Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 29 April 2019
Sec. Health Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Implications of Weight Bias Internalization View all 10 articles

Weight Bias Internalization as an Embodied Process: Understanding How Obesity Stigma Gets Under the Skin

  • 1University of Leicester, Leicester, United Kingdom
  • 2Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care East Midlands, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United Kingdom
  • 3University of York, York, United Kingdom

Introduction

This Opinion Article contributes to this Special Issue a supportive critique of the weight bias internalization analysis. The explicit aim is to broaden the ways in which “internalization” is currently defined and analyzed in research on weight bias and to encourage interdisciplinary research endeavors to increase our understanding of its implications. Both authors are sociologists who understand and analyse the individual condition as embodied1. In short, we are interested in how and in what ways the social world “gets under the skin” and thus has psychosomatic implications. It is for this reason why, despite commending much of the scholarship on weight bias internalization and accepting the validity of the research findings, we feel it necessary to challenge the current application of the “internalization” terminology. Our argument is that weight bias internalization research is limited in that it is largely disembodied. This is considered problematic because to fully understand the implications of weight bias internalization (the express concern of this Special Issue), it is necessary to appreciate both how and in what ways it gets under the skin.

Weight Bias: Discriminatory, Detrimental and Counter-Productive

Weight bias and obesity stigma are terms commonly used synonymously in research literature. Both describe a cultural framing that emphasizes the role of individual behavior or “lifestyle” as the cause and cure of “obesity” (Crossley, 2004) and forefronts an economic rationale to moralize and individualize the issue (i.e., implying that those who are classified as “overweight” and/or “obese”2 are irresponsible individuals who are placing an avoidable burden on national health systems). As evidenced elsewhere (Williams and Annandale, 2018), our analysis sits within the tradition of Critical Obesity Studies recognizing that (i) obesity and overweight are socially constructed clinical categories—established via the Body Mass Index (BMI)—which offer crude and flawed indicators of health and (ii) the metabolically healthy but obese phenotype has been demonstrated empirically with physical activity and diet (independent of weight) acting as more reliable indicators of overall health status than BMI (e.g., Ortega et al., 2013). We endorse Rich's (2011, p. 16) argument that dominant cultural framings of people of higher weights “not only position individuals as blameworthy, but moralize and decontextualize health inequalities by glossing over the social and structural contexts.” Because obesity has been consistently shown to follow social gradients in wealth and inequality (e.g., Pickett et al., 2005; Krueger and Reither, 2015; Baker, 2018), this depiction is often tantamount to victim blaming. Even outside of the influence of social inequalities, the logic of motivating people to comply with official health guidelines by moralizing behaviors and promoting the internationalization of weight-based stigma is highly questionable (see e.g., LeBesco, 2011; Täuber et al., 2018). Therefore, the implications of weight bias/stigma are important from both health promotion and social justice perspectives and have thus predictably become significant research inquiries.

The study of weight bias/stigma is a truly multidisciplinary field and despite significant disciplinary differences, the research findings are characterized by coherence. Review articles have concluded that not only is weight bias/stigma an ineffective means by which to reduce the incidence of obesity but that it actually promotes weight gain and has additional iatrogenic consequences (Puhl and Heuer, 2009; Brewis, 2014; Rees et al., 2014; Phelan et al., 2015). Weight bias/stigma has been shown to have significant detrimental mental health and behavioral implications, e.g., increasing vulnerability to stress, depression, low self-esteem, poor body image, maladaptive eating behaviors, and exercise avoidance (Hayward et al., 2018; Tomiyama et al., 2018; Tomiyama, 2019). Tomiyama et al., 2018 review outlines the rapidly growing evidence base that indicates the detrimental impact of weight bias/stigma cannot simply be explained away by higher bodyweights leading to poorer health and/or greater likelihood of perceiving weight-related discrimination. Rather, the review highlights that negative characterization of people classified as overweight/obese has led to simply perceiving oneself as overweight to have a prospective association with biological markers of poorer health. Research is in its infancy but findings indicate that the biological implications of weight bias/stigma range from increased secretion of the fat-storage promoting stress hormone cortisol (Jackson and Steptoe, 2018) to higher risk of developing dementia (Sutin et al., 2018); the former linked to the common practice of yo-yo dieting/weight cycling and its associated adverse health effects (Tomiyama, 2014; Madigan et al., 2018). Additionally, weight bias/stigma has been shown to translate into structural inequities, e.g., in employment, healthcare, and education, which reproduces the social disadvantage that drives the uneven distribution of obesity incidence throughout the socioeconomic spectrum (Puhl and Heuer, 2009; Tomiyama, 2019).

Puhl and Heuer (2010) argue that to improve public health it is essential that common societal assumptions that perpetuate weight bias/stigma are challenged and that the deleterious repercussions of weight bias/stigma inform the ways in which obesity is popularly framed and understood. Some will be unconcerned with the ethics of stigma and the imperative to reduce human suffering. However, they may be convinced of the need for change by the evidence demonstrating the ineffective, counter-productive and detrimental outcomes of weight bias/stigma. Consequently, it is vital that researchers analyse the processes through which the sociocultural phenomenon of weight bias/stigma affects people's health; that is, how the social (weight bias/stigma) gets under the skin. Or put another way, the processes by which external social factors are internalized and the psychosomatic consequences that follow. Despite the necessity of this inquiry, when moving from research on weight bias/stigma more generally to the more specific inquiry of weight bias internalization the field is limited in what it can offer. Therefore, it is necessary to critically evaluate the theoretical and methodological traditions that define the study of weight bias internalization.

Getting Under the Skin: Internalization as Embodiment

Weight bias internalization has been defined as the “internalization of negative weight stereotypes and subsequent self-disparagement” (Pearl and Puhl, 2018, p. 1141). In short, over time people who are classified as overweight or obese come to accept and endorse derogatory and discriminatory cultural depictions of people who are classified as overweight or obese (i.e., as irresponsible, gluttonous, and lazy). A systematic review of the literature has demonstrated that the empirical study of this phenomenon is in its infancy (Pearl and Puhl, 2018). But early findings illustrate a significant detrimental impact on mental (e.g., depression, anxiety, body dissatisfaction) and physical health (e.g., metabolic syndrome, weight cycling) as well as related health behaviors through rejection of dietary advice, binge eating and exercise avoidance (Ratcliffe and Ellison, 2015; Jackson and Steptoe, 2017; Puhl and Himmelstein, 2018). These are useful and important findings, but in such studies internalization is defined and analyzed predominantly as a cognitive process. Indeed, the Weight Bias Internalization Scale is “a measure of belief in social stereotypes relating to obesity and negative self-evaluations due to one's weight” (Durso and Latner, 2008, p. 81). This is an unnecessarily restrictive definition of “internalization” that unhelpfully narrows the parameters of inquiry.

While finding that self-blame and behaviors considered detrimental to one's health result from both self-endorsement of anti-fat attitudes and accepting weight-based stereotypes is certainly one element of weight bias internalization (i.e., a psychological component which subsequently influences patterns of behavior), it is only that: one component of a far more complex process of internalization. However, this component has come to define the field. This helps explain why the only systematic review of this literature (Pearl and Puhl, 2018) found that while there is evidence of strong, negative relationships between weight bias internalization and mental health outcomes, few studies have examined the relationship between weight bias internalization and physical health. Succinctly put, the predominance of psychological analysis is skewing the field.

On the basis of existing research it is fair to argue that in the study of weight bias internalization cognition has taken precedence over other interrelated processes. Given that weight bias has been shown to have social and biological implications, the delimited focus in the field on a cognitive process seems unjustified. The predominantly cognitive component that is currently most commonly referred to and researched as weight bias internalization could perhaps more accurately be referred to as weight bias “endorsement,” “acceptance,” or “agreement.” This would better reflect its partial (albeit important) role in the broader biopsychosocial process of weight bias internalization—which could alternatively be defined as: detrimental psychosomatic responses caused by exposure to discrimination on the basis of negative stereotypes about people of higher weights. An interdisciplinary approach to understanding weight bias internalization as an embodied phenomenon would go some way to painting a more complete picture of the implications of weight bias internalization.

Crossley (2006, p. 2) succinctly summarizes “reflexive embodiment” by explaining that “human bodies exist in two dimensions. We are our bodies (being) but sometimes perceive them as an object that we possess (having).” Bodies are both subjectively (personally) and objectively (materially) experienced. This is how weight bias/stigma (an external/social factor) can make bodies feel particular ways through psychosomatic experience and initiate changes in biological markers of health. For Freund (2011), this is because mind and body do not operate separately but rather we are all “mindbodies” with the potential to self-initiate health states on a conscious-unconscious level. It is this interconnected and dependent relationship between mental and physical, cognitive and carnal, which accounts for the well-established “placebo effect,” but it also explains how the social gets under the skin. For instance, on top of the physical repercussions of related behaviors (e.g., maladaptive eating behaviors and exercise avoidance), this is how the depression someone may experience as a result of being/having a stigmatized body can detrimentally impact their physical health (through, for instance, the secretion of cortisol and associated effects—see e.g., Tomiyama, 2019). An embodied analysis of weight bias internalization needs to be inclusive of, but to extend beyond cognitive processes and their behavioral consequences by appreciating the biopsychosocial mechanisms through which weight bias is internalized and has consequence. That is, how the social (weight bias/stigma) comes to have material implications (detrimental health impacts).

Social scientists, particularly those employing a feminist analysis, have been at the forefront of establishing an embodied analysis of obesity (e.g., Bordo, 1993; Murray, 2012; Warin, 2015; Lupton, 2018). This work demonstrates the limits of approaching the study of obesity as primarily a biological, psychological or social and political phenomenon instead highlighting the inextricable interplay of these constituent factors and strengthens the analysis, methodology, and ethics of weight bias/stigma research. For example, the research trend of putting thin people in “fat suits” has been critiqued by Meadows et al. (2017) who highlight the impossibility of replicating the physiological, affective, and behavioral responses to weight stigma outside of the embodied experiences of people of higher weights. Furthermore, it is increasingly appreciated that the physical body does not simply respond to external social forces, but dynamically engages with them in an iterative process (see e.g., Barad, 2007). For example, in her book Gut Feminism Wilson (2015) explores the “biological enactments” of bulimia—a condition not uncommon amongst people categorized as overweight and obese (Brownwell and Walsh, 2017)—to think anew about the mind-body relationship. As she explains, especially where girls and women are concerned, bulimia is commonly accounted for as an ideational response to living within patriarchal societies; a visceral response as the individual “wills” the food back up by induced vomiting (antiperistalsis). Wilson (2015, p. 62) argues that in chronic cases, “organic thought” occurs as binging and vomiting become compulsive and not necessarily tied to consciously meaningful and analyzable events in a person's social world. Thus, she argues that the “organism itself is beginning to think” as distress and anger become “primarily organic.”

We have used an embodied analysis in a study of three weight-loss groups in England to highlight how obesity stigma can confuse people's objective and subjective experiences of their bodies (Williams and Annandale, 2018). Confusion was primarily evident on occasions when group members felt heavier after engaging in negatively moralized behaviors associated with weight-gain but this “weight” did not register on the weighing scales. We conceptualize this as the weight of expectation which we take as illustrative of how the morality that characterizes weight-management within a culture that is hostile to those categorized as overweight or obese gets under the skin and is felt in the flesh. An embodied analysis allowed us to pay attention to and take seriously an implication of weight bias that would otherwise be unobservable via the Weight Bias Internalization Scale. Analyzing the embodiment of obesity stigma also allowed us to demonstrate how weight-loss group participants came to ascribe their experiences of sensations deriving from physiological responses to exercise (e.g., sweating, delayed onset muscle soreness) with positive moral and social significance. These carnal cues played an important role in their attempts to negotiate obesity stigma and illustrate how the effects of weight bias extend beyond verbal or written endorsement of discriminatory anti-fat attitudes to include interpretations of physiological processes and bodily sensation. These findings are important to the analysis of the implications of weight bias internalization because they deepen understanding of the lived experience of being stigmatized as well as how and why obesity stigma is an inappropriate and ineffective means of promoting weight-loss and health. However, they are not and cannot be researched within the parameters, and via the methods, established for the study of weight bias internalization. The same is true of the biological markers of poorer health attributable to weight bias internalization.

We contend that an embodied analysis would go some way to necessarily extending the analysis of weight bias internalization but, of course, we do not claim to have all the answers, as no one discipline can. Instead, we argue for an opening up of the definition and analysis of weight bias internalization so as to fully appreciate and measure the implications of weight bias/stigma. This is necessarily an interdisciplinary endeavor. In a demonstration of the potential utility of interdisciplinarity in this field, Tomiyama (2014) took a biopsychosocial approach to create a generative model to explain how and why weight-based stigma is counter-productive: the cyclic obesity/weight-based stigma (COBWEBS) model. The model depicts a “vicious cycle”—with people getting “caught” in COBWEBS—wherein weight-based stigma is characterized as a stressor that begets weight gain through increased eating and other biobehavioral mechanisms (e.g., elevated secretion of cortisol and associated fat storage). This appears to us to be a useful model through which to engage with revealing one of the biopsychosocial implications of weight bias internalization, but to test its fidelity requires the kind of interdisciplinary research that is as yet outside the scope of current definitions and measures used in this field of study.

Conclusion

The dominant definition of weight bias internalization and the associated methods for measuring its effects are limited and thus risk rendering its full impact immeasurable to those working in the field. Relatedly, they elevate mind and rational decision making over body and psychosomatic sensation. Ironically this has the effect that current analysis reveals very little about the process of internalization understood more broadly as a biopsychosocial process—that is, how weight bias quite literally gets under the skin. Presently a preoccupation with internalization as a psychological process has generated evidence that tells us far more about changes in states of mental health and behavioral outcomes than about biological effects and the lived experience of obesity stigma as mediated through the body. This is not to denigrate this contribution, on the contrary, it has greatly advanced understanding of the implications of weight bias. However, developing and strengthening the evidence base now relies upon broadening the definition of internalization to foster the interdisciplinarity necessary to realize the biopsychosocial analysis required to fully comprehend the implications of weight bias.

Author Contributions

OW and EA both contributed more or less evenly to the formation of the theoretical argument and the writing of the manuscript.

Funding

The writing up of this research was supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care West (NIHR CLAHRC West) and East Midlands (NIHR CLAHRC East Midlands). The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR, or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Both authors would like to thank the reviewers for their constructive and prompt comments.

Footnotes

1. ^We have defined embodiment elsewhere as depicting “the fusion of the mind and body in a process whereby the society and culture within which we live are experienced in bodily terms and internalized by us: they are embodied” (Williams and Annandale, 2014, p. 1868).

2. ^We initially place these terms in inverted commas in recognition that their meaning and use are challenged. In this article these terms are not referred to uncritically but are used to be consistent with the wider literature.

References

Baker, C. (2018). Obesity Statistics: Briefing Paper. London: House of Commons Library.

Barad, K. (2007). Meeting the Universe Halfway. Durham: Duke University Press.

Google Scholar

Bordo, S. (1993). Unbearable Weight: Western Culture and the Body. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Google Scholar

Brewis, A. A. (2014). Stigma and the perpetuation of obesity. Soc. Sci. Med. 118, 152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brownwell, K. D., and Walsh, B. T. (2017). Eating Disorders and Obesity. 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Guildford Press.

Google Scholar

Crossley, N. (2004). Fat is a sociological issue: obesity rates in late modern, body-conscious societies. Soc. Theory Health 2, 222–253. doi: 10.1057/palgrave.sth.8700030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crossley, N. (2006). Reflexive Embodiment in Contemporary Society. Berkshire: Open University Press.

Google Scholar

Durso, L. E., and Latner, J. D. (2008). Understanding self-directed stigma: development of the weight bias internalization scale. Obesity 16, S80–S86. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freund, P. (2011). Embodying psychosocial health inequalities: bringing back materiality and bioagency. Soc. Theory Health 9, 59–70. doi: 10.1057/sth.2010.5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, L. E., Vartanian, L. R., and Pinkus, R. T. (2018). Weight stigma predicts poorer psychological well-being through internalized weight bias and maladaptive coping responses. Obesity 26, 755–761. doi: 10.1002/oby.22126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. E., and Steptoe, A. (2017). Association between perceived weight discrimination and physical activity: a population-based study among English middle-aged and older adults. BMJ Open. 7:e014592. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-014592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. E., and Steptoe, A. (2018). Obesity, perceived weight discrimination, and hair cortisol: a population-based study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 98, 67–73. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krueger, P. M., and Reither, E. N. (2015). Mind the gap: race/ethnic and socioeconomic disparities in obesity. Curr. Diab. Rep. 15:95. doi: 10.1007/s11892-015-0666-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeBesco, K. (2011). Neoliberalism, public health, and the moral perils of fatness. Crit. Public Health 21, 153–164. doi: 10.1080/09581596.2010.529422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupton, D. (2018). Fat. Abingdon: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Madigan, C. D., Pavey, T., Daley, A. J., Jolly, K., and Brown, W. J. (2018). Is weight cycling associated with adverse health outcomes? A cohort study. Prevent. Med. 108, 47–52. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meadows, A., Daníelsdóttir, S., Calogero, R., and O'Reilly, C. (2017). Why fat suits do not advance the scientific study of weight stigma. Obesity 25:275. doi: 10.1002/oby.21742

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, S. (2012). ‘Editor’s introduction: rethinking fatness - a critical dialogue'. Femin. Psychol. 22, 287–289. doi: 10.1177/0959353512445354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortega, F. B., Lee, D. C., Katzmarzyk, P. T., Ruiz, J. R., Sui, X., Church, T. S., et al. (2013). The intriguing metabolically healthy but obese phenotype: cardiovascular prognosis and role of fitness. Eur. Heart J. 34, 389–397. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehs174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearl, R. L., and Puhl, R. M. (2018). Weight bias internalization and health: a systematic review. Obes. Rev. 19, 1141–1163. doi: 10.1111/obr.12701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phelan, S. M., Burgess, D. J., Yeazel, M. W., Hellerstedt, W. L., and Ryn, M. (2015). Impact of weight bias and stigma on quality of care and outcomes for patients with obesity. Obesity Rev. 16, 319–326. doi: 10.1111/obr.12266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickett, K. E., Kelly, S., Brunner, E., Lobstein, T., and Wilkinson, R. G. (2005). Wider income gaps, wider waistbands? An ecological study of obesity and income inequality. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 59, 670–674. doi: 10.1136/jech.2004.028795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puhl, R., and Himmelstein, M. (2018). Weight bias internalization among adolescents seeking weight loss: implications for eating behaviours and parental communication. Front. Psychol. 9:2271. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puhl, R. M., and Heuer, C. A. (2009). The stigma of obesity: a review and update. Obesity 17, 941–964. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puhl, R. M., and Heuer, C. A. (2010). Obesity stigma: important considerations for public health. Am. J. Public Health 100, 1019–1028. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.159491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratcliffe, D., and Ellison, N. (2015). Obesity and internalized weight stigma: A formulation model for an emerging psychological problem. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 43, 239–252. doi: 10.1017/S1352465813000763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rees, R. W., Caird, J., Dickson, K., Vigurs, C., and Thomas, J. (2014). 'It's on your conscience all the time': a systematic review of qualitative studies examining views on obesity among young people aged 12-18 years in the UK. BMJ Open 4:e004404. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-004404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rich, E. (2011). ‘I see her being obesed!’: public pedagogy, reality media and the obesity crisis. Health 15, 3–21. doi: 10.1177/1363459309358127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutin, A. R., Stephan, Y., Robinson, E., Daly, M., and Terracciano, A. (2018). Perceived weight discrimination and risk of incident dementia. Int. J. Obesity 1. doi: 10.1038/s41366-018-0211-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Täuber, S., Gausel, N., and Flint, S. W. (2018). Weight bias internalization: the maladaptive effects of moral condemnation on intrinsic motivation. Front. Psychol. 9:1836. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomiyama, A. J. (2014). Weight stigma is stressful. A review of evidence for the cyclic obesity/weight-based stigma model. Appetite 82, 8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.06.108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomiyama, A. J. (2019). Stress and obesity. Ann. Rev. Psychol 70, 703–718.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tomiyama, A. J., Carr, D., Granberg, E. M., Major, B., Robinson, E., Sutin, A. R., et al. (2018). How and why weight stigma drives the obesity 'epidemic' and harms health. BMC Med. 16:123. doi: 10.1186/s12916-018-1116-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warin, M. (2015). 'Material feminism, obesity science and the limits of discursive critique'. Body Soc. 21, 48–76. doi: 10.1177/1357034X14537320

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, O., and Annandale, E. (2018). Obesity, stigma and reflexive embodiment: feeling the ‘weight’ of expectation. Health 14:1363459318812007. doi: 10.1177/1363459318812007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, O. S, and Annandale, E. (2014). “Embodied subjectivity,” in Encyclopedia of Quality of Life and Well-Being Research, ed A. C. Michalos (Dordrecht: Springer), 1868–1871. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-0753-5_2908

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, E. (2015). Gut Feminism. Durham: Duke University Press. doi: 10.1215/9780822375203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: obesity, embodiment, stigma, internalization, health

Citation: Williams O and Annandale E (2019) Weight Bias Internalization as an Embodied Process: Understanding How Obesity Stigma Gets Under the Skin. Front. Psychol. 10:953. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00953

Received: 02 November 2018; Accepted: 10 April 2019;
Published: 29 April 2019.

Edited by:

Gianluca Castelnuovo, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy

Reviewed by:

Angela Meadows, University of Birmingham, United Kingdom
Shelly Russell-Mayhew, University of Calgary, Canada
Sarah Nutter, University of Calgary, Canada, in collaboration with reviewer SRM

Copyright © 2019 Williams and Annandale. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Oli Williams, osw3@le.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.