Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychol., 29 August 2022
Sec. Gender, Sex and Sexualities
This article is part of the Research Topic Sex and Gender Effects on Power, Status, Dominance, and Leadership – An Interdisciplinary Look at Human and Other Mammalian Societies View all 22 articles

Heavier Lies Her Crown: Gendered Patterns of Leader Emotional Labor and Their Downstream Effects

\r\nAndrea C. Vial
Andrea C. Vial*Colleen M. CowgillColleen M. Cowgill
  • Department of Psychology, Division of Science, New York University Abu Dhabi, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates

Women use power in more prosocial ways than men and they also engage in more emotional labor (i.e., self-regulate their emotions to respond and attend to the needs and emotions of other people in a way that advances organizational goals). However, these two constructs have not been previously connected. We propose that gendered emotional labor practices and pressures result in gender differences in the prosocial use of power. We integrate the literature on emotional labor with research on the psychology of power to articulate three routes through which this happens. First, women may be more adept than men at the intrapersonal and interpersonal processes entailed in emotional labor practices—a skill that they can apply at all hierarchical levels. Second, given women’s stronger internal motivation to perform emotional labor, they construe power in a more interdependent manner than men, which promotes a more prosocial use of power. As a result, female powerholders tend to behave in more prosocial ways. Third, when they have power, women encounter stronger external motivation to engage in emotional labor, which effectively constrains powerful women’s behaviors in a way that fosters a more prosocial use of power. We discuss how, by promoting prosocial behavior among powerholders, emotional labor can be beneficial for subordinates and organizations (e.g., increase employee well-being and organizational trust), while simultaneously creating costs for individual powerholders, which may reduce women’s likelihood of actually attaining and retaining power by (a) making high-power roles less appealing, (b) guiding women toward less prestigious and (c) more precarious leadership roles, (d) draining powerful women’s time and resources without equitable rewards, and (e) making it difficult for women to legitimize their power in the eyes of subordinates (especially men). Thus, emotional labor practices can help explain the underrepresentation of women in top leadership positions.

Introduction

When they have power, women tend to behave in more prosocial ways than men. For example, a meta-analysis of 162 studies by Eagly and Johnson (1990) found a stronger tendency in women than in men to lead in an interpersonally oriented style in laboratory experiments. Across studies, women in positions of authority were more likely than men in those positions to prioritize the maintenance of interpersonal relationships, to tend to the morale and welfare of others, or to show consideration for others—e.g., helping and doing favors for subordinates. Subsequent meta-analyses confirmed these early conclusions (van Engen and Willemsen, 2004). Clearly, many female powerholders show no shortage of care for others, even when some scholars have argued that power can unleash self-serving and often destructive behavior that is insensitive to the needs of other people (e.g., Anderson et al., 2003; Keltner et al., 2003; Van Kleef et al., 2008; Lammers et al., 2012; Rucker and Galinsky, 2016). But what makes female powerholders more interpersonally sensitive than their male counterparts? And what are the consequences of women’s tendency to wield power “with a velvet glove” (i.e., in a more prosocial way)?

We posit that the answer to these questions partly lies on gendered patterns of emotional labor—which we define as the act of self-regulating one’s emotions to respond and attend to others’ needs and emotions in a manner that advances organizational goals (Hochschild, 1983; Grandey, 2000; Cheung and Tang, 2010). We argue that women in power behave in a more prosocial manner than men because they have a stronger tendency to practice emotional labor. These gender1 differences have a mixed set of consequences: On the one hand, women’s more prosocial use of power can be beneficial for subordinates and organizations. On the other hand, we argue that gendered emotional labor practices can simultaneously create costs for individual powerholders—especially when emotional labor is externally motivated—and undermine gender equality in top leadership positions in multiple ways. Here, we integrate the previously disconnected literatures on emotional labor and the psychology of power to articulate three routes through which this happens. We contribute to existing models of gender and leadership by identifying emotional labor as a key construct that can illuminate why men and women express power differently and why it is more difficult for women to attain and retain powerful roles (Vial et al., 2016).

Power is often defined as the extent to which an individual exerts asymmetric control or influence over others (Schmid Mast et al., 2009)—for example, having the authority to issue orders that others must follow, or controlling access to valued resources (Magee and Frasier, 2014)—while also being free from others, or having the discretion to operate autonomously, unfettered by the will and needs of other people (Fast et al., 2009; Inesi et al., 2011; Lammers et al., 2016). Ostensibly, then, power runs counter to emotional labor, as the latter prioritizes accommodating the emotions of other people rather than imposing one’s own views independently from others (Rucker and Galinsky, 2016). Conversely, we propose that when people in power engage in emotional labor, this practice may foster a more prosocial use of power, one that is considerate of others and that promotes or protects their welfare (Batson, 2012). We contribute to the literature on the psychology of power (e.g., Sassenberg et al., 2014; Sturm and Antonakis, 2015; Tost and Johnson, 2019; Foulk et al., 2020) by identifying emotional labor practices as an important antecedent to prosocial power use—one that can help explain why men and women in high-power roles may behave differently.

We argue that gender differences in emotional labor practices and demands translate into women’s more prosocial use of power in at least three ways (Figure 1). First, women have a stronger ability than men to practice emotional labor (Figure 1, path a), a skill that underlies the prosocial use of power. Second, women have a stronger internal motivation than men to perform emotional labor (Figure 1, path b), which may lead women to construe power in an interdependent manner that fosters prosocial rather than self-serving behavior. And third, women face stronger external demands than men to practice emotional labor (Figure 1, path c), which constrain powerful women’s ability to exercise their authority in self-serving ways. In the sections that follow, we integrate the literatures on emotional labor and the psychology of power to develop a theoretical framework in which we articulate these three pathways connecting emotional labor to female powerholders’ prosocial use of power (Figure 1, path g) as well as positive and negative consequences for individuals, groups, and gender equality at large (Figure 1, paths j-l). We begin by discussing gender differences in emotional labor and then review research that supports the claim that women are more likely than men to behave in a prosocial way when they occupy high-power roles.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Theoretical model connecting gendered patterns of emotional labor with the prosocial use of power and its downstream consequences. Powerholder gender is related to differences in the ability (path a) and the internal motivation (path b) to practice emotional labor, as well as in external emotional labor demands (path c). These three factors directly contribute to emotional labor (paths d–f), which underlies the prosocial use of power (path g). Additionally, a stronger internal motivation to practice emotional labor is associated with a more interdependent construal of power (path h), which influences the tendency to enact power in prosocial ways (path i). The prosocial use of power has benefits for subordinates and organizations (path j), but it can also create costs for individual powerholders (path k) and undermine gender equality in leadership roles (path l).

What Is Emotional Labor and What Are Its Antecedents?

A variety of social settings have tacit (and sometimes explicit) norms or “display rules” for what is an acceptable or desirable emotional expression. These norms delineate socially appropriate ways of interacting with others, including whether and to what degree felt emotions should be expressed (Ekman and Friesen, 1975; Matsumoto, 1990). At its core, emotional labor is the regulation of felt and expressed emotions (whether effortful or not) to match these emotional display rules with the objective of fulfilling organizational goals (Hochschild, 1983; Grandey, 2000; Cheung and Tang, 2010). People practice emotion regulation in a wide variety of contexts (e.g., students at school, spouses at home; Gross, 1998); however, emotional labor specifically occurs when people deploy emotion regulation strategies to meet organizational goals (Grandey, 2000). For example, when an employee in a service job continues to smile to an angry customer to prevent them from leaving the establishment, or when an employee masks their feelings of frustration during a long staff meeting to preserve harmony within the team. Indeed, emotional labor has tangible organizational benefits, as it fosters an atmosphere in which people feel at ease, valued, and understood (Iszatt-White, 2009).

Emotional labor is enacted on two different levels. One of these levels is strictly intrapersonal and involves self-regulation and expression of the right kind and amount of emotion (Totterdell and Holman, 2003; Hülsheger and Schewe, 2011). To achieve this, individuals resort to one of two strategies: (a) a response-focused emotion regulation strategy known as “surface acting,” which entails the suppression of felt negative emotions through the modification of facial displays (e.g., “putting on a smiley face”); and (b) an antecedent-focused emotion regulation strategy known as “deep acting” that involves changing inner feelings through cognitive reappraisal—for example focusing on positive rather than negative feelings (Hochschild, 1983; Grandey, 2000, 2003; Cheung and Tang, 2010). In addition to these intrapersonal processes, emotional labor is enacted on an interpersonal level. In order to respond adequately to the emotions of another person, one must first identify how that person is feeling (Ashtar et al., 2021). Thus, although an important part of the process is self-oriented, emotional labor is inherently other-oriented as it requires one to be attuned to others’ emotions and to accommodate and cater to those emotions (i.e., paying attention to, accurately recognizing, and responding effectively to the feelings of others). Interpersonal and intrapersonal processes can take place sequentially and repeat several times in the course of a single interaction (e.g., identify another person’s feelings; self-regulate one’s own emotions; produce the appropriate response; identify the person’s reaction; recalibrate or maintain one’s emotional expression, and so on).

When individuals practice emotional labor (e.g., by being attentive to the emotional experience of others and by self-regulating to respond to those emotions in a way that facilitates organizational goals), such practices can manifest in an interdependent and relational approach and in a wide variety of behaviors. These behaviors, which essentially grease the wheels of human interaction in organizational settings, can be classified into three broad categories, including (a) acting in prosocial rather than self-serving ways (e.g., being interpersonally helpful; showing concern for others’ welfare at work; guiding, comforting, and defending others); (b) being sensitive to others’ views (e.g., seeking out their opinion; allowing them to voice opposing perspectives; accommodating others’ needs); and (c) maintaining a positive emotional environment (e.g., making others feel at ease and valued; tending to their morale; providing them with emotional support).2

Antecedents of Emotional Labor: Ability and Motivation

When considering the psychological antecedents of emotional labor, it is important to distinguish between the ability to practice it (Figure 1, path a) and the motivation to engage in it (Figure 1, path b and path c). One may be skilled at practicing emotional labor but not be particularly motivated to do so—either generally or in a specific context. Alternatively, one may desire to engage in emotional labor and fail miserably. This distinction is key to understanding how emotional labor practices relate to gender, as research suggests that women may be more skilled at behaviors relevant to emotional labor than men as well as more strongly motivated to practice them. Similarly, it is crucial to distinguish between emotional labor that springs from internal motivation (Figure 1, path b) and emotional labor that is externally motivated (Figure 1, path c): Whether one genuinely wishes to cater to others’ emotions (for instance, out of an empathic concern for others; Batson, 1987, 2011), or whether one feels an external demand to do so (e.g., due to formal work-role requirements; Hochschild, 1983; Grandey, 2000; Cheung and Tang, 2010).

These distinctions between ability, internal, and external motivation form the basis of three different routes in our theoretical model through which gender differences in emotional labor result in women’s stronger prosocial use of power. According to social role theory (Wood and Eagly, 2002; Eagly and Wood, 2012), gender differences and similarities in social behavior are the product of the disparate distribution of women and men into distinct social roles. For instance, women occupy the great majority of care-oriented roles in society. Such gendered distribution of labor, which is due in part to evolved physical differences between women and men (e.g., in terms of size, strength, reproductive activities), influences their behavior through various processes, including the creation of gender roles. These gender roles dictate different expectations for women and men—for example, the kinds of psychological characteristics they are believed to possess. Given that women tend to occupy care-oriented roles, they are expected to possess the psychological characteristics that are best suited to those roles, such as being highly communal, kind, cooperative, etc. These expectations, as well as the requirements of the specific roles that women and men occupy, shape their social behavior (see Eagly et al., 2000) via hormonal processes, socialization practices (i.e., how women and men are taught to behave from a young age), and social regulation (e.g., penalties and rewards for behaving in ways that contradict or uphold expectations, respectively). As we discuss below, gender differences have been identified in the ability, internal motivation, and external demands to practice emotional labor, which reflect the influence of gendered social roles (Wood and Eagly, 2002; Eagly and Wood, 2012).

Gender Differences in Ability to Practice Emotional Labor

Some evidence suggests that women may be more adept than men at the intrapersonal processes entailed in emotional labor practices, as they usually engage in a wider range of emotion regulation strategies than men (Garnefski et al., 2004). For instance, fMRI studies indicate that they use positive emotions in the service of reappraising negative emotions to a greater degree (McRae et al., 2008). With regards to the more interpersonal aspects of emotional labor, studies indicate that women possess better empathic accuracy than men—the ability to correctly infer what another person is thinking or feeling (Ickes et al., 2000). Women are also more successful than men at deciphering others’ non-verbal cues (La France et al., 2009; Williams et al., 2009), and at recalling people’s non-verbal cues and facial expressions (Hall et al., 2007). Other research has revealed that women score higher than men on interpersonal aspects of emotional intelligence, which involves the ability to perceive and express emotion and regulate emotion in the self and others (Mayer et al., 1999), including social skills such as being perceptive, empathic, and adaptable (Argyle, 1994; Petrides and Furnham, 2000; Joseph and Newman, 2010; Cabello et al., 2016).

In sum, empirical evidence indicates that women may be more skilled than men at a host of behaviors that constitute the building blocks of emotional labor. In line with social role theory (Wood and Eagly, 2002; Eagly and Wood, 2012), these ability differences may reflect women’s historical tendency to occupy positions in which emotional skills are paramount (e.g., care-oriented roles). As care-oriented roles promote and require emotional abilities, such abilities may become entrenched into the female gender role in a way that shapes women’s behavior (Eagly et al., 2000). For instance, the socialization of young girls may place a stronger emphasis than that of boys on the development of emotional skills such as being able to identify and name emotions (McClure, 2000; Brody and Hall, 2010).

However, as alluded to earlier, an ability to practice emotional labor successfully should not be confused with a motivation to do so. This distinction becomes particularly important when considering emotional labor practices that are externally motivated (as discussed below), because ability and motivation in this case may have opposite effects on well-being. Specifically, whereas being more adept at managing emotions could make emotional labor feel effortless, a strong external pressure to do so may take a psychological toll, reducing emotional autonomy and feelings of authenticity (Brotheridge and Grandey, 2002). We discuss these burdens in detail in the section on The Downstream Consequences of Women’s Prosocial Use of Power.

Gender Differences in Internal Motivation to Practice Emotional Labor

Both internal (i.e., intrinsic) and external (i.e., extrinsic) motivation to practice emotional labor to achieve organizational goals may be stronger in women than in men. With regards to internal motivation, women may genuinely have a stronger proclivity than men to both consider and accommodate the emotions of others. For example, women appear to care more than men about correctly reading and responding to others’ emotions (Ickes et al., 2000; Klein and Hodges, 2001). Women are also generally more likely than men to engage in the kinds of non-verbal behaviors that facilitate social interaction (for a review, see LaFrance and Vial, 2016), such as smiling (LaFrance et al., 2003), maintaining eye contact (Hall, 1984); keeping closer physical proximity (Hall and Gunnery, 2013); orienting their body more directly toward their interaction partners (Helweg-Larsen et al., 2004); employing affiliative speech, or language that affirms and shows support to the other person (Leaper and Ayres, 2007); and using back-channel responses (e.g., head nodding) to signal that they are listening (Leaper and Robnett, 2011). Other research suggests that women (but not men) may generally internalize prosocial rather than self-interested behavior as their intuitive response to other people (Rand et al., 2016).

As discussed in the previous sections when describing the basic tenets of social role theory (Wood and Eagly, 2002; Eagly and Wood, 2012), although the origins of these gender differences are likely multiply determined, one clear source can be found in different socialization practices that, from a young age, encourage girls more so than boys to cultivate communal attributes (Brody, 1993; Hibbard and Buhrmester, 1998; Shields, 2002; Chaplin et al., 2005). Women consistently report a stronger communal self-concept than men, viewing themselves as friendlier, less selfish, and more concerned with others (e.g., Witt and Wood, 2010; Hsu et al., 2021). Thus, the evidence indicates that women are more highly internally motivated than men to practice emotional labor.

Gender Differences in External Emotional Labor Demands

In addition to having stronger internal motivation, women may also experience stronger external pressures than men to get emotional labor right. Whereas some individuals may be more adept at emotional labor than others and/or personally more inclined to practice it, organizational norms often impinge on people’s ability to express their emotions freely. Indeed, one strong antecedent to emotional labor is the extent to which there are contextual pressures that create a sense of obligation to practice it, or emotional labor demands. Organizational contexts and roles vary in how much emotional labor they require. Women are generally more likely than men to hold jobs entailing high work-role demands to display positive emotions either to customers and clients or to coworkers and superiors (Guy and Newman, 2004; Cortes and Pan, 2018). In many female-dominated (i.e., “pink collar”) occupations, workers are expected to employ emotional skills to bring about organizational ends, whereas workers in male-dominated occupations do not face these demands (Meier et al., 2006; Johnson and Spector, 2007; Nixon, 2009). Indeed, the concept of emotional labor was originally developed by Hochschild (1983) to describe the experience of low-level service jobs (e.g., flight attendants, customer-oriented roles), which do not offer much opportunity for advancement up the organizational hierarchy, and which continue to be occupied primarily by women (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2021).

Importantly, not only are women more likely than men to work in occupations with strong emotional labor demands; they are also more likely than men to encounter pressures to practice emotional labor even when occupying the same organizational roles (e.g., Schaubroeck and Jones, 2000). From the perspective of social role theory (Wood and Eagly, 2002; Eagly and Wood, 2012), cultural gender stereotypes develop from observation of the historical distribution of women and men into different social roles, leading people to expect and require women to accommodate others’ needs and emotions to a greater extent than men. Indeed, there is a strong belief that women, more so than men, tend to care about and be sensitive to the feelings of other people (Prentice and Carranza, 2002; White and Gardner, 2009; Haines et al., 2016). Stereotypes around emotion expression in particular portray women as well-suited to comply with emotional display rules that require gauging and responding to others’ emotions (Lopez-Zafra and Gartzia, 2014) and expressing positivity and interpersonal sensitivity (Shields, 2002; Timmers et al., 2003; Fischbach et al., 2015). People tend to implicitly associate the expression of anger with men (Bijlstra et al., 2010; Neel et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2015); indeed, men are commonly stereotyped as aggressive or violent (Leach et al., 2017) and as more likely than women to display negative emotions such as anger and hostility (Plant et al., 2000; Shields, 2000)—behaviors that are incompatible with emotional labor practices.

Gender stereotypes have a strong prescriptive component (Burgess and Borgida, 1999; Prentice and Carranza, 2002). Thus, people typically think that women should be caring, kind, and careful with others’ emotions. They do not require the same of men, who are held to a lower communality standard in general (Biernat and Manis, 1994; see also Vial and Cimpian, 2020, for a review of shifting gender standards and social rewards). Unsurprisingly, then, women are judged in relation to a higher standard than men with respect to performing emotional labor at work. For example, an experiment showed that women do not reap any special rewards for being interpersonally helpful with coworkers, whereas men receive accolades for the same behavior (Heilman and Chen, 2005; see also Farrell and Finkelstein, 2007). Women are expected to show positive emotions in general more than men (Hess et al., 2005) and their emotional expression at work is scrutinized more closely (Smith et al., 2016). In contrast, men’s emotional expression is judged based on a relaxed standard: Whereas women elicit penalties from other people when they express anger in a professional context and their anger is viewed as unjustified, men’s anger in the same context is seen as acceptable and warranted (Brescoll and Uhlmann, 2008; Barrett and Bliss-Moreau, 2009; see also Raymondie and Steiner, 2021). In sum, even in the same organizational role, women encounter stronger external pressures than men to practice emotional labor, and are punished when they do not heed them—even when they occupy high-power roles (as we elaborate on the section on The Pressure Route: Emotional Labor Demands Curb Women’s Self-Interested Use of Power).

Emotional Labor at the Top of the Hierarchy: Women’s Prosocial Use of Power

Individuals practice emotional labor at all levels of the organizational hierarchy. Whereas, in its origins, the concept of emotional labor was focused on workers at lower hierarchical levels (Hochschild, 1983), those at the top of the hierarchy also practice emotional labor—identifying others’ emotions and self-regulating their own in order to produce the kind of response that may best achieve their organizational goals. Indeed, emotional labor can be an important part of leadership (e.g., Gardner et al., 2009; Humphrey, 2012). This may be particularly the case for management roles embedded in occupational contexts that have a strong care orientation (e.g., healthcare, early education) in which communal attributes and the capacity to nurture others are viewed as more typical in leaders (Yoder, 2001; Cowgill and Vial, 2022). Unsurprisingly, women are better represented in leadership positions in these organizational contexts compared to non-care-oriented occupations (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2021).

Emotional Labor at the Top Translates Into Prosocial Use of Power

Given the unique behavioral affordances of high-level roles (e.g., the prerogative to impose authority over others; Magee and Frasier, 2014), we argue that emotional labor in these roles translates into a more prosocial use of power. By “prosocial use of power,” we mean a broad range of actions intended to benefit people in the organizational context other than the powerholder (i.e., behaviors such as helping, comforting, sharing, cooperation, etc.; Batson, 2012), which are supported by emotional labor practices that allow for the accurate detection and accommodation of others’ emotional needs. Prosocial attitudes and behaviors are generally valued as key features of effective leaders (e.g., Gerzema and D’Antonio, 2013, 2017; Gartzia and van Knippenberg, 2016). Indeed, promoting cooperative relationships with and among followers is often highlighted as an important leader function (De Cremer and van Knippenberg, 2002).

At the top of the hierarchy, emotional labor practices may translate into a leadership style that is more interpersonally oriented, one that draws less on dominance to influence others and, as a result, increases positive interpersonal behaviors among subordinates (Humphrey, 2012; Kakkar and Sivanathan, 2021). Emotional labor practices among managers and supervisors manifest in prosocial behaviors toward employees (e.g., helping and doing favors for subordinates), showing sensitivity to their views (e.g., not dominating a team interaction; listening and taking subordinates’ concerns into account when making decisions), and seeking to foster a positive, friendly work environment that is psychologically safe (e.g., avoid expressing anger or being too critical or too dominant; showing empathy; promoting cooperative relationships with and among followers). At the same time, those with decision-making power are often expected to self-regulate in order to maintain emotions at bay and keep a cool head to make decisions rationally—for example, to suppress feelings of empathy evoked by a specific individual in order to maximize aggregate outcomes for the group or organization they lead (e.g., Uhlmann et al., 2013). Thus, powerholders’ effective emotional labor practices entail walking a fine line between showing sensitivity and empathy and being accommodating, while at the same time not letting emotions cloud their judgment. These actions require those in high-power roles to carefully read others’ emotions and manage their own—often involving substantial self-regulation—in the service of effective communication and producing the right state of mind in others (i.e., emotional labor; Humphrey, 2012). All of these practices appear to be more common among high-power women compared to high-power men, as we describe next.

Women Wield Power in More Prosocial Ways Than Men

Whether due to a stronger ability (e.g., Garnefski et al., 2004), internal motivation (e.g., Ickes et al., 2000), or external demand (e.g., Heilman and Chen, 2005), we argue that women’s greater likelihood to practice emotional labor results in a more prosocial use of power when they wield it compared to men (Figure 1, path g). Powerholders who engage in emotional labor practices are often described as transformational leaders (Wolfram and Mohr, 2010; Vinkenburg et al., 2011) or as servant leaders (Barbuto and Gifford, 2010; Lemoine and Blum, 2021), who enact a communally oriented leadership style in which individual consideration (“seeing” and nurturing followers) is key. Research has consistently found that women are more likely than men to adopt these kinds of interpersonally oriented leadership styles (Eagly and Johnson, 1990; Eagly and Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). For example, an early study revealed that women showed more concern for others than men even when they occupied high-status organizational roles (Moskowitz et al., 1994). Meta-analytic evidence indicates that women in positions of authority are more likely than men in those positions to prioritize the maintenance of interpersonal relationships, to tend to the morale and welfare of others, or to show consideration for others—e.g., helping and doing favors for subordinates (van Engen and Willemsen, 2004). Other work suggests that women (but not men) in high-power roles are sensitive to other people’s views and perspectives, and less likely to dominate team interactions (Brescoll, 2011). Further supporting these trends, a study commissioned by LeanIn.org and McKinsey and Company (Thomas et al., 2021), which included responses from over 65,000 employees in 423 companies in the United States and Canada, revealed that female managers were more likely than male managers to provide emotional support to employees and to help them navigate work-life challenges. In the context of academia, surveys have found that female faculty perform significantly more uncompensated internal service than male faculty, acquiescing to participate in committee-work more often, even when controlling for rank (i.e., tenure; Guarino and Borden, 2017).

Thus, the existing evidence indicates that women behave in more prosocial ways than men when they have power. Nevertheless, it would be helpful for future investigations to examine this possibility more directly as well as the connection with emotional labor practices. Studies may test whether gender differences in emotion self-regulation among male and female powerholders predicts the latter’s stronger tendency to behave prosocially. Emotion regulation takes time and effortful control (Grandey, 2000); thus, studies could examine whether gender differences in powerholders’ prosocial behavior are eliminated in conditions that might impair emotional labor (e.g., under time constraints or cognitive load). Given that women are more skilled at emotional labor than men (Ickes et al., 2000; Garnefski et al., 2004; McRae et al., 2008; Cabello et al., 2016), future studies could also examine whether female powerholders respond better than male powerholders to emotional labor demands.

Multiple Routes to Gender Differences in the Prosocial Use of Power

We propose that gender differences in the ability (e.g., Garnefski et al., 2004), internal motivation (e.g., Ickes et al., 2000), and external demands (e.g., Heilman and Chen, 2005) to practice emotional labor constitute three distinct pathways or routes through which gendered emotional labor practices and demands result in gender differences in the prosocial use of power. Of these three routes, the “ability” route (Figure 1, paths a, d, and g) is the most straightforward, as we describe below. We also propose that there are two other routes connecting gendered emotional labor practices with the prosocial use of power, which are less obvious but equally (or perhaps even more) influential: a “construal” route (Figure 1, paths b, h, and i) and a “pressure” route (Figure 1, paths c, f, and g).

The Ability Route: Women’s Greater Aptitude for Emotional Labor Facilitates the Prosocial Use of Power

The ability route focuses on gender differences in the ability to practice emotional labor (Figure 1, path a), as previously discussed (Ickes et al., 2000; Garnefski et al., 2004; McRae et al., 2008; Cabello et al., 2016). Such differences in ability may logically underlie gender differences in actual emotional labor (Figure 1, path d). Specifically, women’s greater aptitude for the skills involved in emotional labor relative to men would enable them to practice it, thereby directly influencing women’s prosocial use of power (Figure 1, path g). Women’s superior ability to accurately understand what others are feeling (Ickes et al., 2000) and read their non-verbal expressions (La France et al., 2009; Williams et al., 2009) would make it easier for them to subsequently accommodate their needs (e.g., to display the kind of individualized consideration that is central to transformational leadership styles; Eagly and Johnson, 1990; Eagly and Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). Supporting this view, studies have found that the ability to perceive and respond to others’ emotions is positively related to behaving altruistically toward others (Charbonneau and Nicol, 2002). Thus, women may use power more benevolently than men simply because they are better equipped to practice emotional labor.

The Construal Route: Women’s Greater Internal Motivation to Practice Emotional Labor Translates Into a More Benevolent View (and Use) of Power

In addition to directly promoting prosocial behavior by increasing the amount of emotional labor practiced (Figure 1, paths b, e and g), we propose that women’s stronger internal motivation than men to perform emotional labor may foster prosocial behavior indirectly through a “construal” route: Due to their internal motivation to practice emotional labor (Figure 1, path b), women construe power in a more interdependent way than men (Figure 1, path h), which fosters a prosocial use of power (Figure 1, path i). Beyond the objective degree of power that a person may have (i.e., how much a person is actually in control of their own and others’ fate), how a person construes their power is fundamental to understanding how they wield it (Sassenberg et al., 2014; Sturm and Antonakis, 2015; Tost and Johnson, 2019; Foulk et al., 2020). Specifically, power appears to magnify preexisting individual dispositions to be more self-oriented or, conversely, more communally oriented (e.g., Côté et al., 2011; Galinsky et al., 2016). Those with power are highly attuned to features in their environment that can help them achieve their goals (Keltner et al., 2003; Guinote, 2007, 2008), including interpersonal and prosocial goals. When people feel powerful, they are better able to connect with and enact their true selves (Kraus et al., 2011; Kifer et al., 2013). Some may approach power in a more “personalized” way that highlights autonomy and dominance over others, whereas some may approach power in a more “socialized” manner that highlights the powerholder’s responsibility to ensure the best possible outcomes for the group at large (Frieze and Boneva, 2001; see also Wang and Sun, 2016).

Prosocial effects ensue when powerholders construe their power in interdependent ways that highlight responsibility for the welfare of others (Overbeck and Park, 2006; Gordon and Chen, 2013; De Wit et al., 2017). When people hold a more interdependent self-construal, they use power benevolently (Howard et al., 2007), and when they have a stronger other-orientation they tend to be fairer in their dealings with others (Blader and Chen, 2012). Powerholders with a stronger need to belong or be accepted tend to downplay their power and yield to the opinions and views of other people (Rios et al., 2015). Other work has shown that power can sometimes increase perspective-taking (Hall et al., 2005, 2007; Schmid Mast et al., 2009). The more the powerholder understands the high-power position as empathic and other-oriented, the more he or she is interpersonally sensitive (Chen et al., 2001; Schmid Mast et al., 2009; Côté et al., 2011; Chin et al., 2013). If the psychological experience of power leads powerholders to behave more in line with their other-oriented or self-oriented dispositions (e.g., Kraus et al., 2011; Foulk et al., 2020), then it is possible that baseline gender differences in the internal motivation to perform emotional labor may persist even when men and women occupy positions of power, and women’s propensity to act on such motivation may be enhanced. Indeed, recent investigations provide indirect evidence in line with the idea that women may think of people at the top of the hierarchy as being particularly adept at managing others’ emotions—that they may view emotional labor as central to power and leadership (e.g., Vecchio and Boatwright, 2002; Bellou, 2011; Hays, 2013; Collins et al., 2014; Gino et al., 2015).

First, given that power facilitates goal pursuit (Keltner et al., 2003; Guinote, 2007, 2008), women’s stronger communal goals (Diekman et al., 2011) and internal motivation to practice emotional labor may translate into a more prosocial use of power in alignment with those goals, whereas men’s more agentic goals may result in strong self-oriented behavior. Due to their strong emphasis on connection, interpersonal sensitivity, and the overall tendency to see oneself in a relational manner (Josephs et al., 1992; Cross and Madson, 1997; Witt and Wood, 2010; Hsu et al., 2021), women who acquire power may be overall more attuned to the needs and emotions of others, and willing and emboldened to cater to them. Compared to women, men in power may be more content with the possession of the means to dominate or impose their will onto others (i.e., “being feared”; Hays, 2013), as they generally have a more independent self-construal (Guimond et al., 2006) and tend to self-describe as more dominant and assertive (Prentice and Carranza, 2002; Hentschel et al., 2019). For example, in a series of studies, Gino et al. (2015) found that men were more likely than women to desire a highly dominant type of power, “to have an impact on, control or manage other people, influence other people, or control resources others depend on” (Gino et al., 2015).

Second, women appear to have stronger expectations than men for emotional labor in authority figures, which may mirror their differential approach to wielding power when they have it themselves. To illustrate, a meta-analysis of 69 studies drawing from three different research paradigms testing gender stereotypical perceptions of leaders and authority figures revealed that men tend to construe leadership as more agentic and less communal than women (Koenig et al., 2011). Women more than men tend to view arrogance or being controlling as undesirable characteristics of those in powerful roles, and instead value communal, positive emotional attributes in leaders (Vial and Napier, 2018). Other research has similarly revealed that, compared to men, women expect leaders to be more “people-oriented” (Bellou, 2011) and more relational (Boatwright and Forrest, 2000), and react more positively to leaders who behave more communally (Collins et al., 2014) and who show considerateness toward others (Vecchio and Boatwright, 2002). Thus, women more than men appear to envision the ideal powerful person as someone who is able to relate in a positive emotional manner to other people and to accommodate their feelings and interests; namely, someone who performs emotional labor. This vision may influence how women themselves wield power, leading to more prosocial behavior in female powerholders than in male powerholders.

The Pressure Route: Emotional Labor Demands Curb Women’s Self-Interested Use of Power

Finally, the third and last route is a “pressure” route such that, to some extent, observed gender differences in the prosocial use of power reflect subtly coercive emotional labor demands and looming social threats that impinge on women more strongly than on men (Figure 1, path c). Specifically, we propose that, although attaining structural power could free individuals to behave in more self-serving ways (Kipnis, 1972; Keltner et al., 2003; Van Kleef et al., 2008), the stronger emotional labor demands imposed on women compared to men (e.g., Heilman and Chen, 2005; Brescoll and Uhlmann, 2008; Barrett and Bliss-Moreau, 2009) do not cease as they accrue power. These demands may effectively constrain powerful women’s (but not men’s) ability to exercise their authority in self-serving ways, resulting in more prosocial power use.

Women in top roles are often expected to be more emotionally available and more sensitive to others than men in similar roles. For example, Schaubroeck and Jones (2000) showed that, within the same large organization, women perceived a stronger requirement to express positive emotions relative to men, even when position tenure and salary level were kept constant (see also Bellas, 1999). In another study, participants expected female leaders to be particularly more effective than male leaders at “caretaking” leader behaviors such as encouraging, assisting, praising, mentoring, and providing resources to others (Prime et al., 2009). Comparable expectations of caretaking and nurturing behaviors have been documented in the realm of academia, where female professors are subject to stronger emotional labor demands from students than male professors (e.g., to do special favors; El-Alayli et al., 2018). Women more than men are expected to adopt a communally oriented style of leadership focused on nurturing followers that involves listening, showing empathy, and providing emotional support to subordinates, commonly known as “servant leadership” (Barbuto and Gifford, 2010; Lemoine and Blum, 2021). Similarly, people expect women more than men to lead in a “transformational” way (Embry et al., 2008; Vinkenburg et al., 2011; Stempel et al., 2015), a leadership style that includes a strong component of consideration and support for subordinates’ needs, preferences, and welfare, and the creation of a friendly work environment that is psychologically safe (Rafferty and Griffin, 2004). At a basic cognitive level, research indicates that people expect feminine-faced leaders to be cooperative and display a prosocial leadership style based on altruism, empathy, and reciprocity, whereas they expect masculine-faced leaders to display a dominant style (Spisak et al., 2012).

Expectations that female leaders perform more emotional labor translate into an unspoken requirement that they should do so: In order to be seen as effective leaders, women (but not men) must be interpersonally sensitive—sympathetic, compassionate, understanding, forgiving, helpful. These demands to wield power “with a velvet glove” become sharply apparent in the backlash (i.e., social and economic penalties; Rudman, 1998) that high-power women encounter when they do not accommodate or spare others’ feelings. A plethora of studies following role congruity theory (Eagly and Karau, 2002) have demonstrated that female leaders are evaluated negatively when they enact their role in dominant ways—for instance, when they discipline or give negative feedback to subordinates (Sinclair and Kunda, 2000; Atwater et al., 2001; Brett et al., 2005) or when they demand a behavior change in others (Williams and Tiedens, 2016). These dominant behaviors, which are antithetical to emotional labor, lead to a perceived “communality deficit” in female leaders (Heilman and Okimoto, 2007; see also Ma et al., 2022), causing them to be seen as cold and interpersonally hostile (Heilman and Okimoto, 2007). Women (but not men) who lead with a directive style are more likely to receive negative evaluations than women who lead with a democratic style (Eagly et al., 1992), and abusive leadership tends to be penalized more harshly in female than in male leaders (Kim et al., 2021). When emotional labor is not readily apparent in leaders, those leaders fare worse if they are women.

The demands placed on female leaders focus strongly on the intrapersonal emotional labor aspect of deamplifying emotion—taming the expression of intense emotions (Matsumoto et al., 2005; Moran et al., 2013). Although research indicates that women and men report feeling most emotions to the same degree (Barrett et al., 1998; Else-Quest et al., 2012), women are stereotyped as too emotional and overly sensitive (Fischer, 1993; Shields, 2013; Dolan, 2014), and therefore unable to keep a cool head to make decisions rationally (e.g., Citrin and Roberts, 2004; see Brescoll, 2016, for a review). Thus, the emotional makeup of women is viewed as incompatible with some of the intrapersonal emotional labor requirements of high-level positions (Fischbach et al., 2015), leading to close scrutiny of female powerholders’ emotional expression. For example, women in top positions elicit more negative evaluations than men in similar roles for expressing anger (Lewis, 2000; Timmers et al., 2003; Brescoll and Uhlmann, 2008), a highly dominant emotion that is typically off limits for low-power individuals (Plant et al., 2000; Tiedens et al., 2000; Petkanopoulou et al., 2019) as well as powerful women (but tends to be condoned in high-power men). But the demand on powerful women to deamplify emotion for the benefit of others does not only target negative emotions, but all emotions more generally (for reviews, see Brescoll, 2016; Smith et al., 2016). As a result, women in high places walk a fine line, risking backlash from others if they fail to get emotional labor “just right.”

Research further suggests that performing emotional labor may allow women to successfully ascend the organizational hierarchy, eschewing the negative reactions they often encounter when they behave in explicitly dominant ways (Williams and Tiedens, 2016). For instance, some studies indicate that women reap more benefits than men from enacting transformational leadership practices, such that the teams they lead perform better (Chen and Shao, 2022). Men are held to a lower standard in this regard, as evidenced by research showing that men who practice transformational leadership tend to be evaluated as more promotable than women (Hentschel et al., 2018). These findings highlight the persistence of an impression management conundrum for women in top positions (Phelan and Rudman, 2010), and suggest the possibility that emotional labor practices may help women navigate these hurdles. Indeed, to lead and influence others without seeming overly domineering, women in powerful roles usually tame the way they express their power (e.g., Amanatullah and Morris, 2010; Moss-Racusin and Rudman, 2010; Brescoll, 2011; Amanatullah and Tinsley, 2013). Practicing emotional labor as an impression management strategy may result in a more prosocial use of power overall.

The Downstream Consequences of Women’s Prosocial Use of Power

Women’s higher likelihood than men of engaging in prosocial behaviors when they occupy high-power roles has important consequences on many levels: for individual women, for subordinates who report to female authorities, and for organizations and society as a whole. The positive effects of women’s more prosocial use of power tend to benefit other people: Those who report to or work directly for them, as well as the organizations or groups in which women’s power is embedded. Importantly, a focus on the emotional labor practices that underlie prosocial behavior sheds light on the potential negative effects of women’s prosocial use of power, which burden individual women. We discuss these positive and negative downstream consequences first; then, we articulate how powerful women’s emotional labor practices may contribute to gender inequality in organizational hierarchies.

The Positive Effects of Powerful Women’s Prosocial Behavior

Women’s more prosocial use of power is likely to confer many benefits for subordinates and organizations (Figure 1, path j). When those in powerful positions are interpersonally sensitive, subordinates directly reap the benefits—for example by being able to influence the decision-making process (De Wit et al., 2017). Organizations as a whole may benefit as well, as emotional labor is central to some of the key aspects of transformational leadership, like individualized consideration (Bass et al., 2003; Byrne et al., 2014), and it could foster a more socially responsible use of power (e.g., Chen et al., 2001). Similarly, in the context of political power and leadership, research has found that politicians’ tendency to practice emotional labor (for example, by employing courteous speech and avoiding incivility in debates) can be highly beneficial, reducing political polarization and increasing trust in politicians (Skytte, 2021).

For these reasons, women’s higher tendency than men to behave prosocially when they occupy high-power roles may confer important advantages on the people they lead and the organizations in which their power is embedded. Indeed, gender differences in leadership effectiveness tend to favor women over men (Eagly and Carli, 2003; Eagly et al., 2014; Offermann and Foley, 2020). Employee well-being tends to be higher in companies with more women in top positions (Thomas et al., 2021), and teams led by women tend to report more cohesion and cooperation (Post, 2015). Other research suggests that firms with more women in high-power roles are less likely to face discrimination lawsuits (Abebe and Dadanlar, 2021), have better financial performance (Glass and Cook, 2018; Hoobler et al., 2018), and engage in more socially responsible practices (e.g., Glass et al., 2016). In the political realm, a higher proportion of women in parliaments is associated with lower levels of corruption at the country level (Dollar et al., 2001; Swamy et al., 2001; Rivas, 2013). Although some of these findings may rely on observational data, raising the possibility of reverse causality, they converge with experimental studies that suggest a causal relationship. To illustrate, in a series of experiments, the mere presence of a female leader relative to a male leader caused people to anticipate fairer treatment within an organization and better personal outcomes because they associated stronger communal values in the organization when women occupied leadership roles (Joshi and Diekman, 2021). Similarly, when a hypothetical organization was in crisis, participants in two experiments were more likely to trust the organization (e.g., be willing to invest in it) when it was led by women than by men because they expected women to be more skilled at interpersonal emotion management (Post et al., 2019).

We argue that these advantages and benefits may stem from female leaders’ greater tendency to use their power in prosocial ways, and that male leaders (and the organizations that they lead) would also generally benefit from practicing more emotional labor. Regardless of their gender, powerholders who practice emotional labor can foster an environment in which employees and subordinates feel supported, are happier, and perform better (Thomas et al., 2021). Thus, although our focus in this article has been on the high standard for emotional labor against which female leaders are judged compared to male leaders (which is arguably unfair), perhaps a greater focus should be placed on identifying ways of increasing emotional labor among male leaders. Indeed, recent research indicates that both male and female leaders can enhance their effectiveness by enacting more communal behaviors that foster cooperation and trust (e.g., Gartzia and van Knippenberg, 2016; Hentschel et al., 2018; Gartzia and Baniandrés, 2019; Post et al., 2019). As more women attain high-power roles, their tendency to practice emotional labor might promote a shift in people’s implicit notions of leadership toward valuing communality as a central rather than peripheral aspect (Vial and Napier, 2018), one equally expected and rewarded in all leaders regardless of their gender.

The Negative Effects of Powerful Women’s Prosocial Behavior

Whereas powerful women’s emotional labor tends to benefit other people, we propose that practicing emotional labor also entails costs for individual powerholders (Figure 1, path k). Although women may be socialized to practice emotional labor more than men (Brody, 1993; Hibbard and Buhrmester, 1998; Shields, 2002; Chaplin et al., 2005) and may come to develop stronger emotional abilities than men (e.g., Ickes et al., 2000; Cabello et al., 2016), they may still experience the added external demand to practice emotional labor as a burden. If women in power are interpersonally sensitive because they genuinely care for others (i.e., due to an internal motivation to behave in prosocial ways), then they might feel authentic and experience a higher sense of well-being than when they lack power (Kifer et al., 2013), due to an enhanced felt ability to fulfill their communal goals (Keltner et al., 2003; Guinote, 2007, 2008; Diekman et al., 2011). However, to the extent that women in power feel pressured to perform emotional labor (i.e., when they do so to avoid penalties for behaving too dominantly; Phelan and Rudman, 2010), emotional labor may take a psychological toll and detract from their well-being, making the exercise of power exhausting and emotionally draining for women.

Caring for other people can be burdensome in general. Other-oriented emotions such as empathy and compassion (i.e., the emotions that underlie prosocial behavior; Batson, 2011) are cognitively costly and effortful, and people tend to avoid feeling these emotions when given the chance (Cameron and Payne, 2011; Cameron et al., 2019; Scheffer et al., 2021). More specifically, research has documented how emotional labor can be psychologically costly for those who practice it: The purposeful self-control and the suppression of felt emotions that are often involved in the more intrapersonal aspects of emotional labor (Grandey, 2000) have been linked with intensified negative feelings (Scott and Barnes, 2011); emotional dissonance (Hopp et al., 2010); a reduced sense of authenticity (Brotheridge and Grandey, 2002); impaired memory (Richards and Gross, 2000); diminished task performance (Hülsheger and Schewe, 2011); reduced job satisfaction (Judge et al., 2009; Cheung and Tang, 2010); worsened mental health stemming from emotional exhaustion, stress, and burnout (Grandey, 2000; Brotheridge and Grandey, 2002; Beal et al., 2006; Johnson and Spector, 2007); and physical illness, including high blood pressure and cancer (Grandey, 2000; Johnson and Spector, 2007; Hopp et al., 2010).

As this litany suggests, if female powerholders perform more emotional labor than their male counterparts, then they may also fail to realize some of the benefits that power is supposed to bestow on well-being (Kifer et al., 2013). Indeed, women leaders are more likely to be exhausted and chronically stressed than men in similar positions (Thomas et al., 2021). A recent study further revealed that moving up in organizational rank was associated with greater emotional benefits for men than for women—i.e., diminished negative feelings of frustration and discouragement (Taylor et al., 2021). Other research indicates that, compared to men, women anticipate a higher burden of responsibility from attaining a high-power position as well as other negative outcomes (e.g., stronger stress and anxiety; Gino et al., 2015). It is possible that these negative effects may be countered by a sense of fulfillment or personal reward when emotional labor is internally motivated; however, if strong expectations for powerful women to be prosocial create an added pressure for them to engage in emotional labor, the evidence suggests that women will pay a psychological and physical toll.

Implications for Gender Equality at the Top of the Hierarchy

In addition to the potential negative costs for individual women that we discussed in the previous section, emotional labor practices can create an uneven playing field that can contribute to gender inequality in organizational hierarchies (Figure 1, path l). Women continue to be greatly underrepresented in high-power roles (Eagly and Heilman, 2016; United Nations Women, 2021). Part of this underrepresentation is due to prejudice against women who deviate from the traditional gender role (Heilman and Eagly, 2008). For example, as mentioned earlier, there is strong evidence that women in roles of authority face more careful scrutiny than their male counterparts (Brescoll and Uhlmann, 2008; Barrett and Bliss-Moreau, 2009). But in addition to this prejudice, we argue that women’s stronger internal motivation to practice emotional labor, as well as the stronger external demands to do so that they experience relative to men, may undermine their likelihood of actually attaining and retaining power—helping maintain the unequal distribution of men and women in leadership roles.

Channeling Women Toward Less Prestigious Leader Roles

The internal motivation to do emotional labor and enact power more prosocially may keep women from attaining the most prestigious high-level positions. Indeed, women appear more interested in high-power roles when the communal aspects of leadership are emphasized (Schneider et al., 2016; Pate and Fox, 2018; Schneider and Bos, 2019), which makes leadership and femininity appear more congruous (see also Henningsen et al., 2021; and Hentschel et al., 2021). However, such communal attributes are viewed as compatible with leadership primarily in “female-typed” domains such as healthcare or education rather than “male-typed” domains such as technology or finance (Cowgill and Vial, 2022), which tend to be viewed as much more prestigious and to be valued more (Block et al., 2018). Moreover, emotional labor practices may hinder women’s advancement up the management ladder, getting them stuck in mid-levels (e.g., International Labour Office, Bureau for Employers’ Activities, 2019; Einarsdottir et al., 2018). Emotional labor and prosocial work take up leaders’ finite time and energy resources, but are often “invisible” and not usually rewarded in formal ways in organizational contexts (Steinberg, 1999; Guy and Newman, 2004; Bolino and Grant, 2016). Relational service work in academia (e.g., mentoring or doing special favors for students), which female faculty tend to perform at higher rates than male faculty (Tunguz, 2016; Guarino and Borden, 2017; Hanasono et al., 2018; Berheide et al., 2022), is a chief example of the draining effects of such (invisible) emotional labor: Such work takes limited time away from other activities (e.g., research) that are valued much more highly in promotion and tenure decisions. Indeed, experiments show that women are more likely to volunteer for, be asked to perform, and accept requests to do “low-promotability” tasks that benefit organizations but are unlikely to impact career advancement into more senior leadership roles (Babcock et al., 2017). Thus, emotional labor practices may promote gender segregation within leadership, feeding a two-tier system in which male managers are at the top and female managers are at the bottom.

Discouraging Women From Pursuing Top Roles

By making the psychological experience of power overall less appealing for women, the stronger emotional labor demands that women face could discourage them from pursuing high-power roles, ultimately maintaining male dominance in these roles. Again, the difference between internal and external motivation becomes crucial to understanding this nuanced distinction: Whereas women may be intrinsically drawn to high-power roles in contexts that favor communal behavior (Schneider et al., 2016; Pate and Fox, 2018; Schneider and Bos, 2019), they may nevertheless resent the strong external pressure to practice emotional labor around the clock. Research suggests that power is most appealing when it is construed in terms of personal opportunities, and less so when it is construed in terms of responsibility toward others (Sassenberg et al., 2014). If women, relative to men, construe power in a way that entails less autonomy and more responsibility for others, and if women in power experience stronger demands to toe a fine emotional line when dealing with others, these added burdens may partly explain why women are less interested than men in high-power roles. Women may see power as more of a “chore” than men—and rightly so, based on what can be gleaned from the literature on women’s experiences with high power roles (e.g., Brescoll, 2016; Thomas et al., 2021). Power, saddled by strict emotional labor demands, may not be an attractive prospect.

Pushing Women to Opt Out of High-Power Roles

In addition to making power less appealing to women or guiding women toward less prestigious high-level roles, emotional labor can further undermine gender equality by making it difficult for women to retain their power. Even when they attain a high-power role, emotional labor makes exercising that power more exhausting and personally draining for women than for men, which may encourage them to give up or opt out of these roles. As reviewed earlier, emotional labor is costly both psychologically (Richards and Gross, 2000; Brotheridge and Grandey, 2002; Beal et al., 2006; Hopp et al., 2010; Scott and Barnes, 2011) and physically (Grandey, 2000; Johnson and Spector, 2007; Hopp et al., 2010), and has a negative impact on work-related outcomes such as performance and job satisfaction (Judge et al., 2009; Cheung and Tang, 2010; Hülsheger and Schewe, 2011). Qualitative studies have revealed that the difficult task of expressing one’s authentic self while acquiescing to external expectations to perform emotional labor may drive women to opt out of leadership positions (Frkal and Criscione-Naylor, 2020). Additional research is needed to provide quantitative data to support these findings and to further examine the mechanisms through which emotional labor demands push women out of high-power roles, promoting gender segregation at the top of organizations.

Making Women’s Power More Precarious

Beyond the possibility that emotional labor demands may push women out of high-power roles, practicing emotional labor could make power more precarious, putting women at risk of losing it. One reason why women with power cannot simply ignore emotional labor demands from others is that the legitimacy of their power—the extent to which others feel that women deserve to be heeded as authorities—is usually in question (Vial et al., 2016). Even when they occupy a formal position in an organizational hierarchy that confers them with structural power and control over resources, it is more difficult for female authority figures than it is for men in the same positions to elicit status (i.e., respect, admiration, acceptance from others; Magee and Frasier, 2014). These status attributions are key to imbuing power with the kind of legitimacy that fosters cooperation from subordinates and followers (Tyler, 2002, 2006; Levi et al., 2009; Magee and Frasier, 2014). For female powerholders, foregoing emotional labor seems like a steadfast way to lose legitimacy in the eyes of others and, in turn, to be undermined and questioned (Butler and Geis, 1990; Koch, 2005).

Additionally, emotional labor demands may create a catch-22 for women. When they heed such demands, women may run the risk of having their power contested by being seen as inauthentic (Gardner et al., 2009) or too tentative (Forsyth et al., 1997; Bongiorno et al., 2014; Nandkeolyar et al., 2022). The strong communality implicitly conveyed by emotional labor may be seen as more suitable for the follower role than the leader role (Braun et al., 2017). Thus, women in high-power roles who engage in emotional labor may sometimes lose credibility as leaders, especially among male subordinates (Embry et al., 2008; Bongiorno et al., 2014) who have a stronger preference than female subordinates for dominant leaders (Koenig et al., 2011; Vial and Napier, 2018), and who overall appear less supportive of female leaders (e.g., Netchaeva et al., 2015; Vial et al., 2018). These inequalities in leader support contribute to gender segregation at the top of organizations.

Finally, expectations that women will practice emotional labor to a higher extent when they become leaders may result in them being appointed to highly precarious high-power roles—a phenomenon known as the “Glass Cliff” (Ryan and Haslam, 2005, 2007; Glass and Cook, 2016; Morgenroth et al., 2020). Emotional labor (e.g., responding to and managing others’ negative emotions) and a prosocial use of power (i.e., an interpersonally oriented leadership style) may be particularly relevant in crisis situations, making people more likely to support and promote women into high-level roles that are risky and uncertain (Ryan et al., 2011; Gartzia et al., 2012). These precarious appointments, however, can set female leaders up for failure—which may subsequently negatively impact the prospects of other aspiring women leaders. For example, Manzi and Heilman (2021) showed in a series of experiments that participants were less likely to appoint a female candidate to replace an unsuccessful female leader, whereas male candidates were judged independently from the previous leader’s performance. Thus, by encouraging the promotion of women into high-risk leadership positions, emotional labor demands and expectations may help maintain gender inequality in executive roles.

Conclusion

Research indicates that women wield power in more prosocial, interpersonally sensitive ways than men. We propose that a focus on emotional labor can illuminate why men and women express power differently. The current review highlights multiple routes through which emotional labor practices underlie this gender difference, focusing on women’s stronger ability, internal motivation, and external demands to practice emotional labor. By distinguishing among these different routes, we shed light on the disparate consequences of women’s more prosocial use of power. Although it has benefits for other people, it also represents an important burden for women themselves—especially when it is motivated by external demands and the prospect of backlash. The stronger emotional labor demands placed on high-power women relative to high-power men can create an uneven playing field, helping explain why women continue to be sorely underrepresented in high-power roles.

Author’s Note

We are grateful to Jenny Veldman at New York University Abu Dhabi for helpful comments on previous drafts of this manuscript.

Author Contributions

AV conceptualized and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. CC contributed to the literature review and manuscript revision. Both authors approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Throughout this article, we use the term gender to refer to the behavioral, social, and psychological characteristics of women and men (as well as norms and expectations about them), which are frequently associated with the biological categories of female and male (e.g., Deaux, 1985; Pryzgoda and Chrisler, 2000). We favor the term gender over the term sex in our writing because, although it is impossible to fully know the extent of biological and environmental influences on human behavior, the term gender has more inclusive implications than the term sex (Frieze and Chrisler, 2011).
  2. ^ Emotional labor practices can be combined with the unique behavioral affordances of high-power roles, manifesting in leadership styles that are more interpersonally oriented, as we explain in the section on Emotional Labor at the Top of the Hierarchy: Women’s Prosocial Use of Power.

References

Abebe, M., and Dadanlar, H. (2021). From tokens to key players: the influence of board gender and ethnic diversity on corporate discrimination lawsuits. Hum. Relat. 74, 527–555. doi: 10.1177/0018726719888801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amanatullah, E. T., and Morris, M. W. (2010). Negotiating gender roles: gender differences in assertive negotiating are mediated by women’s fear of backlash and attenuated when negotiating on behalf of others. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 256–267. doi: 10.1037/a0017094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amanatullah, E. T., and Tinsley, C. H. (2013). Punishing female negotiators for asserting too much…or not enough: exploring why advocacy moderates backlash against assertive female negotiators. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 120, 110–122. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, C., Keltner, D., and John, O. P. (2003). Emotional convergence between people over time. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 1054–1068. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Argyle, M. (1994). The Psychology of Interpersonal Behaviour. London: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Ashtar, S., Yom-Tov, G. B., Akiva, N., and Rafaeli, A. (2021). When do service employees smile? Response-dependent emotion regulation in emotional labor. J. Organ. Behav. 42, 1202–1227. doi: 10.1002/job.2562

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atwater, L. E., Carey, J. A., and Waldman, D. A. (2001). Gender and discipline in the workplace: wait until your father gets home. J. Manag. 27, 537–561. doi: 10.1177/014920630102700503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babcock, L., Recalde, M. P., Vesterlund, L., and Weingart, L. (2017). Gender differences in accepting and receiving requests for tasks with low promotability. Am. Econ. Rev. 107, 714–747. doi: 10.1257/aer.20141734

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbuto, J. E., and Gifford, G. T. (2010). Examining gender differences of servant leadership: an analysis of the agentic and communal properties of the Servant Leadership Questionnaire. J. Leadersh. Educ. 9, 4–21. doi: 10.12806/V9/I2/RF1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., and Bliss-Moreau, E. (2009). She’s emotional. He’s having a bad day: attributional explanations for emotion stereotypes. Emotion 9, 649–658. doi: 10.1037/a0016821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., Robin, L., Pietromonaco, P. R., and Eyssell, K. M. (1998). Are women the “more emotional” sex? Evidence from emotional experiences in social context. Cogn. Emot. 12, 555–578. doi: 10.1080/026999398379565

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bass, B. M., Avolio, B. J., Jung, D. I., and Berson, Y. (2003). Predicting unit performance by assessing transformational and transactional leadership. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 207–218. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.2.207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C. D. (1987). “Prosocial motivation: is it ever truly altruistic?,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 20, ed. L. Berkowitz (New York, NY: Academic Press), 65–122. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60412-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C. D. (2011). Altruism in Humans. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195341065.001.0001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C. D. (2012). “A history of prosocial behavior research,” in Handbook of the History of Social Psychology, eds A. W. Kruglanski and W. Stroebe (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 243–264. doi: 10.4324/9780203808498

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beal, D. J., Trougakos, J. P., Weiss, H. M., and Green, S. G. (2006). Episodic processes in emotional labor: perceptions of affective delivery and regulation strategies. J. Appl. Psychol. 91, 1053–1065. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.5.1053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellas, M. L. (1999). Emotional labor in academia: the case of professors. Ann. Am. Acad. Polit. Soc. Sci. 561, 96–110. doi: 10.1177/000271629956100107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellou, V. (2011). Do women followers prefer a different leadership style than men? Int. J. Hum. Resour. Manag. 22, 2818–2833. doi: 10.1080/09585192.2011.599677

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berheide, C. W., Carpenter, M. A., and Cotter, D. A. (2022). Teaching college in the time of COVID-19: gender and race differences in faculty emotional labor. Sex Roles 86, 441–455. doi: 10.1007/s11199-021-01271-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biernat, M., and Manis, M. (1994). Shifting standards and stereotype-based judgments. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66, 5–20. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bijlstra, G., Holland, R. W., and Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2010). The social face of emotion recognition: evaluations versus stereotypes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 657–663. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blader, S. L., and Chen, Y. R. (2012). Differentiating the effects of status and power: a justice perspective. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102, 994–1014. doi: 10.1037/a0026651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Block, K., Croft, A., and Schmader, T. (2018). Worth less: why men (and women) devalue care-oriented careers. Front. Psychol. 9:1353. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boatwright, K. J., and Forrest, L. (2000). Leadership preferences: the influence of gender and needs for connection on workers’ ideal preferences for leadership behaviors. J. Leadersh. Stud. 7, 18–34. doi: 10.1177/107179190000700202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolino, M. C., and Grant, A. M. (2016). The bright side of being prosocial at work, and the dark side, too: a review and agenda for research on other-oriented motives, behavior, and impact in organizations. Acad. Manag. Ann. 10, 599–670. doi: 10.5465/19416520.2016.1153260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bongiorno, R., Bain, P. G., and David, B. (2014). If you’re going to be a leader, at least act like it! Prejudice towards women who are tentative in leader roles. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 53, 217–234. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, S., Stegmann, S., Hernandez Bark, A. S., Junker, N. M., and van Dick, R. (2017). Think manager—think male, think follower—think female: gender bias in implicit followership theories. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 47, 377–388. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brescoll, V. L. (2011). Who takes the floor and why: gender, power, and volubility in organizations. Adm. Sci. Q. 56, 622–641. doi: 10.1177/0001839212439994

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brescoll, V. L. (2016). Leading with their hearts? How gender stereotypes of emotion lead to biased evaluations of female leaders. Leadersh. Q. 27, 415–428. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2016.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brescoll, V. L., and Uhlmann, E. L. (2008). Can an angry woman get ahead? Status conferral, gender, and expression of emotion in the workplace. Psychol. Sci. 19, 268–275. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02079.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brett, J. F., Atwater, L. E., and Waldman, D. A. (2005). Effective delivery of workplace discipline. Group Organ. Manag. 30, 487–513. doi: 10.1177/1059601104267606

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brody, L. R. (1993). “On understanding gender differences in the expression of emotion: gender roles, socialization, and language,” in Human Feelings: Explorations in Affect Development and Meaning, eds S. L. Ablon, D. Brown, E. J. Khantzian, and J. E. Mack (Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press, Inc.), 87–121. doi: 10.4324/9780203778821

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brody, L. R., and Hall, J. A. (2010). “Gender, emotion, and socialization,” in Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology, eds J. Chrisler and D. McCreary (New York, NY: Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-1-4419-1465-1_21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brotheridge, C. M., and Grandey, A. A. (2002). Emotional labor and burnout: comparing two perspectives of “people work”. J. Vocat. Behav. 60, 17–39. doi: 10.1006/jvbe.2001.1815

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, D., and Borgida, E. (1999). Who women are, who women should be: descriptive and prescriptive gender stereotyping in sex discrimination. Psychol. Public Policy Law 5, 665–692. doi: 10.1037/1076-8971.5.3.665

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, D., and Geis, F. L. (1990). Nonverbal affect responses to male and female leaders: implications for leadership evaluations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 48–59. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.1.48

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, A., Dionisi, A. M., Barling, J., Akers, A., Robertson, J., Lys, R., et al. (2014). The depleted leader: the influence of leaders’ diminished psychological resources on leadership behaviors. Leadersh. Q. 25, 344–357. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2013.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabello, R., Sorrel, M. A., Fernández-Pinto, I., Extremera, N., and Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2016). Age and gender differences in ability emotional intelligence in adults: a cross-sectional study. Dev. Psychol. 52, 1486–1492. doi: 10.1037/dev0000191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, C. D., and Payne, B. K. (2011). Escaping affect: how motivated emotion regulation creates insensitivity to mass suffering. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 1–15. doi: 10.1037/a0021643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, C. D., Hutcherson, C. A., Ferguson, A. M., Scheffer, J. A., Hadjiandreou, E., and Inzlicht, M. (2019). Empathy is hard work: people choose to avoid empathy because of its cognitive costs. J. Exp. Psychol. 148, 962–976. doi: 10.1037/xge0000595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaplin, T. M., Cole, P. M., and Zahn-Waxler, C. (2005). Parental socialization of emotion expression: gender differences and relations to child adjustment. Emotion 5, 80–88. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.80

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charbonneau, D., and Nicol, A. A. (2002). Emotional intelligence and prosocial behaviors in adolescents. Psychol. Rep. 90, 361–370. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2002.90.2.361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S. C., and Shao, J. (2022). Feminine traits improve transformational leadership advantage: investigation of leaders’ gender traits, sex and their joint impacts on employee contextual performance. Gender Manag. doi: 10.1108/GM-06-2020-0167 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S., Lee-Chai, A. Y., and Bargh, J. A. (2001). Relationship orientation as a moderator of the effects of social power. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 173–187. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, F. Y.-L., and Tang, C. S.-K. (2010). Effects of age, gender, and emotional labor strategies on job outcomes: moderated mediation analyses. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 2, 323–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2010.01037.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chin, M. K., Hambrick, D. C., and Treviño, L. K. (2013). Political ideologies of CEOs: the influence of executives’ values on corporate social responsibility. Adm. Sci. Q. 58, 197–232. doi: 10.1177/0001839213486984

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Citrin, L. B., and Roberts, T. A. (2004). “A feminist psychological perspective on gendered affect,” in The Social Life of Emotions, eds L. Z. Tiedens and C. W. Leach (Cambridge: Cambridge Press), 203–223. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511819568

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, B. J., Burrus, C. J., and Meyer, R. D. (2014). Gender differences in the impact of leadership styles on subordinate embeddedness and job satisfaction. Leadersh. Q. 25, 660–671. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2014.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortes, P., and Pan, J. (2018). “Occupation and gender,” in The Oxford Handbook of Women and the Economy, eds S. L. Averett, L. M. Argys, and S. D. Hoffman (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 425–452. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190628963.013.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Côté, S., Kraus, M. W., Cheng, B. H., Oveis, C., Van der Löwe, I., Lian, H., et al. (2011). Social power facilitates the effect of prosocial orientation on empathic accuracy. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 217–232. doi: 10.1037/a0023171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowgill, C. M., and Vial, A. C. (2022). “Think manager, think female? When managers are associated with stereotypically-female attributes (and why),” in Proceedings of the SPSP 2022 Convention, San Francisco, CA.

Google Scholar

Cross, S. E., and Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: self-construals and gender. Psychol. Bull. 122, 5–37. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.122.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Cremer, D., and van Knippenberg, D. (2002). How do leaders promote cooperation? The effects of charisma and procedural fairness. J. Appl. Psychol. 87, 858–866. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.87.5.858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Wit, F. R., Scheepers, D., Ellemers, N., Sassenberg, K., and Scholl, A. (2017). Whether power holders construe their power as responsibility or opportunity influences their tendency to take advice from others. J. Organ. Behav. 38, 923–949. doi: 10.1002/job.2171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deaux, K. (1985). Sex and gender. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 36, 49–81. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.36.020185.000405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diekman, A. B., Clark, E. K., Johnston, A. M., Brown, E. R., and Steinberg, M. (2011). Malleability in communal goals and beliefs influences attraction to stem careers: evidence for a goal congruity perspective. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 902–918. doi: 10.1037/a0025199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolan, K. (2014). Gender stereotypes, candidate evaluations, and voting for women candidates: what really matters? Polit. Res. Q. 67, 96–107. doi: 10.1177/1065912913487949

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dollar, D., Fisman, R., and Gatti, R. (2001). Are women really the “fairer” sex? Corruption and women in government. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 46, 423–429. doi: 10.1016/S0167-2681(01)00169-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Carli, L. L. (2003). The female leadership advantage: an evaluation of the evidence. Leadersh. Q. 14, 807–834. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2003.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., Gartzia, L., and Carli, L. L. (2014). “Female advantage: revisited,” in The Oxford Handbook of Gender in Organizations, eds S. Kumra, R. Simpson, and R. J. Burke (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 153–174. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199658213.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Heilman, M. E. (2016). Gender and leadership: introduction to the special issue [Editorial]. Leadersh. Q. 27, 349–353. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2016.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C. (2001). The leadership styles of women and men. J. Soc. Issues 57, 781–797. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Johnson, B. T. (1990). Gender and leadership style: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 108, 233–256. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.108.2.233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychol. Rev. 109, 573–598. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., and Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 111, 3–22.

Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Wood, W. (2012). “Social role theory,” in Handbook of Theories in Social Psychology, eds P. van Lange, A. Kruglanski, and E. T. Higgins (London: Sage), 458–476.

Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., and Diekman, A. B. (2000). “Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: a current appraisal,” in The Developmental Social Psychology of Gender, eds T. Eckes and H. M. Trautner (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 123–174.

Google Scholar

Einarsdottir, U. D., Christiansen, T. H., and Kristjansdottir, E. S. (2018). “It’s a man who runs the show”: how women middle-managers experience their professional position, opportunities, and barriers. Sage Open 8:2158244017753989. doi: 10.1177/2158244017753989

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the Face: A Guide to Recognizing Emotions from Facial Clues. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Google Scholar

El-Alayli, A., Hansen-Brown, A. A., and Ceynar, M. (2018). Dancing backwards in high heels: female professors experience more work demands and special favor requests, particularly from academically entitled students. Sex Roles 79, 136–150. doi: 10.1007/s11199-017-0872-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Else-Quest, N. M., Higgins, A., Allison, C., and Morton, L. C. (2012). Gender differences in self-conscious emotional experience: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 138, 947–981. doi: 10.1037/a0027930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Embry, A., Padgett, M. Y., and Caldwell, C. B. (2008). Can leaders step outside of the gender box? An examination of leadership and gender role stereotypes. J. Leadersh. Organ. Stud. 15, 30–45. doi: 10.1177/1548051808318412

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrell, S. K., and Finkelstein, L. M. (2007). Organizational citizenship behavior and gender: expectations and attributions for performance. North Am. J. Psychol. 9, 81–96. doi: 10.1037/e518612013-535

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fast, N. J., Gruenfeld, D. H., Sivanathan, N., and Galinsky, A. D. (2009). Illusory control: a generative force behind power’s far-reaching effects. Psychol. Sci. 20, 502–508. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02311.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischbach, A., Lichtenthaler, P. W., and Horstmann, N. (2015). Leadership and gender stereotyping of emotions: think manager – think male? J. Pers. Psychol. 14, 153–162. doi: 10.1027/1866-5888/a000136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, A. H. (1993). Sex differences in emotionality: fact or stereotype? Fem. Psychol. 3, 303–318. doi: 10.1177/0959353593033002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forsyth, D. R., Heiney, M. M., and Wright, S. S. (1997). Biases in appraisals of women leaders. Group Dyn. Theory Res. Pract. 1, 98–103. doi: 10.1037/1089-2699.1.1.98

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foulk, T. A., Chighizola, N., and Chen, G. (2020). Power corrupts (or does it?): an examination of the boundary conditions of the antisocial effects of experienced power. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 14:e12524. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frieze, I. H., and Boneva, B. S. (2001). “Power motivation and motivation to help others,” in The Use and Abuse of Power: Multiple Perspectives on the Causes of Corruption, eds A. Y. Lee-Chai and J. A. Bargh (Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis), 75–92. doi: 10.4324/9781315783055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frieze, I. H., and Chrisler, J. C. (2011). Editorial policy on the use of the terms “sex” and “gender”. Sex Roles 64, 789–790. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-9988-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frkal, R. A., and Criscione-Naylor, N. (2020). Opt-out stories: women’s decisions to leave corporate leadership. Gender Manag. 36, 1–17. doi: 10.1108/GM-09-2019-0154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galinsky, A. D., Rucker, D. D., and Magee, J. C. (2016). Power and perspective-taking: a critical examination. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 67, 91–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, W. L., Fischer, D., and Hunt, J. G. J. (2009). Emotional labor and leadership: a threat to authenticity? Leadersh. Q. 20, 466–482. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.03.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garnefski, N., Teerds, J., Kraaij, V., Legerstee, J., and van den Kommer, T. (2004). Cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: differences between males and females. Pers. Individ. Dif. 36, 267–276. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00083-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gartzia, L., and Baniandrés, J. (2019). How feminine is the female advantage? Incremental validity of gender traits over leader sex on employees’ responses. J. Bus. Res. 99, 125–139. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2018.12.062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gartzia, L., and van Knippenberg, D. (2016). Too masculine, too bad: effects of communion on leaders’ promotion of cooperation. Group Organ. Manag. 41, 458–490. doi: 10.1177/1059601115583580

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gartzia, L., Ryan, M. K., Balluerka, N., and Aritzeta, A. (2012). Think crisis–think female: further evidence. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 21, 603–628. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2011.591572

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerzema, J., and D’Antonio, M. (2013). The Athena Doctrine: How Women (and the Men Who Think Like Them) Will Rule the Future. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Google Scholar

Gerzema, J., and D’Antonio, M. (2017). The Athena doctrine: Millennials seek feminine values in leadership. J. Leadersh. Stud. 10, 63–65. doi: 10.1002/jls.21506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gino, F., Wilmuth, C. A., and Brooks, A. W. (2015). Compared to men, women view professional advancement as equally attainable, but less desirable. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 12354–12359. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1502567112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glass, C., and Cook, A. (2016). Leading at the top: understanding women’s challenges above the glass ceiling. Leadersh. Q. 27, 51–63. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2015.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glass, C., and Cook, A. (2018). Do women leaders promote positive change? Analyzing the effect of gender on business practices and diversity initiatives. Hum. Resour. Manag. 57, 823–837. doi: 10.1002/hrm.21838

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glass, C., Cook, A., and Ingersoll, A. R. (2016). Do women leaders promote sustainability? Analyzing the effect of corporate governance composition on environmental performance. Bus. Strategy Environ. 25, 495–511. doi: 10.1002/bse.1879

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, A. M., and Chen, S. (2013). Does power help or hurt? The moderating role of self–other focus on power and perspective-taking in romantic relationships. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 39, 1097–1110. doi: 10.1177/0146167213490031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotional regulation in the workplace: a new way to conceptualize emotional labor. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 5, 95–110. doi: 10.1037//1076-8998.5.1.95

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandey, A. A. (2003). When “the show must go on”: surface acting and deep acting as determinants of emotional exhaustion and peer-rated service delivery. Acad. Manag. J. 46, 86–96. doi: 10.2307/30040678

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guarino, C. M., and Borden, V. M. (2017). Faculty service loads and gender: are women taking care of the academic family? Res. High. Educ. 58, 672–694. doi: 10.1007/s11162-017-9454-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guimond, S., Chatard, A., Martinot, D., Crisp, R. J., and Redersdorff, S. (2006). Social comparison, self-stereotyping, and gender differences in self-construals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90, 221–242. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guinote, A. (2007). Behaviour variability and the situated focus theory of power. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 18, 256–295. doi: 10.1080/10463280701692813

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guinote, A. (2008). Power and affordances: when the situation has more power over powerful than powerless individuals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95, 237–252. doi: 10.1037/a0012518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guy, M. E., and Newman, M. A. (2004). Women’s jobs, men’s jobs: sex segregation and emotional labor. Public Adm. Rev. 64, 289–298. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6210.2004.00373.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haines, E. L., Deaux, K., and Lofaro, N. (2016). The times they are a-changing …or are they not? A comparison of gender stereotypes, 1983–2014. Psychol. Women Q. 40, 353–363. doi: 10.1177/0361684316634081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal Sex Differences: Accuracy of Communication and Expressive Style. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Google Scholar

Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., and LeBeau, L. S. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and the vertical dimension of social relations: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 131, 898–924. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, J. A., and Gunnery, S. D. (2013). “Gender differences in nonverbal communication,” in Nonverbal Communication, eds J. A. Hall and M. L. Knapp (Boston, MA: De Gruyter Mouton), 639–669. doi: 10.1515/9783110238150.639

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, J. A., Murphy, N. A., and Mast, M. S. (2007). Nonverbal self-accuracy in interpersonal interaction. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 33, 1675–1685. doi: 10.1177/0146167207307492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanasono, L. K., Broido, E. M., Yacobucci, M. M., Root, K. V., Peña, S., and O’Neil, D. A. (2018). Secret service: revealing gender biases in the visibility and value of faculty service. J. Divers. High. Educ. 12, 85–98. doi: 10.1037/dhe0000081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hays, N. A. (2013). Fear and loving in social hierarchy: sex differences in preferences for power versus status. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 1130–1136. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heilman, M. E., and Chen, J. J. (2005). Same behavior, different consequences: reactions to men’s and women’s altruistic citizenship behavior. J. Appl. Psychol. 90, 431–441. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.3.431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heilman, M. E., and Eagly, A. H. (2008). Gender stereotypes are alive, well, and busy producing workplace discrimination. Ind. Organ. Psychol. 1, 393–398. doi: 10.1111/j.1754-9434.2008.00072.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heilman, M. E., and Okimoto, T. G. (2007). Why are women penalized for success at male tasks? The implied communality deficit. J. Appl. Psychol. 92, 81–92. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.1.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helweg-Larsen, M., Cunningham, S. J., Carrico, A., and Pergram, A. M. (2004). To nod or not to nod: an observational study of nonverbal communication and status in female and male college students. Psychol. Women Q. 28, 358–361. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2004.00152.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henningsen, L., Eagly, A. H., and Jonas, K. (2021). Where are the women deans? The importance of gender bias and self-selection processes for the deanship ambition of female and male professors. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12780 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hentschel, T., Braun, S., Peus, C., and Frey, D. (2018). The communality-bonus effect for male transformational leaders–Leadership style, gender, and promotability. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 27, 112–125. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2017.1402759

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hentschel, T., Braun, S., Peus, C., and Frey, D. (2021). Sounds like a fit! Wording in recruitment advertisements and recruiter gender affect women’s pursuit of career development programs via anticipated belongingness. Hum. Resour. Manag. 60, 581–602. doi: 10.1002/hrm.22043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hentschel, T., Heilman, M. E., and Peus, C. V. (2019). The multiple dimensions of gender stereotypes: a current look at men’s and women’s characterizations of others and themselves. Front. Psychol. 10:11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, U., Adams, R. Jr., and Kleck, R. (2005). Who may frown and who should smile? Dominance, affiliation, and the display of happiness and anger. Cogn. Emot. 19, 515–536. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000364

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hibbard, D. R., and Buhrmester, D. (1998). The role of peers in the socialization of gender-related social interaction styles. Sex Roles 39, 185–202. doi: 10.1023/A:1018846303929

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The Managed Heart: Commercialization of Human Feeling. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Google Scholar

Hoobler, J. M., Masterson, C. R., Nkomo, S. M., and Michel, E. J. (2018). The business case for women leaders: meta-analysis, research critique, and path forward. J. Manag. 44, 2473–2499. doi: 10.1177/0149206316628643

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopp, H., Rohrmann, S., Zapf, D., and Hodapp, V. (2010). Psychophysiological effects of emotional dissonance in a face-to-face service interaction. Anxiety Stress Coping 23, 399–414. doi: 10.1080/10615800903254091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, E. S., Gardner, W. L., and Thompson, L. (2007). The role of the self-concept and the social context in determining the behavior of power holders: self-construal in intergroup versus dyadic dispute resolution negotiations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 614–631. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, N., Badura, K. L., Newman, D. A., and Speach, M. E. P. (2021). Gender, “masculinity,” and “femininity”: a meta-analytic review of gender differences in agency and communion. Psychol. Bull. 147, 987–1011. doi: 10.1037/bul0000343

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hülsheger, U. R., and Schewe, A. F. (2011). On the costs and benefits of emotional labor: a meta-analysis of three decades of research. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 16, 361–389. doi: 10.1037/a0022876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphrey, R. H. (2012). How do leaders use emotional labor? J. Organ. Behav. 33, 740–744. doi: 10.1002/job.1791

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ickes, W., Gesn, P. R., and Graham, T. (2000). Gender differences in empathic accuracy: differential ability or differential motivation? Pers. Relationsh. 7, 95–109. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2000.tb00006.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inesi, M. E., Botti, S., Dubois, D., Rucker, D. D., and Galinsky, A. D. (2011). Power and choice: their dynamic interplay in quenching the thirst for personal control. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1042–1048. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

International Labour Office, Bureau for Employers’ Activities (2019). Women in Business and Management: The Business Case for Change. Available online at: https://www.ilo.org/wcmsp5/groups/public/—dgreports/—dcomm/—publ/documents/publication/wcms_700953.pdf (accessed December 20, 2021).

Google Scholar

Iszatt-White, M. (2009). Leadership as emotional labour: the effortful accomplishment of valuing practices. Leadership 5, 447–467. doi: 10.1177/1742715009343032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, H. A. M., and Spector, P. E. (2007). Service with a smile: do emotional intelligence, gender, and autonomy moderate the emotional labor process? J. Occup. Health Psychol. 12, 319–333. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.12.4.319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joseph, D. L., and Newman, D. A. (2010). Emotional intelligence: an integrative meta-analysis and cascading model. J. Appl. Psychol. 95, 54–78. doi: 10.1037/a0017286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Josephs, R. A., Markus, H. R., and Tafarodi, R. W. (1992). Gender and self-esteem. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 391–402. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshi, M. P., and Diekman, A. B. (2021). My fair lady? Inferring organizational trust from the mere presence of women in leadership roles. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. doi: 10.1177/01461672211035957 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Judge, T. A., Woolf, E. F., and Hurst, C. (2009). Is emotional labor more difficult for some than for others? A multilevel, experience-sampling study. Pers. Psychol. 62, 57–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2008.01129.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kakkar, H., and Sivanathan, N. (2021). The impact of leader dominance on employees’ zero-sum mindset and helping behavior. J. Appl. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/apl0000980 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., and Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychol. Rev. 110, 265–284. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kifer, Y., Heller, D., Perunovic, W. Q. E., and Galinsky, A. D. (2013). The good life of the powerful: the experience of power and authenticity enhances subjective well-being. Psychol. Sci. 24, 280–288. doi: 10.1177/0956797612450891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. K., Harold, C. M., and Holtz, B. C. (2021). Evaluations of abusive supervisors: the moderating role of the abuser’s gender. J. Organ. Behav. 43, 465–482. doi: 10.1002/job.2581

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kipnis, D. (1972). Does power corrupt? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 24, 33–41. doi: 10.1037/h0033390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, K. J. K., and Hodges, S. D. (2001). Gender differences, motivation, and empathic accuracy: when it pays to understand. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 27, 720–730. doi: 10.1177/0146167201276007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koch, S. C. (2005). Evaluative affect display toward male and female leaders of task-oriented groups. Small Group Res. 36, 678–703. doi: 10.1177/1046496405281768

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., and Ristikari, T. (2011). Are leader stereotypes masculine? A meta-analysis of three research paradigms. Psychol. Bull. 137:616. doi: 10.1037/a0023557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraus, M. W., Chen, S., and Keltner, D. (2011). The power to be me: power elevates self-concept consistency and authenticity. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47, 974–980. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.03.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

La France, B. H., Henningsen, D. D., Oates, A., and Shaw, C. M. (2009). Social-sexual interactions? Meta-analyses of sex differences in perceptions of flirtatiousness, seductiveness, and promiscuousness. Commun. Monogr. 76, 263–285. doi: 10.1080/03637750903074701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaFrance, M., Hecht, M. A., and Paluck, E. L. (2003). The contingent smile: a meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. Psychol. Bull. 129, 305–334. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaFrance, M., and Vial, A. C. (2016). “Gender and nonverbal behavior,” in APA Handbook of Nonverbal Communication, eds D. Matsumoto, H. C. Hwang, and M. G. Frank (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 139–161. doi: 10.1037/14669-006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lammers, J., Galinsky, A. D., Gordijn, E. H., and Otten, S. (2012). Power increases social distance. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 3, 282–290. doi: 10.1177/1948550611418679

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lammers, J., Stoker, J. I., Rink, F., and Galinsky, A. D. (2016). To have control over or to be free from others? The desire for power reflects a need for autonomy. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 42, 498–512. doi: 10.1177/0146167216634064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leach, C. W., Carraro, L., Garcia, R. L., and Kang, J. J. (2017). Morality stereotyping as a basis of women’s in-group favoritism: an implicit approach. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 20, 153–172. doi: 10.1177/1368430215603462

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leaper, C., and Ayres, M. M. (2007). A meta-analytic review of gender variations in adults’ language use: talkativeness, affiliative speech, and assertive speech. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 11, 328–363. doi: 10.1177/1088868307302221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leaper, C., and Robnett, R. D. (2011). Women are more likely than men to use tentative language, aren’t they? A meta-analysis testing for gender differences and moderators. Psychol. Women Q. 35, 129–142. doi: 10.1177/0361684310392728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemoine, G. J., and Blum, T. C. (2021). Servant leadership, leader gender, and team gender role: testing a female advantage in a cascading model of performance. Pers. Psychol. 74, 3–28. doi: 10.1111/peps.12379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levi, M., Sacks, A., and Tyler, T. (2009). Conceptualizing legitimacy, measuring legitimating beliefs. Am. Behav. Sci. 53, 354–375. doi: 10.1177/0002764209338797

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, K. M. (2000). When leaders display emotion: how followers respond to negative emotional expression of male and female leaders. J. Organ. Behav. 21, 221–234.

Google Scholar

Lopez-Zafra, E., and Gartzia, L. (2014). Perceptions of gender differences in self-report measures of emotional intelligence. Sex Roles 70, 479–495. doi: 10.1007/s11199-014-0368-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, A., Rosette, A. S., and Koval, C. Z. (2022). Reconciling female agentic advantage and disadvantage with the CADDIS measure of agency. J. Appl. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/apl0000550 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magee, J. C., and Frasier, C. W. (2014). Status and power: the principal inputs to influence for public managers. Public Adm. Rev. 74, 307–317. doi: 10.1111/puar.12203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manzi, F., and Heilman, M. E. (2021). Breaking the glass ceiling: for one and all? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 120, 257–277. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, D. (1990). Cultural similarities and differences in display rules. Motiv. Emot. 14, 195–214. doi: 10.1007/BF00995569

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., Hirayama, S., and Petrova, G. (2005). Development and validation of a measure of display rule knowledge: the display rule assessment inventory. Emotion 5, 23–40. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., and Salovey, P. (1999). Emotional intelligence meets traditional standards for an intelligence. Intelligence 27, 267–298. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00016-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClure, E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychol. Bull. 126, 424–453. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McRae, K., Ochsner, K. N., Mauss, I. B., Gabrieli, J., and Gross, J. J. (2008). Gender differences in emotion regulation: an fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 11, 143–162. doi: 10.1177/1368430207088035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meier, K. J., Mastracci, S. H., and Wilson, K. (2006). Gender and emotional labor in public organizations: an empirical examination of the link to performance. Public Adm. Rev. 66, 899–909. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6210.2006.00657.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moran, C. M., Diefendorff, J. M., and Greguras, G. J. (2013). Understanding emotional display rules at work and outside of work: the effects of country and gender. Motiv. Emot. 37, 323–334. doi: 10.1007/s11031-012-9301-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgenroth, T., Kirby, T. A., Ryan, M. K., and Sudkämper, A. (2020). The who, when, and why of the glass cliff phenomenon: a meta-analysis of appointments to precarious leadership positions. Psychol. Bull. 146, 797–829. doi: 10.1037/bul0000234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moskowitz, D. S., Suh, E. J., and Desaulniers, J. (1994). Situational influences on gender differences in agency and communion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66, 753–761. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.4.753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moss-Racusin, C. A., and Rudman, L. A. (2010). Disruptions in women’s self-promotion: the backlash avoidance model. Psychol. Women Q. 34, 186–202. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2010.01561.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nandkeolyar, A. K., Bagger, J., and Ekkirala, S. (2022). Damned if she does, damned if she doesn’t: the interactive effects of gender and agreeableness on performance evaluation. J. Bus. Res. 143, 62–71. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2022.01.066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neel, R., Becker, D. V., Neuberg, S. L., and Kenrick, D. T. (2012). Who expressed what emotion? Men grab anger, women grab happiness. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 583–586. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Netchaeva, E., Kouchaki, M., and Sheppard, L. D. (2015). A man’s (precarious) place: men’s experienced threat and self-assertive reactions to female superiors. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 41, 1247–1259. doi: 10.1177/0146167215593491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nixon, D. (2009). ‘I can’t put a smiley face on’: working-class masculinity, emotional labour and service work in the ‘new economy’. Gender Work Organ. 16, 300–322. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0432.2009.00446.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Offermann, L. R., and Foley, K. (2020). “Is there a female leadership advantage?,” in Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Business and Management, ed. M. A. Hitt (Oxford: Oxford University Press). doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190224851.013.61

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overbeck, J. R., and Park, B. (2006). Powerful perceivers, powerless objects: flexibility of powerholders’ social attention. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 99, 227–243. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pate, J., and Fox, R. (2018). Getting past the gender gap in political ambition. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 156, 166–183. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2018.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petkanopoulou, K., Rodríguez-Bailón, R., Willis, G. B., and van Kleef, G. A. (2019). Powerless people don’t yell but tell: the effects of social power on direct and indirect expression of anger. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 49, 533–547. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V., and Furnham, A. (2000). Gender differences in measured and self-estimated trait emotional intelligence. Sex Roles 42, 449–461. doi: 10.1023/A:1007006523133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phelan, J. E., and Rudman, L. A. (2010). Prejudice toward female leaders: backlash effects and women’s impression management dilemma. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 4, 807–820. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00306.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plant, E. A., Hyde, J. S., Keltner, D., and Devine, P. G. (2000). The gender stereotyping of emotions. Psychol. Women Q. 24, 81–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb01024.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Post, C. (2015). When is female leadership an advantage? Coordination requirements, team cohesion, and team interaction norms. J. Organ. Behav. 36, 1153–1175. doi: 10.1002/job.2031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Post, C., Latu, I. M., and Belkin, Y. (2019). A female leadership trust advantage in times of crisis: under what conditions? Psychol. Women Q. 43, 215–231. doi: 10.1177/0361684319828292

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prentice, D. A., and Carranza, E. (2002). What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: the contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychol. Women Q. 26, 269–281. doi: 10.1111/1471-6402.t01-1-00066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prime, J. L., Carter, N. M., and Welbourne, T. M. (2009). Women “take care,” men “take charge”: managers’ stereotypic perceptions of women and men leaders. Psychol. Manag. J. 12, 25–49. doi: 10.1080/10887150802371799

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pryzgoda, J., and Chrisler, J. C. (2000). Definitions of gender and sex: the subtleties of meaning. Sex Roles 43, 553–569. doi: 10.1023/a:1007123617636

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rafferty, A. E., and Griffin, M. A. (2004). Dimensions of transformational leadership: conceptual and empirical extensions. Leadersh. Q. 15, 329–354. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2004.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rand, D. G., Brescoll, V. L., Everett, J. A., Capraro, V., and Barcelo, H. (2016). Social heuristics and social roles: intuition favors altruism for women but not for men. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 145, 389–396. doi: 10.1037/xge0000154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raymondie, R. A., and Steiner, D. D. (2021). Backlash against counter-stereotypical leader emotions and the role of follower affect in leader evaluations. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1–17. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12778 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, J. M., and Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: the cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 410–424. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rios, K., Fast, N. J., and Gruenfeld, D. H. (2015). Feeling high but playing low: power, need to belong, and submissive behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 41, 1135–1146. doi: 10.1177/0146167215591494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivas, M. F. (2013). An experiment on corruption and gender. Bull. Econ. Res. 65, 10–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8586.2012.00450.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rucker, D. D., and Galinsky, A. D. (2016). The agentic-communal model of power: implications for consumer behavior. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 10, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: the costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression management. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 629–645. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, M. K., and Haslam, S. A. (2005). The glass cliff: evidence that women are over-represented in precarious leadership positions. Br. J. Manag. 16, 81–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8551.2005.00433.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, M. K., and Haslam, S. A. (2007). The glass cliff: exploring the dynamics surrounding the appointment of women to precarious leadership positions. Acad. Manag. Rev. 32, 549–572. doi: 10.5465/amr.2007.24351856

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, M. K., Haslam, S. A., Hersby, M. D., and Bongiorno, R. (2011). Think crisis–think female: the glass cliff and contextual variation in the think manager–think male stereotype. J. Appl. Psychol. 96, 470–484. doi: 10.1037/a0022133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sassenberg, K., Ellemers, N., Scheepers, D., and Scholl, A. (2014). ““Power corrupts” revisited: the role of construal of power as opportunity or responsibility,” in Power, Politics, and Paranoia: Why People are Suspicious of Their Leaders, eds J.-W. van Prooijen and P. A. M. van Lange (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 73–87. doi: 10.1017/cbo9781139565417.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaubroeck, J., and Jones, J. R. (2000). Antecedents of workplace emotional labor dimensions and moderators of their effects on physical symptoms. J. Organ. Behav. 21, 163–183.

Google Scholar

Scheffer, J. A., Cameron, C. D., and Inzlicht, M. (2021). Caring is costly: people avoid the cognitive work of compassion. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 151, 172–196. doi: 10.1037/xge0001073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid Mast, M., Jonas, K., and Hall, J. A. (2009). Give a person power and he or she will show interpersonal sensitivity: the phenomenon and its why and when. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 835–850. doi: 10.1037/a0016234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, M. C., and Bos, A. L. (2019). The application of social role theory to the study of gender in politics. Polit. Psychol. 40, 173–213. doi: 10.1111/pops.12573

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, M. C., Holman, M. R., Diekman, A. B., and McAndrew, T. (2016). Power, conflict, and community: how gendered views of political power influence women’s political ambition. Polit. Psychol. 37, 515–531. doi: 10.1111/pops.12268

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, B. A., and Barnes, C. M. (2011). A multilevel field investigation of emotional labor, affect, work withdrawal, and gender. Acad. Manag. J. 54, 116–136. doi: 10.5465/amj.2011.59215086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shields, S. A. (2000). “Thinking about gender, thinking about theory: gender and emotional experience,” in Gender and Emotion: Social Psychological Perspectives, ed. A. H. Fischer (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 3–23. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511628191.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shields, S. A. (2002). Speaking from the Heart: Gender and the Social Meaning of Emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Shields, S. A. (2013). Gender and emotion: what we think we know, what we need to know, and why it matters. Psychol. Women Q. 37, 423–435. doi: 10.1177/0361684313502312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair, L., and Kunda, Z. (2000). Motivated stereotyping of women: she’s fine if she praised me but incompetent if she criticized me. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 1329–1342. doi: 10.1177/0146167200263002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skytte, R. (2021). Dimensions of elite partisan polarization: disentangling the effects of incivility and issue polarization. Br. J. Polit. Sci. 51, 1457–1475. doi: 10.1017/S0007123419000760

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. S., Brescoll, V. L., and Thomas, E. L. (2016). “Constrained by emotion: women, leadership, and expressing emotion in the workplace,” in Handbook on Well-Being of Working Women, eds M. Connerley and J. Wu (Dordrecht: Springer), 209–224.

Google Scholar

Smith, J. S., LaFrance, M., Knol, K. H., Tellinghuisen, D. J., and Moes, P. (2015). Surprising smiles and unanticipated frowns: how emotion and status influence gender categorization. J. Nonverbal Behav. 39, 115–130. doi: 10.1007/s10919-014-0202-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spisak, B. R., Homan, A. C., Grabo, A., and Van Vugt, M. (2012). Facing the situation: testing a biosocial contingency model of leadership in intergroup relations using masculine and feminine faces. Leadersh. Q. 23, 273–280. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.08.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, R. J. (1999). Emotional labor in job evaluation: redesigning compensation practices. Ann. Am. Acad. Polit. Soc. Sci. 561, 143–157. doi: 10.1177/0002716299561001010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stempel, C. R., Rigotti, T., and Mohr, G. (2015). Think transformational leadership–Think female? Leadership 11, 259–280. doi: 10.1177/1742715015590468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sturm, R. E., and Antonakis, J. (2015). Interpersonal power: a review, critique, and research agenda. J. Manag. 41, 136–163. doi: 10.1177/0149206314555769

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swamy, A., Knack, S., Lee, Y., and Azfar, O. (2001). Gender and corruption. J. Dev. Econ. 64, 25–55. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3878(00)00123-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, C. L., Ivcevic, Z., Moeller, J., Menges, J. I., Reiter-Palmon, R., and Brackett, M. A. (2021). Gender and emotions at work: organizational rank has greater emotional benefits for men than women. Sex Roles 86, 1–16. doi: 10.1007/s11199-021-01256-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, R., Cooper, M., Urban, K. M., Cardazone, G., Bohrer, A., Mahajan, S., et al. (2021). Women in the Workplace. McKinsey & Company. Available online at: https://www.mckinsey.com/featured-insights/diversity-and-inclusion/women-in-the-workplace (accessed December 20, 2021).

Google Scholar

Tiedens, L. Z., Ellsworth, P. C., and Mesquita, B. (2000). Sentimental stereotypes: emotional expectations for high-and low-status group members. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 560–575. doi: 10.1177/0146167200267004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timmers, M., Fischer, A., and Manstead, A. (2003). Ability versus vulnerability: beliefs about men’s and women’s emotional behaviour. Cogn. Emot. 17, 41–63. doi: 10.1080/02699930302277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tost, L. P., and Johnson, H. H. (2019). The prosocial side of power: how structural power over subordinates can promote social responsibility. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 152, 25–46. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2019.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Totterdell, P., and Holman, D. (2003). Emotion regulation in customer service roles: testing a model of emotional labor. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 8, 55–73. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.8.1.55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tunguz, S. (2016). In the eye of the beholder: emotional labor in academia varies with tenure and gender. Stud. High. Educ. 41, 3–20. doi: 10.1080/03075079.2014.914919

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, T. R. (2002). Leadership and cooperation in groups. Am. Behav. Sci. 45, 769–782. doi: 10.1177/0002764202045005003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, T. R. (2006). Psychological perspectives on legitimacy and legitimation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 375–400. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics (2021). Women in the Labor Force: A Databook. Available online at: https://www.bls.gov/opub/reports/womens-databook/2020/pdf/home.pdf (accessed December 20, 2021).

Google Scholar

Uhlmann, E. L., Zhu, L. L., and Tannenbaum, D. (2013). When it takes a bad person to do the right thing. Cognition 126, 326–334.

Google Scholar

United Nations Women (2021). Facts and Figures: Women’s Leadership and Political Participation. Available online at: https://www.unwomen.org/en/what-we-do/leadership-and-political-participation/facts-and-figures#_edn1 (accessed December 20, 2021).

Google Scholar

van Engen, M. L., and Willemsen, T. M. (2004). Sex and leadership styles: a meta-analysis of research published in the 1990s. Psychol. Rep. 94, 3–18. doi: 10.2466/PR0.94.1.3-18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Kleef, G. A., Oveis, C., Van Der Löwe, I., LuoKogan, A., Goetz, J., and Keltner, D. (2008). Power, distress, and compassion: turning a blind eye to the suffering of others. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1315–1322. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02241.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vecchio, R. P., and Boatwright, K. J. (2002). Preferences for idealized styles of supervision. Leadersh. Q. 13, 327–342. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00118-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vial, A. C., Brescoll, V. L., Napier, J. L., Dovidio, J. F., and Tyler, T. R. (2018). Differential support for female supervisors among men and women. J. Appl. Psychol. 103, 215–227. doi: 10.1037/apl0000258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vial, A. C., and Cimpian, A. (2020). “Evaluative feedback expresses and reinforces cultural stereotypes,” in Psychological Perspectives on Praise, ed. E. Brummelman (Abingdon: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Vial, A. C., and Napier, J. L. (2018). Unnecessary frills: communality as a nice (but expendable) trait in leaders. Front. Psychol. 9:1866. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vial, A. C., Napier, J. L., and Brescoll, V. L. (2016). A bed of thorns: female leaders and the self-reinforcing cycle of illegitimacy. Leadersh. Q. 27, 400–414. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2015.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vinkenburg, C. J., Van Engen, M. L., Eagly, A. H., and Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C. (2011). An exploration of stereotypical beliefs about leadership styles: is transformational leadership a route to women’s promotion? Leadersh. Q. 22, 10–21. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, F., and Sun, X. (2016). Absolute power leads to absolute corruption? Impact of power on corruption depending on the concepts of power one holds. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 46, 77–89. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, J. B., and Gardner, W. L. (2009). Think women, think warm: stereotype content activation in women with a salient gender identity, using a modified Stroop task. Sex Roles 60, 247–260. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-9526-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, L. M., Mathersul, D., Palmer, D. M., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., and Gordon, E. (2009). Explicit identification and implicit recognition of facial emotions: I. Age effects in males and females across 10 decades. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 31, 257–277. doi: 10.1080/13803390802255635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, M. J., and Tiedens, L. Z. (2016). The subtle suspension of backlash: a meta-analysis of penalties for women’s implicit and explicit dominance behavior. Psychol. Bull. 142, 165–197. doi: 10.1037/bul0000039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witt, M. G., and Wood, W. (2010). Self-regulation of gendered behavior in everyday life. Sex Roles 62, 635–646. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9761-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfram, H. J., and Mohr, G. (2010). Gender-typicality of economic sectors and gender-composition of working groups as moderating variables in leadership research. Gend. Manag. 25, 320–339. doi: 10.1108/17542411011048182

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, W., and Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of women and men: implications for the origins of sex differences. Psychol. Bull. 128, 699–727. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.128.5.699

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoder, J. D. (2001). Making leadership work more effectively for women. J. Soc. Issues 57, 815–828. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotional labor, gender, power, leadership, emotion, prosocial behavior

Citation: Vial AC and Cowgill CM (2022) Heavier Lies Her Crown: Gendered Patterns of Leader Emotional Labor and Their Downstream Effects. Front. Psychol. 13:849566. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.849566

Received: 06 January 2022; Accepted: 03 June 2022;
Published: 29 August 2022.

Edited by:

Tanja Hentschel, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Jamie L. Gloor, University of St. Gallen, Switzerland
Richard Ronay, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands

Copyright © 2022 Vial and Cowgill. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andrea C. Vial, andrea.vial@nyu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.