Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 13 June 2019
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement
This article is part of the Research Topic Best Practice Approaches for Mixed Methods Research in Psychological Science View all 33 articles

Contextual Factors and Decision-Making in the Behavior of Finalization in the Positional Attack in Beach Handball: Differences by Gender Through Polar Coordinates Analysis

  • Departamento Psicología Social, Trabajo Social, Antropología Social y Estudios de Asia Oriental, Universidad de Málaga, Málaga, Spain

The aim of this study was to analyze, in the framework of the mixed methods, the relationship of different contextual factors with the decisions shown in the finalization of the positional attack in beach handball. For this reason, a polar coordinates analysis was carried out, by gender, using as focal behaviors the simple and double goal, and the loss of possession of the attacking team, which are considered decisive in assessing the success of the decisions at the end of the positional attack. These focal behaviors have been linked to criteria that characterize the attack situation such as the minute, the score, the numerical balance, the defensive system and the duration. A total of 24 sessions were observed with the HOISAN computer software, using an ad hoc designed tool. The model of observation used was punctual, multidimensional, and nomothetic. The observation unit used for the positional attack from when the specialist gains control of play until the possession changes. The results obtained showed that an advantage of goals scored and the numerical balance situations of the teams modulate significantly the appropriate decision made by both categories. However, they also indicated differences in the flow of pairing behaviors for both categories in some aspects. Specifically, it has been observed that a longer duration of the attack in the female category has been linked significantly to a successful performance and the results also show that an elaborated attack is related to successful behaviors. In the male category, it has been observed that the technical fouls made by the attackers increase in the last minutes of the match. Likewise, as regards the to the opposing team’s defensive system, the results in the male category are related to successful behaviors before any of them; whereas, in the female category, when playing against a defensive pressure system, the results relate more to errors during the performance of the pass and reception. The use of polar coordinates for the estimation of technical-tactical relations allows, from a psychological point of view, determine the techniques and procedures of psychological intervention that optimize the action resources of the players individually and the team as a collective.

Introduction

Published research on the planning of training in collective sports emphasizes the importance of the gradual increase of physical, technical and tactical demands in the competition in recent decades (Carling, 2011; Sarmento et al., 2017). This fact has led to the need to increase specific knowledge to carry out proper planning and make the preparation of the team as complete as possible (Nunes et al., 2015). Beach handball has also been immersed in this tendency, proof of this is the constant evolution that has developed in all aspects of the game since its beginnings in the nineties to today (Morillo-Baro and Hernández-Mendo, 2015; Navarro et al., 2018), promoting in the professionals of this sport a continuous update and innovation.

Despite being a method that comes from the handball court, it also brings with it many differences, turning it into a completely new method (Belančić, 2005). One of the most significant differences lies in the possibility of scoring goals, each with a different point value, achieved through a variation of types of game play. Goals of simple value are achieved by means of a classic throw made by any of the players except the goalkeeper-specialist, and goals of double value are made by a specialist's shot, throwing from a dive shot or throwing in 360 degrees (Ostoić and Ohnjec, 2015). The commitment of the International Handball Federation (IHF) and the European Handball Federation (EHF) by this modality is evident. This has led an increase in the number of international tournaments that are held every year, such as world championships and continental championships (Morillo-Baro et al., 2015).

Being a collective sport, beach handball has an open character. This means that it presents a high degree of uncertainty, which can lead to thinking about randomness of the game (Jiménez-Salas and Hernández-Mendo, 2016). So, to compete at the highest level it requires a high level of selective attention that allows the athlete to capture the most important stimuli of the game for further processing, enabling a correct decision making at all times (Gil-Arias et al., 2016). During the game there are constant changes of players and different situations of numerical superiority or inferiority that occur at a high speed, this recreates a continuously changing environment, where athletes have to make decisions constantly to adapt to the large number of different game situations found (Gil-Arias et al., 2012).

There are numerous factors or situational variables that can alter the decisions made during the course of a match, such as the type or level of the opponent he/she faces to, the result, the moment of the game or the evolution of the score (Aguilar et al., 2012). Another aspect is the possibility that a player from the opposing team receives a suspension, creating a situation of double numerical superiority, creating a different decision situation that can lead to a change in the decisional performance of the attacking team (Aguilar et al., 2012). There is research that has studied the influence of these determining factors in athletes’ decisions. In handball, there has been studies concerning the situations of numerical inequality (Aguilar et al., 2012). Research in volleyball analyzes the relationship between decision-making and performance in three actions of the game (reception, stage, attack) (Conejero et al., 2018). Studies in soccer show the results of the influence of contextual factors on the decision making of highly qualified players through semi-structured personal interviews, where it was clear the importance of considering the dynamic and static context in which players make decisions (Levi and Jackson, 2018). The study of these factors or external variables that determine the decision making of athletes is the central axis around which this research has revolved. Araújo et al. (2016) propose to study the decision-making process based on the ecological perspective, observing the results of the athletes’ performance through patterns of success against unsuccessful patterns considering various factors such as the game space, the team mates and rivals. The observation of this result of performance can be done through a Field Format and Systems mixed observation tool of E/ME categories (Exhaustive and Mutually Exclusive) (Araújo et al., 2016).

The investigations carried out in the field of sports, within the framework of the mixed methods, have to connect qualitative and quantitative elements (Plano-Clark and Sanders, 2015). In this way, the Observational Methodology (OM) is a technique and/or methodology suitable for analyzing performance behavior in sport (Anguera and Hernández-Mendo, 2013; Portell et al., 2015). Also, systematic observation is widely applicable and offers an optimal balance between rigor and flexibility. The wide scope of opportunities available for processing data derived from observation that supports the idea that purely observational studies should be considered as mixed methods research studies, even though they constitute a somewhat special case and do not follow traditional patterns (Anguera et al., 2017). Numerous tools have been created to observe and analyze the different situations of play that occur during a handball match (González, 2012; González et al., 2013; Sousa et al., 2014; Loffing et al., 2015; Helm et al., 2016; Jiménez-Salas and Hernández-Mendo, 2016) and beach handball (Morillo-Baro and Hernández-Mendo, 2015). In recent years, the analysis of decision-making has gained great interest with the publication of different investigations (Martín et al., 2013; Weigel et al., 2015). Specifically, in handball observations of the referees have showed the influence of playing time on the decision making in relation to the tactical means employed (Prudente et al., 2017) and the influence on the decisions depending on the area of the referee’s placement (Morillo-Baro et al., 2017).

The OM takes place in natural or habitual contexts, and is based on a scientific procedure that, depending on the objectives that are stated, expresses the occurrence of perceptible behaviors, for its subsequent registration through an observation tool built specifically using the appropriate parameters. The motor behavior can be studied through the most characteristic analysis of OM, the sequential analysis of delays and the analysis of polar coordinates (Anguera and Hernández-Mendo, 2013, 2014, 2015).

In recent years, the use of polar coordinates analysis as one of the most used analysis techniques has been developed in Sports Sciences (Morillo-Baro et al., 2015; Araújo et al., 2016; Castañer et al., 2016; Maneiro et al., 2019). Numerous investigations have used this analysis of polar coordinates focusing on different sports methods, a great number of which concerning football (Echeazarra et al., 2015; Castañer et al., 2017; Maneiro and Amatria, 2018), but also in other sports such as karate or basketball (Nunes et al., 2015; Riveiro-Bozada et al., 2016). In handball, the study was focused on the offensive situations of two against two, obtaining the different effective behavioral vectors (Sousa et al., 2014). Moreover, specifically, in beach handball the positional attack was studied, determining differences by gender (Morillo-Baro et al., 2015; Navarro et al., 2018).

Based on all this background on beach handball as an object of study, the objective is to study the relationship of some situational factors in the decisions of beach handball players through the analysis of polar coordinate.

Materials and Methods

For the development of the research, a nomothetic, punctual, and multidimensional design was chosen (Anguera et al., 2011). The sequence of positional attack that has been observed begins from the moment the specialist gains play until the possession is changed, being this, the unit of observation used.

Participants

Six teams have been observed for each category of the 24 that participated in total during the 2016 Senior Spanish Cup (M ± SD: age male = 26.67 ± 5.85; age female = 23.27 ± 5.74). The six teams have been chosen randomly with the intention of examining two observations from each team. There were 24 observation sessions, 12 in the male category and 12 in the female category, these being the number of sessions that were estimated necessary to be able to generalize with precision (Morillo-Baro and Hernández-Mendo, 2015). The analyzed matches are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Analyzed matches.

Instruments

The Royal Spanish Handball Federation (RFEBM) was in charge of recording the matches. The HOISAN computer software was used to perform the data recording and coding, the sequential analysis, the polar coordinate analysis and its vectorial representation (Hernández-Mendo et al., 2012). The tool used was already created and was ad hoc designed and had passed the quality tests of the data that are required in OM (Morillo-Baro and Hernández-Mendo, 2015). It consists of 11 criteria and 90 categories that correspond chronologically with the development of an attack on beach handball (Morillo-Baro and Hernández-Mendo, 2015). The criteria of the observation tool and the categories that make up each one of them are shown in Table 2. Next, Figure 1 shows the division of games spaces.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Observation instrument: criteria and corresponding categories and codes.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Division of game spaces. The numbering is related to the sense of attack.

Procedure

Data Quality

The correlations have been obtained, once the data has been collected, with the coefficients of Pearson, Spearman, Tau b of Kendall and the coefficient Phi besides the concordance index of Cohen Kappa for the complete session. This is a necessary process in OM since the observer must have the necessary guarantee on the quality of the data obtained (Anguera, 2003). The results are shown in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Intra- and inter-observer agreement.

As it can be observed, the Cohen Kappa index for this research exceeds 0.90 for both intra-observer (0.94) and inter-observer (0.91) reliability. Due to this, it is considered that there is high reliability, according to the values proposed by Gelfand and Hartmann (1975) or Landis and Koch (1977).

Generalizability Analysis

The Theory of Generalizability has been applied through the use of SAGT computer software (Hernández-Mendo et al., 2016). A design of two facets, categories and observers = C/O has been determined, which has served to determine the intra-observer and inter-observer concordance. The homogeneity of the categories has also been assessed, also using a two-faceted design, but with an O/C design. In the Theory of Generalizability, the definitions of reliability, validity and precision are unified. The Generalizability study consists basically of four phases: (1) Definition of the facets of study; (2) Analysis of variance of the scores obtained on the study facets; (3) Calculation of the error components; (4) Optimization of the Generalizability coefficients (Blanco-Villaseñor et al., 2014).

To determine inter-observer reliability, a two-faceted design (C = O) was established, as we previously mentioned. The results show that practically all the variability is associated with the facet categories (97.12%), being 0 for the facet observers and of 2.28% for the interaction of the facets categories/observers. The generalizability coefficients for this structure show a result of 0.98, which corresponds to excellent results.

The same design was taken to determine intra-observer reliability. The obtained results showed that almost all the variability was associated to the facet categories (97.99%) being 0 for the facet observers and resulting for the interaction of the facets categories/observers 2.01%. The generalizability coefficients for this design structure showed excellent results when it was obtained a value of 0.99.

Finally, the homogeneity of the categories was assessed, to do it, a two-faceted design (observers and categories = O/C) was chosen, thanks to this design, the degree of differentiation of the proposed categories was obtained. The generalization coefficients obtained for this design are zero (0.00), which means that the homogeneity of the categories is optimal in the sense of differentiators.

Polar Coordinate Analysis

Sackett (1980) was the precursor of this technique, which over the years was optimized by Anguera’s (1997) genuine technique, which allows a drastic reduction of data and a graphic representation of the vectors that determine the interrelations between the different categories that make up the proposed taxonomic system (Hernández-Mendo and Anguera, 1999; Gorospe and Anguera, 2000). This technique is supported by a sequential analysis of prospective delays (Sackett, 1980) and retrospective, with the genuine technique (Anguera, 1997) of the successive behaviors that occurred. The values obtained in the calculation of the conditioned probability will allow to obtain the Zsum parameter (Zsum = Σz/√n, where n is the number of delays) (Cochran, 1954). The distribution of this Zsum parameter has one www.frontiersin.org = 0 and one SD = 1. The interrelation map of behavior or map of polar coordinates (Gorospe and Anguera, 2000) is obtained from obtaining these values. It is necessary to determine the value of the vectors (they must be equal or greater than 1.96 to be considered significant) for the preparation of behavioral maps. The module or length of the radius is obtained by taking the square root of the sum of the square of the Zsum of the X (prospective) and the square of the Zsum of the Y (retrospective). The angle of the vector (φ) (which will depend on the quadrant where it is located) marking the nature of the relationship (Castellano and Hernández-Mendo, 2003). This angle (φ) is calculated as φ = Arc sine of Y/Radius.

The HOISAN computer program (Hernández-Mendo et al., 2012) allowed the coding and analysis of polar coordinates. Firstly, once the criterion behavior was selected, a sequential analysis was performed for each category of all the observations, obtaining the Z results for a range of delays −5 and 5. Once these values were obtained, the necessary calculations were made to obtain the parameters Zsum (prospective and retrospective), the assignment of the quadrant, the module, the angle and the transformed angle (AT) for the rest of the categories (Castellano and Hernández-Mendo, 2003):

Quadrant I [+, +]: The criterion behavior is excited with respect to the pairing behavior in retrospective and prospective perspective.

Quadrant II [−, +]: The criterion behavior has a relation with respect to the pairing of excitation in retrospective perspective and of inhibition in prospective perspective.

Quadrant III [−, −]: The criterion behavior has a relation with respect to the pairing of inhibition in retrospective and prospective perspective.

Quadrant IV [+,−]: The criterion behavior has a relationship with the behavior of arousal pairing in prospective perspective and inhibition in retrospective perspective.

The behaviors selected as criteria (focal) have been:

- GOAL2: this behavior is what refers to the final objective of the attack and, therefore, its appearance can be associated with a pattern of success in the decision made by the attacking team. In addition, it includes all the possibilities of scoring a goal of double value (flight, turn, throw of the goalkeeper and throw of 6 m).

- TURN and TECNF: both behaviors refer to the loss of possession of the ball by the team that attacks either by an own error or a forced error by the defensive action of the opposing team. Therefore, both are associated with an unsuccessful pattern in the decision made by the attacking team.

It has been decided to link these behaviors only with the criteria of the observation tool that are related to external factors or variables and that can modulate the decision making of the athletes: Minute, Score, Numerical Balance, Defensive System, and Duration.

Results

Then, it can observe the results obtained by analyzing polar coordinates for the selected behaviors as a criterion. Table 4 shows the pairing behaviors that are significantly linked when the criterion behavior is GOAL2 (per quadrant and category).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Relationships between focal behavior GOAL2 and conditional behaviors.

In quadrant I, where it is excited with respect to pairing behavior in retrospective and prospective perspective. In masculine category, it is associated with four pairing behaviors: the score of more than two goals in favor (M2WIN), the score of a goal in favor (1WIN), the defensive system of the opposing team (SRETR) and the minute fifteen of the second half (M15). While in the female category, it is associated with three pairing behaviors: the score of more than two goals in favor (M2WIN), the score of two goals in favor (2WIN) and the defensive system of the opposing team 3: 0 (S30). It emphasizes that in both categories the two behaviors that more intensity present in their relation refers to the score in favor of the attacking team.

In quadrant II, where the criterion behavior has a relationship with respect to the mate of excitation in retrospective perspective and inhibition in prospective perspective, in the male category, three pairing behaviors that belong to the same criterion, minute number, are excited, the minute one, six, and seven of the first half (M1, M6, and M7). Meanwhile, in the female category, there is only a pairing behavior that is the score of two goals against (2LOS).

In quadrant III, the criterion behavior has a relationship with respect to the pairing of inhibition in retrospective and prospective perspective. In both categories, two pairing behaviors are excited, and for both of them, the greater radius presents the score of more than two goals against (M2LOS).

Finally, in quadrant IV, the criterion behavior has a relationship with the behavior of excitation matching in prospective perspective and inhibition in retrospective perspective. In male category, it is associated with two behaviors of pairing: the score of two goals in favor (2WIN) and the minute eight of the second half (M18); whereas for feminine category only one appears, the duration of the attack between eleven and fifteen seconds (D1115).

Next, in Figures 2, 3, it can observe the graphical representation resulting from the analysis of polar coordinates for this criterion behavior.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Vector maps for focal behavior GOAL2 in male category.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Vector maps for focal behavior GOAL2 in female category.

Table 5 shows the results obtained when the criterion behavior is TURN.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Relationships between focal behavior TURN and conditional behaviors.

In quadrant I, in the two categories there are four pairing behaviors, in the male category the one with the highest radius is a player in inferiority (1INF), while in the female category, it is the defensive system of the opposing team two plus one (S2M1).

In quadrant II, four paring behaviors also appear in both categories, for the male category all pairing behaviors refer to the criterion minute number, being the one with the greatest intensity of relation the minute two of the second half (M12); On the other hand, in the female category, the score of more than two goals in favor (M2WIN) stands out.

In quadrant III, again the number of pairing behaviors that are excited in both categories coincides, two in this case. Being those that greater radius presents the score of more than two goals in favor (M2WIN), in male category, and the defensive system of the opposite team 3: 0 (S30).

To finish this criterion behavior, in quadrant IV, in the male category there are five pairing behaviors, where four belong to the criterion minute number, being the one with the highest radius the minute seven of the second half (M17), and one to the result of the score in a tie (TIE). While, in the female category, three pairing behaviors are excited, being the score of more than two goals against (M2LOS), the most intense in their relationship.

Next, in Figures 4, 5, it can observe the graphical representation resulting from the analysis of polar coordinates for this criterion behavior.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Vector maps for focal behavior TURN in male category.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Vector maps for focal behavior TURN in female category.

Table 6 shows the results obtained when the criterion behavior is TECNF.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Relationships between focal behavior TECNF and conditional behaviors.

For quadrant I, the results show that in male category there are three pairing behaviors, the minute corresponding to the golden goal in the first set (MGOAL1), the score more than two goals against (M2LOS) and the minute one of the first half (M1). In female category, four behaviors appear, highlighting that it coincides with the male category, the score of more than two goals against (M2LOS).

In quadrant II, three pairing behaviors are excited in the two categories, being the minute eight of the second half (M18) in the male category, and the score of one goal against (1LOS), in the female, those with the highest radius.

In quadrant III, in male category, only a pairing behavior appears, the score of more than two goals in favor (M2WIN) and in the female category there are two, the defensive system of the opposing team two plus one (S2M1) and the minute one of the second half (M11).

Finally, in quadrant IV, two pairing behaviors are excited in both categories, in the male, the minute three of the second half (M13) and the score of two goals against (2LOS), and in the female, the minute six and five of the first half (M6 and M5).

Next, in Figures 6, 7, it can observe the graphical representation resulting from the analysis of polar coordinates for this criterion behavior.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Vector maps for focal behavior TECNF in male category.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Vector maps for focal behavior TECNF in female category.

Discussion

The objective of this research was to study the relationship of some external factors with the decisions made by beach handball players through the analysis of polar coordinates. The results obtained showed that there are differences between the flow of behaviors that are significantly related in the male category compared to the female category, this agrees with the results obtained in the investigations of Morillo-Baro et al. (2015) and Navarro et al. (2018).

In the male category, the achievement of the double value goal excites the appearance of pairing behaviors related to the result of the favorable score, as shown in quadrants one, two and four. Therefore, it can be interpreted that the success in the decision making of the players is closely related to winning. A fact that agrees with the results obtained by Conejero et al. (2017). It is important to bear this in mind, since Morillo-Baro (2011) determined that 70% of the time of the match, the score takes place with differences between plus two and minus two points; however, it must be pointed out that this is a study carried out both with a population and in a different competition. Another noteworthy result is the pressing defensive system presented by the opposing team. This can be related to the fact that, faced with open defensive systems, players are able to make appropriate decisions and show that not only the specialist is capable of making decisions, a fact that provides a great tactical wealth to the collective game in this category.

Analyzing together the other two selected criterion behaviors, it stands out that both are associated with the unfavorable score. Players commit more pass or reception errors and more technical fouls when their team is losing, which could indicate an increase in pressure to reverse that situation resulting in more precipitation. They also agree in both that they occur more in the last minutes of the matches, when the match moment is critical (overtime and last 5 min of the game when the difference of points is less than six, Navarro et al., 2012). What is in line with other works that indicated a decrease in performance and accuracy in the decision-making in the last minutes of the match (Oñoro et al., 2015). It should also be noted that errors of pass or reception also occur more when the attacking team is in numerical inferiority, a fact that agrees with that stated by Aguilar et al. (2012).

With regard to the female category, the double value goal, as it happens in the male category, is directly related to the score in favor, therefore, it can be considered that this fact favors the decisions made by the players. On the other hand, this occurs before closed defensive systems, so, there is less pressure on the player that acts as a specialist and is able to make better decisions when constructing the static attack. It should be noted that in the female category, there is a great dependence on the actions of the specialist player (Morillo-Baro et al., 2015), therefore, she bears great responsibility when she comes to making decisions. Finally, it is noteworthy that the duration of the attack is between 11 and 15 s. A fact that does not offer information about whether the decisions made by the players are modulated by this criterion, but that it agrees with the fact that it lasts half a positional attack in beach handball (Morillo-Baro, 2011) and that it is interpreted as a reasonable construction of the static attack.

With regard to the error of pass or reception, the excitement of behaviors that are related to defensive pressing systems is observed, that is, the defense acts in a more active way reducing the spaces and times of action of the attacking team, besides limiting the action of the specialist. Another interesting result is the excitement of the duration of the attack less than 5 s; this is translated into a quick attack and probably it has been little elaborated. Therefore, it can be considered that this type of defense encourages players to make quick decisions under pressure through an immediate reading of the situations that arise, carrying out a negative influence on decision-making (Ruiz et al., 2014). For that reason, it could be interpreted that in front of pressing defensive systems the female teams make fast attacks that produce a greater number of errors in the pass or reception, diminishing the effectiveness of their actions.

Finally, it is directly related to the performance of technical faults in what has been previously determined critical moment of the game, therefore, it can see again how this fact modulates in a negative way the decisions made by the players. In addition, it is curiously the excitement of behavior one in superiority, this fact can be interpreted as the teams do not handle well the different decision scenarios that are created in the game and even being in superiority are not always able to make appropriate decisions. It cannot be interpreted clearly in this conduct criterion that the result influences in some way in the decision making.

By virtue of all explained until now, one might think that in both categories a correct decision-making is linked to having a favorable result on the score. In the male category, players are able to make correct decisions against any defensive system, while in the female category when they are pressured, the decisions made are reduced properly. The numerical balance has also shown its negative influence on the decision making of the athletes, in the male category when the attacking team is in inferiority and in the female category when it is in superiority. The duration of the attack is only significantly linked to the female category, showing that when they perform elaborate attacks they make a greater number of appropriate decisions than when they perform quick attacks with little elaboration, while in the male category a decrease in the success of decision-making has been observed in the last minutes of the matches.

There is hardly any published research on this modality, which leads to be cautious when conclusions are established about the results obtained. Since the research published up to now (Morillo-Baro et al., 2015; Navarro et al., 2018) had discussed the analysis of polar coordinates but they had not dealt with the analysis of decision making, which makes it difficult to compare these results with them. Another caution to consider is the limitation that is found when interpreting the decision-making process through the observation of the behaviors, ignoring other cognitive variables that surely influence. Finally, only Spanish players formed the sample, so this research has focused on the development of the game in this country.

All these results have highlighted the significant relationship between different external factors in the decisions made by beach handball players during the game. Thus, the use of observation and the technique of polar coordinates have been useful to discriminate between specific actions and behaviors, which provides data on the decisions of athletes during the competition. For all this, it would be of great interest to continue exploring this line of research on beach handball, creating new investigations with different focal behaviors to be able to establish the different patterns of the game in both categories.

Author Contributions

AH-M, VM-S, and RR: design of the work, acquisition, analysis, and interpretation of data for the work. JPM-B: acquisition, analysis, and interpretation of the data for the work. JAV-D: acquisition and analysis of the data for the work. All authors were involved in drafting, revising, and approving the final version of the manuscript, and also agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding

This study was supported by two grants (PSI2015-71947-REDT and DEP2015-66069-P (MINECO/FEDER, UE) from the Department of Research, Development and Innovation of the Spanish Ministry of the Economy and European Regional Development’s funds (FEDER) and by the Research Group of the Junta de Andalucia (SEJ444).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The handling Editor declared a past co-authorship with the authors AH-M and VM-S.

References

Aguilar, J., Chirosa, L. J., Martín, I., and And Chirosa, I. J. (2012). Influencia del número de jugadores/as en la toma de decisiones y el rendimiento en la enseñnanza del balonmano [Influence of the number of players in decision making and performance in the teaching of handball]. Rev. Cien. Dep. 8, 253–263.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (1997). “From prospective patterns in behavior to joint analysis with a retrospective perspective,” in Proceedings of the Colloque Sur Invitation «Méthodologie d’analyse des interactions sociales. (Paris: Université de la Sorbona).

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (2003). “La observación [The observation],” in Evaluación Psicológica. Concepto, Proceso y Aplicación en Las Áreas de Desarrollo y de la Inteligencia. ed. C. Moreno Rosset (Madrid: Sanz y Torres), 271–308.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., Hernández-Mendo, A., and Losada, J. L. (2011). Diseños observacionales: ajuste y aplicación en psicología del deporte [Observational designs: their suitability and application in sports psychology]. Cuad. Psico. Dep. 11, 63–76.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Camerino, O., Castañer, M., Sánchez-Algarra, P., and Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2017). The specificity of observational studies in physical activity and sports sciences: moving forward in mixed methods research and proposals for achieving quantitative and qualitative symmetry. Front. Psychol. 8:2196. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2013). La metodología observacional en el ámbito del deporte [Observational methodology in sport sciences]. Rev. Cien. Dep. 9, 135–160.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2014). Metodología observacional y psicología del deporte: estado de la cuestión [Observational methodology and sports psychology: state of affairs]. Rev. Psicol. Dep. 23, 103–119.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2015). Técnicas de análisis en estudios observacionales en ciencias del deporte [Data analysis techniques in observational studies in sport sciences]. Cuad. Psico. Dep. 15, 13–30. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232015000100002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Araújo, D., Teques, P., Hernández-Mendo, A., Reigal, R., and Anguera, M. T. (2016). La toma de decisión, ¿es una conducta observable?: discusión sobre diferentes perspectivas teóricas utilizadas en el estudio del rendimiento deportivo [Decision-making, is it an observable behaviour?: discussion of contrasting theoretical perspectives in use in the study of sport performance]. Cuad. Psico. Dep. 16, 183–196.

Google Scholar

Belančić, Z. (2005). Rukomet na Pijesku. [Beach handball. In Croatian.]. Zagre: Zagrebački Rukometni Savez.

Google Scholar

Blanco-Villaseñor, A., Castellano, J., Hernández-Mendo, A., Sánchez-López, C. R., and Usabiaga, O. (2014). Aplicación de la TG en el deporte para el estudio de la fiabilidad, validez y estimación de la muestra [Application of the generalizability theory in sport to study the validity, reliability and estimation of samples]. Rev. Psicol. Dep. 23, 131–137.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Carling, C. (2011). Influence of opposition team formation on physical and skill-related performance in a professional soccer team. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 11, 155–164. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2010.499972

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castañer, M., Barreira, D., Camerino, O., Anguera, M. T., Canton, A., and Hileno, R. (2016). Goal scoring in soccer: a polar coordinate analysis of motor skills used by Lionel Messi. Front. Psychol. 7:806. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castañer, M., Barreira, D., Camerino, O., Anguera, M. T., Fernandes, T., and Hileno, R. (2017). Mastery in goal scoring, T-pattern detection, and polar coordinate analysis of motor skills used by Lionel Messi and Cristiano Ronaldo. Front. Psychol. 8:741. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellano, J., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2003). El análisis de coordenadas polares para la estimación de relaciones en la interacción motriz en fútbol [Polar coordinates analysis to estimate the relationships in the motor interaction in soccer]. Psicothema 15, 569–579.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cochran, W. G. (1954). Some methods for streghning the common χ2 test. Biometrics 10, 417–451. doi: 10.2307/3001616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conejero, M., Claver, F., Fernández-Echeverría, C., Gil-Arias, A., and Moreno, M. P. (2017). Toma de decisiones y rendimiento en las acciones de juego intermedias y finalistas en voleibol, en sets con diferente resultado [Decision-making and performance in intermediate and terminal actions in volleyball according to the set result]. RETOS. Nuevas Tendencias en Educación Física, Deporte y Recreación 31, 28–33.

Google Scholar

Conejero, M., Claver, F., Fernández-Echeverría, C., and Moreno, M. P. (2018). Relationship between decision-making and performance in game actions in volleyball. Eur. J. Human Move. 39, 82–95.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Echeazarra, I., Castellano, J., Usabiaga, O., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2015). Diferencias en el uso estratégico del espacio en categorías infantil y cadete de fútbol: una aplicación del análisis de coordenadas polares [Differences in the strategic use of space in under 14 and under 16 Soccer: a polar coordinate analysis]. Cuad. Psico. Dep. 15, 169–180. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232015000100017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelfand, D. M., and Hartmann, D. P. (1975). Child Behavior Analysis and Therapy. Michigan, MI: Pergamon Press.

Google Scholar

Gil-Arias, A., Claver, F., Alías, P., Moreno, M., García-González, L., and Del Villar, F. (2012). Influencia del rendimiento en el estilo de toma de decisiones de jugadores de fútbol [Infuence of performance on decisional style in soccer players]. Rev. Move. Hum. 3, 35–44.

Google Scholar

Gil-Arias, A., Moreno, M., Claver, F., Moreno, A., and Del Villar, F. (2016). Manipulation of the task constraints in physical education: a proposal from nonlinear pedagogy. 22–27.

Google Scholar

González, A. (2012). Análisis de la Eficacia del Contraataque en Balonmano como Elemento de Rendimiento Deportivo [Analysis of fast break’s efficiency in handball as an element of sport performance]. Tesis doctoral. Universidad de León: León

Google Scholar

González, A., Botejara, J., Puñales, L., Trejo, A., and Ruy, E. (2013). Análisis de la finalización del ataque en partidos igualados en balonmano de alto nivel mediante coordenadas polares [Analysis of the finalization of the attack in equalized games of high level handball using polar coordinates]. Rev. Cien. Dep. 9, 71–89.

Google Scholar

Gorospe, G., and Anguera, M. T. (2000). Modificación de la técnica clásica de coordenadas polares mediante un desarrollo distinto de la retrospectividad: aplicación al tenis [Retrospectivity in polar coordinates analysis: application to tennis]. Psicothema 12, 279–282.

Google Scholar

Helm, F., Reiser, M., and Munzert, J. (2016). Domain-specific and unspecific reaction times in experienced team handball goalkeepers and novices. Front. Psychol. 7:882. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández-Mendo, A., and Anguera, M. T. (1999). “Aportaciones de análisis de coordenadas polares a los deportes de equipo [Contributions of polar coordinate analysis to team sports],” in La Psicología del Deporte en España al final del milenio. ed. F. Guillén (Las Palmas: Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria), 169–175.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hernández-Mendo, A., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., Pastrana, J. L., Morales-Sánchez, V., and Ramos-Pérez, F. J. (2016). SAGT: aplicación informática para análisis de generalizabilidad [SAGT: new software for generalizability analysis]. Rev. Iberoam. Psicol. Ejerc. Dep. 11, 77–89.

Google Scholar

Hernández-Mendo, A., López-López, J. A., Castellano, J., Morales-Sánchez, V., and Pastrana, J. L. (2012). Hoisan 1.2: programa informático para uso en metodología observacional [Hoisan 1.2: computer program for use in observational methodology]. Cuad. Psico. Dep. 12, 55–78. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232012000100006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiménez-Salas, J., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2016). Análisis de la calidad del dato y generalizabilidad de un sistema de observación del contraataque en el balonmano de élite [Analysis of data quality and generalizability observing system counter in elite handball]. Rev. Cien. Dep. 12, 31–44.

Google Scholar

Landis, J. R., and Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33, 159–174. doi: 10.2307/2529310

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levi, H. R., and Jackson, R. C. (2018). Contextual factors influencing decision making: perceptions of professional soccer players. Psychol. Sport. Exerc. 37, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loffing, F., Sölter, F., Hagemann, N., and Strauss, B. (2015). Accuracy of outcome anticipation, but not gaze behavior, differs against left-and right-handed penalties in team-handball goalkeeping. Front. Psychol. 6:1820. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maneiro, R., and Amatria, M. (2018). Polar coordinate analysis of relationships with teammates, areas of the pitch, and dynamic play in soccer: a study of Xabi Alonso. Front. Psychol. 9:389. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maneiro, R., Amatria, M., and Anguera, M. T. (2019). Dynamics of Xavi Hernández’s game: a vectorial study through polar coordinate analysis. Proc. Inst. Mech. Eng. Part P J. Sports Eng. Technol. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232015000100005 [Epub ahead of Print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martín, I., González, A., Cavalcanti, L. A., Chirosa, L. J., and Aguilar, J. (2013). Fiabilidad y optimización del programa PROTODEBA v 1.0 para la observación de la Toma de decisiones en balonmano [Reliability and optimization of PROTODEBA v 1.0 software for the observation of the decision making in handball]. Cuad. Psico. Dep. 13, 63–70. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232013000100007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morillo-Baro, J. P. (2011). Beach Handball: Analysis of Positional Attack in the Men. Vienna: EHF Web Periodical.

Google Scholar

Morillo-Baro, J. P., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2015). Análisis de la calidad del dato de un instrumento para la observación del ataque en balonmano playa [Analysis of quality data a tool for observing the attack in beach handball]. Rev. Iberoam. Psicol. Ejerc. Dep. 10, 15–22.

Google Scholar

Morillo-Baro, J. P., Reigal, R. E., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2015). Análisis del ataque posicional de balonmano playa masculino y femenino mediante coordenadas polares [Analysis of positional attack in beach handball male and female with polar coordinates]. Rev. Int. Cien. Dep. 11, 226–244. doi: 10.5232/ricyde2015.04103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morillo-Baro, J. P., Reigal, R. E., Hernández-Mendo, A., Montaña, A., and Morales-Sánchez, V. (2017). Decision-making by handball referees: design of an ad hoc observation instrument and polar coordinate analysis. Front. Psychol. 8:1842. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01842

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarro, A., Morillo-Baro, J. P., Reigal, R. E., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2018). Polar coordinate analysis in the study of positional attacks in beach handball. Int. J. Perf. Anal. Sport 18, 151–167. doi: 10.1080/24748668.2018.1460052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarro, R. M., Gómez, M. A., Lorenzo, J., Lorenzo, A., and Jiménez-Sáiz, S. (2012). An analysis of home advantage effect affecting the final outcome of the critical moments in basketball. Rev. Esp. Educ. Fís. Dep. 396, 49–64.

Google Scholar

Nunes, H., Iglesias, X., Daza, G., Irrutia, A., Caparrós, T., and Anguera, M. T. (2015). Influencia del pick and roll en el juego de ataque en baloncesto de alto nivel [The influence of pick and roll in attacking play in top-level basketball]. Cuad. Psico. Dep 16, 129–142.

Google Scholar

Oñoro, M. A., Gómez-Ruano, M. A., Jiménez-Sáiz, S., and Calvo, S. (2015). Análisis del drop en baloncesto a través del estudio de los tiros libres [Analysis of drop in basketball trough the study of free throws]. Kronos 14, 2.

Google Scholar

Ostoić, S., and Ohnjec, K. (2015). 2015 Beach Handball European Championships Qualitative Analysis. Vienna: EHF Web Periodical.

Google Scholar

Plano-Clark, V. L., and Sanders, K. (2015). “The use of visual displays in mixed methods research,” in Use of Visual Displays in Research and Testing. eds M. T. McCrudden, G. Schraw, and C. W. Buckendahl (Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing, Inc), 177–206.

Google Scholar

Portell, M., Anguera, M. T., Hernández-Mendo, A., and Jonsson, G. K. (2015). Quantifying biopsychosocial aspects in everyday contexts: an integrative methodological approach from the behavioral sciences. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 8, 153–160. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S82417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prudente, J., Sousa, D., Sequeira, P., López-López, J. A., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2017). Analyzing the influence of playing time and partial score on the tactical behavior in the duel 2 vs 2 in the offensive process in handball, using the polar coordinates technique. Ann. Psico. 33, 515–529. doi: 10.6018/analesps.33.3.271071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riveiro-Bozada, A., García-García, O., Serrano-Gómez, V., Morales-Sánchez, V., López-López, J. A., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2016). Influencia del nivel de competición en las acciones técnicas de punto realizadas en Shiai Kumite femenino de karate. Análisis de coordenadas polares [Influence the level of competition in technical actions performed point female Shiai Kumite -karate]. Cuad. Psico. Dep. 16, 51–68.

Google Scholar

Ruiz, L. M., García, V., Palomo, M., Navia, J. A., and Miñano, J. (2014). Inteligencia contextual y pericia en el fútbol [Contextual intelligence and expertise in soccer]. Rev. Int. Med. Cien. Activ. Fís. Dep. 14,307–317.

Google Scholar

Sackett, G. P. (1980). “Lag sequential analysis as a data reduction technique in social interaction research,” in Exceptional Infant. Phychosocial Risks in Infant-Environment Transactions. eds D. B. Sawin, R. C. Hawkins, L. O. Walker, and J. H. Penticuff (New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel), 300–340.

Google Scholar

Sarmento, H., Clemente, P. M., Araújo, D., Davids, K., McRobert, A., and Figueiredo, A. (2017). What performance analysts need to know about research trends in association football (2012-2016). Sports Med. 48, 799–836. doi: 10.1007/s40279-017-0836-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sousa, D., Prudente, J., Sequeira, P., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2014). Análise da qualidade dos dados de um instrumento para observação do 2vs2 no andebol [Analysis of 2vs2 situations in men’s european handball championship 2012: application of polar coordinates technique]. Rev. Iberoam. Psicol. Ejerc. Dep. 9, 173–190.

Google Scholar

Weigel, P., Raab, M., and Wollny, R. (2015). Tactical decision making in team sports-a model of cognitive processes. Int. J. Sports Sci. 5,128–138.

Google Scholar

Keywords: mixed methods, systematic observation, polar coordinates, beach handball, decision making

Citation: Vázquez-Diz JA, Morillo-Baro JP, Reigal RE, Morales-Sánchez V and Hernández-Mendo A (2019) Contextual Factors and Decision-Making in the Behavior of Finalization in the Positional Attack in Beach Handball: Differences by Gender Through Polar Coordinates Analysis. Front. Psychol. 10:1386. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01386

Received: 24 December 2018; Accepted: 28 May 2019;
Published: 13 June 2019.

Edited by:

Mariona Portell, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Spain

Reviewed by:

Alfonso Valero-Valenzuela, University of Murcia, Spain
Luca Paolo Ardigò, University of Verona, Italy
Constantino Arce, University of Santiago de Compostela, Spain

Copyright © 2019 Vázquez-Diz, Morillo-Baro, Reigal, Morales-Sánchez and Hernández-Mendo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Antonio Hernández-Mendo, mendo@uma.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.