Skip to main content
Log in

Small Group Predictions on an Uncertain Outcome: The Effect of Nondiagnostic Information

  • Published:
Theory and Decision Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research has established that exposure to a combination of diagnostic (i.e., relevant) and nondiagnostic (i.e., irrelevant) information results in predictions that are more regressive than predictions based on diagnostic information (Hackenbrack, 1992; Hoffman and Patton, 1997). This phenomenon has been labeled the dilution effect (e.g., Tetlock and Boettger, 1989) and has been documented when individuals make predictions. This study tests for the dilution effect when small groups make predictions, and examines the effect of using a procedure designed to reduce the dilution effect. Results indicate that group predictions are influenced by nondiagnostic information in the same manner as are individual predictions, and allowing participants to rate the diagnosticity of information prior to making predictions does not reduce the dilution effect.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Accountants International Study Group (1975), Going Concern Problems. Newport Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Institute of Certified Public Accountants (AICPA) (1988), The auditor's consideration of an entity's ability to continue as a going concern, Statement on Auditing Standards No. 59. AICPA, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, V. and Edwards, D.E. (1993),Going concern evaluation: Factors affecting decisions, The CPA Journal 63 (10): 58-60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1965), Social Psychology. Free Press of Glencoe, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burnstein, E. and Vinokur, A. (1975), What a person thinks upon learning he has chosen differently from others: Nice evidence for the persuasive arguments explanation of choice shifts, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 11: 412-426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J.A. and Davis, C.M. (1971), Cultural values and the risky-shift:A crosscultural test in Uganda and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 20: 392-399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988), Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc., Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deutch, M. and Gerard, H.G. (1955), A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgment, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 51: 629-636.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forsyth, D.R. (1990), Decision Making. Group Dynamics (2). Brooks/Cole Publishing Company, Pacific Grove, CA. pp 308-315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goethals, G.R. and Zanna, M.P. (1979), The role of social comparison in choice shifts, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37: 1469-1476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hackenbrack, K. (1992), Implications of seemingly irrelevant evidence in audit judgment, Journal of Accounting Research 30: 126-136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hans, V.P. and Doob, A.N. (1976), S. 12 of the Canada Evidence Act and the deliberations of simulated juries, Criminal Law Quarterly 18: 235-253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, V.B. and Patton, J.M. (1997), Accountability, the dilution effect, and conservatisim in auditors' fraud judgments, Journal of Accounting Research 35: 227-237.

    Google Scholar 

  • International Federation of Accountants (IFAC) (1986), Going concern, International Standard on Auditing No. 23. AICPA, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1979), Prospect theory: An analysis of decisions under risk, Econometrica 47: 263-291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, M.F. (1975), Information integration in social judgment: Interaction of judge and informational components, in M.F. Kaplan and S. Schwartz (eds), Human Judgment and Decision Processes, Academic Press, New York. pp 139-171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, M.F. (1984), How do people influence deliberation? A social psychological view, Behavioral Sciences and the Law 2: 1-6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, M.F. (1987), The influencing process in group decision making, in C. Hendrick. (eds), Group Processes, Sage Publications, Newbury Park, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCauley, C., Teger, A.I. and Kogan, N. (1971), Effect of the pretest in the riskyshift paradigm, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 20: 379-381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moscovici, S. and Zavalloni, M. (1969), The group as a polarizer of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 12: 125-135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R.E., Zukier, H. and Lemley, R. (1981), The dilution effect: Nondiagnostic information weakens the effect of diagnostic information, Cognitive Psychology 13: 248-277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutledge, R.W. (1993), The effects of group decisions and group-shifts on use of the anchoring and adjustment heuristic. Social Behavior and Personality 21: 215-226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutledge, R.W. and Harrell, A.M. (1994), The impact of responsibility and framing of budgetary information on group-shifts, Behavioral Research in Accounting 6: 92-109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, J.J. and Reckers, P.M.J. (1981), The impact of group processing on selected audit disclosure decisions, Journal of Accounting Research 19: 482-501.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoner, J.A.F. (1961), A comparison of individual and group decisions involving risk. Unpublished master's thesis. School of Industrial Management, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Tetlock, P.E. and Boettger, R. (1989), Accountability: A social magnifier of the dilution effect, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57: 388-398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zukier, H. (1982), The dilution effect: The role of the correlation and the dispersion of predictor variables in the use of nondiagnostic information, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43: 1163-1174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zukier, H. and Jennings, D.L. (1983-1984), Nondiagnosticity and typicality effects in prediction, Social Cognition 2: 187-198.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to George R. Young II.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Young, G.R., Price, K.H. & Claybrook, C. Small Group Predictions on an Uncertain Outcome: The Effect of Nondiagnostic Information. Theory and Decision 50, 149–167 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010390527827

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010390527827

Navigation