Ministero dell’struzione, Univ. e della Ricerca. Ann. Pubblica Istruzione 2012, 3–88.

Google Scholar

Peets, K. F., Yim, O., and Bialystok, E. (2022). Language proficiency, reading comprehension and home literacy in bilingual children: The impact of context. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 25, 226–240. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2019.1677551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, G., Bigozzi, L., Gamannossi, B. A., and Vezzani, C. (2009). Emergent literacy and learning to write: A predictive model for Italian language. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 24, 61–78. doi: 10.1007/BF03173475

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, G., Bigozzi, L., Gamannossi, B. A., and Vezzani, C. (2012). Emergent literacy and early writing skills. J. Genet. Psychol. 173, 330–354. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2011.609848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, G., Tarchi, C., and Bigozzi, L. (2015). The relationship between oral and written narratives: A three-year longitudinal study of narrative cohesion, coherence, and structure. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 85, 551–569. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, G., Tarchi, C., and Bigozzi, L. (2016). Peer interaction does not always improve children’s mental state talk production in oral narratives. A study in 6-to 10-year-old Italian children. Front. Psychol. 7:1669. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, G., Tarchi, C., and Bigozzi, L. (2020). Improving children’s textual competence in kindergarten through genre awareness. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 35, 137–154. doi: 10.1007/s10212-019-00418-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roch, M., and Hržica, G. (2020). Narrative comprehension by Croatian-Italian bilingual children 5–7 years old. Dev. Narrat. Comprehension Multiling. Assess. Instrum. Narrat. 61:171. doi: 10.1075/sibil.61.06roc

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spinillo, A. G., and Pinto, G. (1994). Children’s narratives under different conditions: A comparative study. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 12, 177–193. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1994.tb00627.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics, 6th Edn. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Google Scholar

UNESCO Institute for Statistics (2012). International standard classification of education: ISCED 2011. Montreal, QC: UNESCO Institute for Statistics.

Google Scholar

Vettori, G., Bigozzi, L., Incognito, O., and Pinto, A. G. (2022). Relationship between oral narrative and vocabulary skills among bilingual language-minority children and their monolingual peers in primary school. Int. J. Biling. doi: 10.1177/13670069221089064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, Y. K. (2017). Relationships between reading comprehension and its components in young Chinese-as-a-second-language learners. Read. Writ. 30, 969–988. doi: 10.1007/s11145-016-9708-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: bilingual language-minority children, textual competence, oral narrative, written narrative, spelling skills, Italian, Chinese

Citation: Vettori G, Bigozzi L, Incognito O and Pinto G (2022) Contribution of oral narrative textual competence and spelling skills to written narrative textual competence in bilingual language-minority children and monolingual peers. Front. Psychol. 13:946142. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.946142

Received: 17 May 2022; Accepted: 28 July 2022;
Published: 23 August 2022.

Edited by:

Pedro García Guirao, WSB Universities, Poland

Reviewed by:

Carol A. Miller, The Pennsylvania State University, United States
Lillyam Gonzalez, University of Ostrava, Czechia

Copyright © 2022 Vettori, Bigozzi, Incognito and Pinto. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giulia Vettori, giulia.vettori@unifi.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.