Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 03 November 2020
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

The Memory Effect of Reflected Self-Appraisals on Different Types of Others

  • 1College of National Culture and Cognitive Science, Guizhou Minzu University, Guiyang, China
  • 2School of Psychology, Capital Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 3Laboratory of Emotion and Mental Health, Chongqing University of Arts and Sciences, Chongqing, China
  • 4Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China

The development of one’s self-concept unfolds within early interactions with intimate significant others for childhood and adolescence. Previous studies suggest that people define themselves in part through internalized perceptions of other people’s beliefs about them, known as reflected self-appraisals. Even in adulthood, reflected self-appraisals still remain critically influential on direct self-appraisals, and the affect might depend on the different types of others. In the present study, for the first time, we extend the classic “other-reference” paradigm to the field of reflected self-appraisals in order to examine whether there is a difference in the memory performance of reflected self-appraisals on different types of others in an early adult sample. In the experiment, participants were told to encode personality trait words by judging how different types of others (romantic partners, friends, and classmates) think about the participants themselves. After a retention interval, they received a surprise recognition memory test. The results showed that the memory performance of romantic partners is significantly better than that of friends and classmates, indicating that the memory performance of reflected self-appraisals varies across the others with different levels of closeness. Specifically, the closer the relationship between people and others is, the better the memory performance of reflected self-appraisals will be. Meanwhile, the speed and the encoding deepness of the reflected self-appraisals vary among different genders, leading to the gender effect of recognition memory. This study might help deepen our understanding on the development of self-concept in adulthood.

Introduction

“Know yourself” is not just an old philosophical issue that arouses the interest of philosophers, such as Socrates, Plato, and Descartes (Vazire, 2010), but has also become a hot spot for psychologists (Cooley, 1902; Byrne and Shavelson, 1986). Self-knowledge is usually viewed as self-concept or an individual’s perception of his/her self. Cognitive psychologists propose that an individual’s view of the self is a “special” construct that engages unique organizational and elaborative processes (Gutchess et al., 2007). However, social psychologists emphasize the social component of the self and regard the self as a reflection of social life (Cooley, 1902; Mead, 1934). In general, self-knowledge is recognized as both a cognitive and social construction (James, 1890; Harter, 1999; Pfeifer and Peake, 2012).

How do we master the self-concept? The development of one’s self-concept unfolds within early interactions with caregivers and significant others (Murray et al., 2012). During adolescence, self-concept develops profoundly, self-evaluations become more complex, and peers and their opinions become increasingly salient (van Buuren et al., 2020). According to symbolic interactionism, one of the oldest but most influential theories of self-development, people define themselves in part through internalized perceptions of other people’s beliefs about them, known as reflected self-appraisals (Cooley, 1902; Mead, 1934). Pfeifer and Peake (2012) espoused that people’s self-views may be significantly shaped by the actual and/or perceived views of others (reflected self-appraisals). A number of studies have found the importance of reflected self-appraisals in the construction of one’s self-concept (Xu et al., 2015; Van der Cruijsen et al., 2019). In the process of internalization, reflected self-appraisals (what I think you think of me) were hypothesized to evolve into direct self-appraisals (what I think of myself) (Pfeifer and Peake, 2012). The process of reflected self-appraisals is validated in many fields, such as in the academic performance of middle school students (Bouchey and Harter, 2005; Tomasetto et al., 2015), a teacher’s teaching ability (Hu et al., 2014), professional athletic ability (Bois et al., 2005; Jose et al., 2015), juvenile delinquency (Walters, 2016; De Coster and Lutz, 2018), and the ethnic identity of ethnic minorities (Khanna, 2010; Sims, 2016). Several neuroimaging studies on the neural substrate of reflected self-appraisals also support this view (Pfeifer et al., 2009; Pfeifer and Peake, 2012; Romund et al., 2016).

Despite the importance of reflected self-appraisals in self-concept development, there is great discrepancy in the internal process. Specifically, some research found that reflected self-appraisals from different people have different effects on self-concept (Amorose, 2003). Previous research has noted that self-knowledge might be fully realized through the use of reflected self-appraisals from close others (Xu et al., 2015). This means that different types of others have different mental meaning for people. Accordingly, perceived opinions of others depend on the interpersonal closeness. In turn, reflected self-appraisals from distinct close others shape different self-concepts. The existing researches have found that information connected to the self allows for more efficient and deeper processing and encoding, which in turn facilitates later recall and recognition (Symons and Johnson, 1997). This is the well-known self-reference effect. Specifically, the memory benefits gained from self-reference have been found to extend to those with whom we are close (Mashek et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2016). When participants are asked to encode information in relation to a close other (other-reference), the overlaps in self and other representations are believed to facilitate memory processes (Serbun et al., 2011). This extension of the self-reference effect to close others may stem from a shared representation of self and close others (Ketay et al., 2018). Research from the cognitive, behavioral, and neural domains supports the idea of a shared representation between the self and close others (Aron et al., 1991; Gardner et al., 2002; Ketay et al., 2018). Taken together, both reflected self-appraisals and direct self-appraisals rely on the interpersonal closeness. Reflected self-appraisals can be viewed as a cycle of mutually influential judgments (Wallace and Tice, 2012).

The pattern of closeness for the self–other relationship varies across different cultures. The culture in which one is raised has a significant impact on the way one views himself or herself in relation to others (Triandis, 1995; Nisbett et al., 2001; Pfeifer et al., 2017). Markus and Kitayama (1991) distinguished the nature of the self from the cultural perspective, stating that people in Western cultures have independent selves, while Eastern cultures have dependent selves. Generally, East Asian cultures are characterized by thinking about things relationally (Gardner et al., 1999; Masuda and Nisbett, 2006). A cross-cultural study further demonstrated that Japanese college students’ self-concepts were more influenced by the presence of others than their American counterparts (Kanagawa et al., 2001). A study on the Chinese self-reference effect also found that the memory performances under the conditions of self-reference and mother-reference are equivalent (Zhu and Zhang, 2002). It is plausible that the development of self-concept may be affected by the unique characteristics of self–other relationship in different cultures.

In the process of self-concept development, the roles of reflected self-appraisals vary among different age groups. A lot of studies have found that a person’s self-appraisals in childhood and adolescence periods may be significantly shaped by his or her perceived views toward others (Cooley, 1902; Harter, 1999; Pfeifer et al., 2017). However, cross-cultural work suggests that reflected self-appraisals remain influential on direct self-appraisals in the process of development for members of collectivist cultures (Triandis, 1995; Gardner et al., 1999; Pfeifer et al., 2017). Therefore, it is necessary to study the characteristics of reflected self-appraisals in adulthood, especially those in the collectivist culture. The intimacy relationship is one of the most important social relationships in one’s early adulthood (Connolly et al., 2014). As Erikson’s eight-stage theory of life development states, the central issue in one’s early adulthood involves the establishment of intimacy (Erikson, 1985). Research on the process of reflected self-appraisals for Chinese young adults may help deepen our understanding on the development of self-concept.

The current study aimed to explore whether there are differences in the memory performance of reflected self-appraisals of different types of others among Chinese young people. Three types of others are selected – romantic partners, friends, and classmates – which show different levels of interpersonal closeness. On the basis of the previous researches, we raise a presumption that romantic partners are significant others for Chinese young adults. Moreover, reflected self-appraisals from romantic partners should have an important effect on their self-concept. Specifically, Chinese young adults may pay more attention to the opinions of their romantic partners and therefore encode these opinions deeply, which shows that romantic partners’ reflected self-appraisals display memory advantage. Moreover, previous studies suggested that women emphasize connectedness and sensitivity to others (Josephs et al., 1992), and they define themselves as higher in relational interdependence than men (Guimond et al., 2006). Accordingly, we speculate that males and females may have different memory advantages.

In order to study the memory process of reflected self-appraisals from different types of others, we conduct the revised remember/know self-reference paradigm in our experiment (Conway and Dewhurst, 1995; Ketay et al., 2018). In a classic self-reference task, at the encoding phase, participants were asked to rate personality traits with reference to themselves and to others. Following a retention interval, the participants were given an incidental recall or recognition test. The researchers believed that “remember” and “know” responses reflect qualitatively distinct components of recognition memory (Conway and Dewhurst, 1995; Carson et al., 2018). The “remember” responses reflect episodic memory, which in turn represents self-awareness. The “know” responses reflect the ability of semantic memory and represent the general awareness component (Conway and Dewhurst, 1995; Zhu and Zhang, 2002). Zhang et al. (2006) demonstrated that self-referential processing produced significantly higher proportions of “remember” and lower proportions of “know” judgments than do semantic-processing conditions. In the revised paradigm, the actual appraisals are replaced with reflected self-appraisals. The task of the experiment is to let the participants judge the trait words (do others think I am such a person?) and then conduct an incidental recognition test, which was a remember/know judgment. The revised paradigm was proven to be able to detect the memory advantage on the reflected self-appraisals for adolescence (Yue and Huang, 2012).

Materials and Methods

Participants

An earlier study exploring the differences between the two subject factors and three within-subject factors used a sample size of 56 (Tomova et al., 2019 with ηp2 = 0.07). We relied on the effect size to estimate the required sample size in our study using the above eta-squared as input in G-Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007 with a power of 0.8 at an alpha of 0.05). Finally, it yielded a required sample size of 42, with at least 21 participants per group. Thus, we recruited 54 healthy undergraduates (27 males and 27 females, with a mean age of 20.24 years; SD = 1.06) to participate in our study.

All subjects were in romantic relationships (with a mean length of the romantic relationship of 18.81 months, SD = 15.86) and familiar with computer operations. All participants also had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. Each subject received a written description of the study and provided informed and written consent prior to participation. This study was conducted in accordance with the recommendations of the Ethics Committee of the Chongqing University of Arts and Sciences. The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Chongqing University of Arts and Sciences. All subjects gave the written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Materials

A set of 240 Chinese adjectives were selected to compose a list of stimuli for encoding and recognition phases. The pleasure, meaningfulness, familiarity, and valence of the adjectives were considered and balanced based on the norms of Wang (2005). In all, 120 adjectives were presented at the encoding phase and the other 120 adjectives were used as lures at recognition. The study words were divided into six sub-lists (40 words each; 20 positive and 20 negative), matched on the basis of familiarity, meaningfulness, and pleasure from Wang’s (2005) norms (see Table 1). There were no significant differences on pleasure [F(5,239) = 0.072, p = 0.996], meaningfulness [F(5,239) = 0.115, p = 0.989], and familiarity [F(5,239) = 0.339, p = 0.889] across the six groups of adjectives. The number of characters in each sub-list of adjectives is equal (each adjective is composed of two to four Chinese characters). Each sub-list was assigned to one of three encoding conditions (romantic partners condition, close friends condition, or classmates condition) and counterbalanced across participants. Each sub-list was composed of half positive (e.g., generous and pleasant) and half negative (e.g., jealous and rude) traits. The order of presentation was randomized for each word for each participant, with trials from different conditions intermixed throughout the study.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistical results of 240 Chinese adjectives of different dimensions.

The “Inclusion of Other in the Self” (IOS) scale (Aron et al., 1991) was used to measure the degree of interpersonal closeness. The IOS scale consists of seven pairs of overlapping circles, with each pair overlapping slightly more than the preceding pair. The participants were asked to select the pair of circles that best portrays their relationship with another (romantic partners, close friends, or classmates).

Procedures

Questionnaire Task

After the participants entered the laboratory and sat down, they were asked to choose a close friend of the opposite sex, write down the name of that friend on the paper, and then describe the friend. The participants were next asked to choose a classmate of the opposite sex who was an acquaintance (rather than a close friend), write down that classmate’s name, and then describe the classmate. After the description was written down, the participants were asked to assess their levels of intimacy with lovers, friends, and classmates, respectively, using the IOS scale. After filling out the IOS scale, the participants were reminded that, in the subsequent experiment, when “friend” appeared on the screen, it referred to the friend whose name he/she had just written down. Likewise, when “classmate” appeared on the screen, it referred to the classmate whose name he/she had just written down.

Encoding Task

After receiving their instructions and practicing the adjective judgment task, the participants incidentally encoded adjectives in one of three ways: reflected self-appraisals of romantic partners (e.g., Does my romantic partner think that I am a friendly person?), reflected self-appraisals of friends (e.g., Does my friend think that I am a friendly person?), and reflected self-appraisals of classmates (e.g., Does my classmate think that I am a friendly person?). When “friend” (or classmate) appeared in the instruction, the experimenter reminded the participant to refer to the specific friend (or classmate) who was previously selected. Adjectives were presented on the computer screen for 4 s, during which time the participants pressed a key on a computer keyboard to provide a yes (S key) or no (K key) response. A total of 120 adjectives were encoded, with 40 adjectives assigned to each of the three conditions. Three counterbalanced orderings allowed the adjectives to be assigned to each condition across the participants, and trials were presented in a random order, with each trial unique to each participant.

Recognition Task

Immediately after the encoding task and a 6-min retention interval, the participants received instructions for the surprise recognition test, which was self-paced on the computer. The participants responded by pressing the F key to denote a previously studied word or the J key to denote a new word. If the participants pressed the F key, they indicated their subjective state of awareness accompanying that recognition by clicking on the “remember” (S key) or “know” (D key) option on the screen. The remember/know instructions were adapted from Gardiner’s (1988) study. The participants were instructed to select the “remember” option only if they were sure that they saw the word in the study phase and remembered some aspects of what was experienced at the time the word was presented. If they were sure that they had previously seen the word but could not remember any specific details about it, they were told to select the “know” option. Recognition was tested for all 120 encoded adjectives and 120 lures. Instructions placed equal emphasis on responding with accuracy and speed. Tasks were presented with E-Prime software.

Results

IOS Ratings

In order to compare the intimacy of the three types of others and the effect of gender, we conducted a 3 × 2 mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the IOS ratings with encoding condition (romantic partners, friends vs. classmates) as the within-subject factor and gender (male vs. female) as the between-subject factor. The results are shown in Table 2. The ANOVA yielded a significant main effect of encoding condition [F(2, 104) = 167.195, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.763]. Specifically, the intimacy levels with romantic partners are significantly higher than those with friends and classmates (all p < 0.001), and the intimacy levels with friends are significantly higher than those with classmates (p < 0.001). The main effect of gender was not significant [F(1, 52) = 0.514, p = 0.477, η2 = 0.010]. The interaction effect of encoding condition × gender did not reach significance [F(2, 104) = 1.604, p = 0.206, η2 = 0.030].

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistical results of the Inclusion of Other in the Self (IOS) ratings of the encoding condition.

Response Time in the Encoding Task

A 3 × 2 mixed ANOVA was conducted on the response time, with encoding condition (romantic partners, friends vs. classmates) as the within-subject factor and gender (male vs. female) as the between-subject factor. The results are shown in Table 3. The main effect of encoding condition was significant [F(2, 104) = 4.233, p = 0.017, η2 = 0.075]. The response times (RTs) with friends were significantly faster than with those of romantic partners (p = 0.041) and classmates (p = 0.013), while the RTs with the romantic partners and classmates were not significantly different (p > 0.05). The main effect of gender was also significant [F(1,52) = 5.711, p = 0.021, η2 = 0.099]. The RTs with the females were significantly faster than those of the males. The interaction effect of encoding condition × gender did not reach significance [F(2,104) = 0.114, p = 0.893, η2 = 0.002].

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Descriptive statistical results of response time (ms) of the encoding condition.

Recognition Memory Performance

A 3 × 2 × 2 mixed ANOVA was conducted on the corrected recognition scores, with encoding condition (romantic partners, friends vs. classmates) and response type (remember vs. know) as the within-subject factors and gender (male vs. female) as the between-subject factor. The results are shown in Table 4. The main effects of the three independent variables reached significance: encoding condition, F(2,104) = 3.624, p = 0.030, η2 = 0.065; response type, F(1,52) = 108.156, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.776; and gender, F(1,52) = 4.493, p = 0.039, η2 = 0.080. Specifically, the corrected recognition scores with romantic partners and friends were significantly better than those with classmates (all p < 0.05). Participants gave a significantly higher number of “remember” responses than “know” responses. And the corrected recognition scores with males were significantly better than those with females. The interaction effect of encoding condition × response type × gender was not significant [F(2,104) = 2.029, p = 0.143, η2 = 0.038]. The interaction effect of encoding condition × gender was not significant [F(2,104) = 0.753, p = 0.474, η2 = 0.014]. The interaction effect of response type × gender was also not significant [F(1,52) = 2.238, p = 0.133, η2 = 0.043]. However, the interaction effect of encoding condition × response type reached significance [F(2,104) = 4.043, p = 0.020, η2 = 0.072]. For the “remember” condition, the corrected recognition scores with romantic partners were significantly better than those with the friends (p = 0.037) and classmates (p = 0.018), but the difference between friends and classmates was not significant (p > 0.05). For the “know” condition, there was no significant difference among the three different encoding conditions (see Figure 1).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Descriptive statistical results of gender differences in the correct recognition rate of the coding condition.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Corrected recognition rates of the different types of encoding condition. Error bars denote 95% CI. p < 0.05; ∗∗∗p < 0.001.

Discussion

The present study is the first experiment to demonstrate memory performance on reflected self-appraisals of different types of others. For those in early adulthood, an intimate relationship is the most important interpersonal relationship (Erikson, 1985; Connolly et al., 2014). Therefore, we selected three types of others in the experiment: romantic partners, friends, and classmates. The task of the experiment was to ask the participants to judge whether others think that they are people with certain traits, and then an incidental recognition test was conducted. The results show that the memory performance of reflected self-appraisals varies with the degree of intimacy between individuals and others: the closer the relationship, the better the memory performance. Meanwhile, females encode information faster than males in the reflected self-appraisal process, but males gain better memory performance than females in the subsequent recognition task.

The present study recorded the response time during the encoding task. The results show that the response time under the friends condition was significantly faster than that for romantic partners and classmates. This result may indicate that the cognitive processing mechanism of reflected self-appraisals is different under the three types of conditions. A neuroimaging study revealed the internal encoding process and found that reflecting on a close other’s opinions about you (as compared to your own opinions of yourself) activated brain areas that are associated with the representation and regulation of emotional/motivational states. In contrast, reflecting a non-close other’s opinions about you activated areas that are involved in storing semantic and visual memories for people and objects (Ochsner et al., 2005). Due to the importance of intimacy in early adulthood, compared with the friends condition, the participants were apparently full of emotion and motivated when inferring romantic partners’ opinions about themselves. On the other hand, a lot of studies confirmed the overlaps in self and close others representations (Serbun et al., 2011; Ketay et al., 2018). It seems difficult to distinguish romantic partners’ opinions about themselves from their self-view. It may take up a lot of cognitive resources. Therefore, the reaction time was longer. In contrast, classmates are not relatively intimate and show a long psychological distance; therefore, participants have to rely on memories of prior experiences with them. Thus, it took a longer time to search for information in the memory to make judgments when inferring their classmates’ opinions about themselves. Further research is needed to demonstrate the internal cognitive processing of different types of others’ reflected self-appraisals. In this study, we found that the RTs with females were significantly faster than those with males. Bakan (1966) argued that a man is “egocentrist,” making himself the center of the world, while a woman is “alterocentrist,” making another person the center of her emotions. So a woman is considered as more sensitive to others (Josephs et al., 1992; Guimond et al., 2006). But a man needs more time to draw a distinction between his self-view and the opinions from close others, which involves a more deep encoding of information.

The present study finds that the proportions of “remember” are significantly higher than those of “know,” which is consistent with previous research (Zhu and Zhang, 2002; Zhang et al., 2006; Boduroglu et al., 2015). In self-reference research, researchers generally believe that remember/know is a very sensitive indicator of the self and that the remember responses reflect episodic memory, which in turn represents self-awareness (Tulving, 1985; Conway and Dewhurst, 1995). The know responses reflect the semantic memory and represent the general awareness component (Tulving, 1985; Zhu and Zhang, 2002). The results of the present study suggest that people usually have more self-processing when they infer others’ views on themselves.

In the present study, the memory performance of romantic partners is significantly better than that of friends and classmates on the remember condition. As mentioned above, the remember responses typically feature recollective experiences, whereby subjects can recall sensory aspects of the original events or thoughts and feelings that occurred during the event, while the know responses primarily feature feelings of knowing or familiarity (Conway and Dewhurst, 1995). When inferring a close other’s opinions about you, much emotion and motivation are involved in the process (Ochsner et al., 2005). As Symons and Johnson (1997) argued, the closer and more familiar a person is, the more organized and elaborate the information is about the individual. In early adulthood, one of the most important interpersonal relationships is intimacy (Zhou and Su, 2008; Connolly et al., 2014). Therefore, compared to friends and classmates, inferring romantic partners’ opinions can arouse more emotional details and episodic memory, which is conducive to deep processing of the information. Thus, the memory performance of romantic partners is better than that of others. It is worth noting that the corrected recognition scores for males are significantly better than those for females on the whole, which may be regarded as the by-product of a more cognitive resource allocation and a deep encoding of the opinions from close others for males (Bakan, 1966; Guimond et al., 2006).

It is interesting that our study found that people’s reflected self-appraisals of different types of others are hierarchical, which is in line with the hierarchy of other-reference (Symons and Johnson, 1997; Wang et al., 2019). That is, the closer the relationship between people and others is, the deeper people’s cognitive processing of how others perceive themselves will be and the better the memory performance of reflected self-appraisals will be. It is believed that the East Asian culture exhibit dependent selves (Markus and Kitayama, 1991), and one of the characteristics of the East Asian culture is interpersonal relationships (Yue and Huang, 2012). Thus, the memory effect of reflected self-appraisals on different types of others may be more significant in collectivist culture. In all, the results have verified our research hypothesis.

Previous studies on other-reference mostly make judgments on different types of others. The present study, for the first time, extends the other-reference to the field of reflected self-appraisals, which was to infer the views of different types of others on themselves. The study could promote the understanding of the cognitive processing mechanism of reflected self-appraisals. However, the study also has some limitations. The present study is a single experiment, and the sample is composed of Chinese college students, which makes the generalizability of the research limited. Therefore, future studies should include more diverse samples with respect to age and geographical region. For example, future research should investigate adolescents because previous studies have found that adolescent self-construal may rely more heavily on others’ perspectives about the self (Pfeifer et al., 2009). The present study focuses on romantic relationship. Besides romantic relationships, familiarity and importance are also important dimensions of interpersonal relationship. Future studies should examine diverse types of relationships in depth, such as kin relationships (e.g., siblings) (Platek and Kemp, 2009), familiar relationships (Mashek et al., 2003), and important relationships (Snodgrass et al., 1998). Finally, as for the content of reflected self-appraisals, the present study is only a general description and does not distinguish different domain-specific self-concepts. Previous studies have found that, in China, the social self is particularly interdependent, whereas the academic self is characterized by a relatively greater autonomy (Li, 2005; Li, 2006; Pfeifer et al., 2017). Therefore, future research should distinguish the content of reflected self-appraisals.

In conclusion, the present study finds the memory effect of reflected self-appraisals on different types of others; the closer the individual is to others, the better the memory performance will be. Females respond significantly faster in the encoding process than do males, but lose the memory advantage in the recognition memory task. People in early adulthood are more concerned about how romantic partners see themselves. The study shows that different types of other people’s reflected self-appraisals are hierarchical, which might promote our understanding of the cognitive mechanism of reflected self-appraisals.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Chongqing University of Arts and Sciences agreed to carry out the study, and all the procedures involved were in line with the sixth revision of the Helsinki Declaration. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

CY, WH, and TY designed the experiments. CY, WP, and YY carried out the experiments. CY and YY analyzed the sequencing data. CY and WP drafted the initial manuscript and revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Education Science Planning Project, China Western Project, “The mental mechanism of group identification for the adolescents in minority area,” grant no. XMA200282.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Amorose, A. J. (2003). Reflected appraisals and perceived importance of significant others’ appraisals as predictors of college athletes’ self-perceptions of competence. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 74, 60–70. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2003.10609065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Tudor, M., and Nelson, G. (1991). Close relationships as including other in the self. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 60, 241–253. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakan, D. (1966). The Duality of Human Existence. Chicago, IL: Rand Mcnally.

Google Scholar

Boduroglu, A., Pehlivanoglu, D., Tekcan, A. I., and Kapucu, A. (2015). Effects of self-referencing on feeling-of-knowing accuracy and recollective experience. Memory 23, 736–747. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.925927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bois, J. E., Sarrazin, P. G., Brustad, R. J., Chanal, J. P., and Trouilloud, D. O. (2005). Parents’ appraisals, reflected appraisals, and Children’s self-appraisals of sport competence: a yearlong study. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 17, 273–289. doi: 10.1080/10413200500313552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouchey, H. A., and Harter, S. (2005). Reflected appraisals, academic self-perceptions, and math/science performance during early adolescence. J. Educ. Psychol. 97, 673–686. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.97.4.673

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M., and Shavelson, R. J. (1986). On the structure of adolescent self-concept. J. Educ. Psychol. 78, 474–481. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.78.6.474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carson, N., Rosenbaum, R. S., Moscovitch, M., and Murphy, K. J. (2018). Self-reference effect and self-reference recollection effect for trait adjectives in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 24, 821–832. doi: 10.1017/S1355617718000395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connolly, J., McIsaac, C., Shulman, S., Wincentak, K., Joly, L., Heifetz, M., et al. (2014). Development of romantic relationships in adolescence and emerging adulthood: implications for community mental health. Can. J. Commun. Ment. Health 33, 7–19. doi: 10.7870/cjcmh-2014-002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, M. A., and Dewhurst, S. A. (1995). The self and recollective experience. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 1–19. doi: 10.1002/acp.2350090102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human Nature and the Social Order. New York, NY: Charles Scribner’s Sons.

Google Scholar

De Coster, S., and Lutz, J. (2018). Reconsidering labels and primary deviance: false appraisals, reflected appraisals, and delinquency onset. J. Res. Crime Delinquency 55:002242781877143. doi: 10.1177/0022427818771437

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erikson, E. H. (1985). Childhood and Society. (Original work published 1950). New York, NY: Norton.

Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). GPower 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardiner, J. M. (1988). Functional aspects of recollective experience. Mem. Cogn. 16, 309–313. doi: 10.3758/bf03197041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., and Hochschild, L. (2002). When you and I are “we,” you are not threatening: the role of self-expansion in social comparison. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 239–251. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.2.239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., and Lee, A. Y. (1999). I” value freedom, but “We” value relationships: self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychol. Sci. 10, 321–326. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00162

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guimond, S., Chatard, A., Martinot, D., Crisp, R. J., and Redersdorff, S. (2006). Social comparison, self-stereotyping, and gender differences in self-construals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90, 221–242. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutchess, A. H., Kensinger, E. A., Yoon, C., and Schacter, D. L. (2007). Ageing and the self-reference effect in memory. Memory 15, 822–837. doi: 10.1080/09658210701701394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harter, S. (1999). The Construction of the Self: a Developmental Perspective. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Hu, C., Yue, C., He, H., and Yang, D. (2014). Relationship among self-appraisals, others’ actual appraisals and reflected appraisals on the teachers’ teaching ability of normal college students. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 30, 520–526. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2014.05.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology. New York, NY: Holt.

Google Scholar

Jose, A. C., Javier, F.-R., and Antonio, M.-G. (2015). Connecting Athletes’ self-perceptions and metaperceptions of competence: a structural equation modeling approach. J. Hum. Kinet. 46, 189–198. doi: 10.1515/hukin-2015-0047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Josephs, R. A., Markus, H. R., and Tafarodi, R. W. (1992). Gender and self-esteem. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 391–402. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanagawa, C., Cross, S., and Markus, H. (2001). Who am I?” The cultural psychology of the conceptual self. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 27, 90–103. doi: 10.1177/0146167201271008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ketay, S., Beck, L., Riela, S., Bailey, C., and Aron, A. (2018). Seeing you in me: preliminary evidence for perceptual overlap between self and close others. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 36, 2474–2486. doi: 10.1177/0265407518788702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khanna, N. (2010). “If You’re half black, You’re just black”: reflected appraisals and the persistence of the one-drop rule. Sociol. Q. 51, 96–121. doi: 10.1111/j.1533-8525.2009.01162.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H.-N., Rosa, N. M., and Gutchess, A. H. (2016). Ageing and the group-reference effect in memory. Memory 24, 746–756. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1049184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J. (2005). Mind or virtue: western and Chinese beliefs about learning. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 14, 190–194. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00362.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J. (2006). Self in learning: Chinese Adolescents’ goals and sense of agency. Child Dev. 77, 482–501. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00883.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mashek, D. J., Aron, A., and Boncimino, M. (2003). Confusions of self with close others. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 382–392. doi: 10.1177/0146167202250220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masuda, T., and Nisbett, R. (2006). Culture and change blindness. Cogn. Sci. 30, 381–399. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_63

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, Self, and Society from the Standpoint of a Social Behaviorist. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Murray, R. J., Schaer, M., and Debbané, M. (2012). Degrees of separation: a quantitative neuroimaging meta-analysis investigating self-specificity and shared neural activation between self- and other-reflection. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1043–1059. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychol. Rev. 108, 291–310. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., Beer, J. S., Robertson, E. R., Cooper, J. C., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Kihsltrom, J. F., et al. (2005). The neural correlates of direct and reflected self-knowledge. Neuroimage 28, 797–814. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfeifer, J. H., Mahy, C. E. V., Merchant, J. S., Chen, C., Masten, C. L., Fuligni, A. J., et al. (2017). Neural systems for reflected and direct self-appraisals in Chinese young adults: exploring the role of the temporal-parietal junction. Cult. Divers. Ethnic Minor. Psychol. 23, 45–58. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfeifer, J. H., Masten, C. L., Borofsky, L. A., Dapretto, M., Fuligni, A. J., and Lieberman, M. D. (2009). Neural correlates of direct and reflected self-appraisals in adolescents and adults: when social perspective-taking informs self-perception. Child Dev. 80, 1016–1038. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01314.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfeifer, J. H., and Peake, S. J. (2012). Self-development: integrating cognitive, socioemotional, and neuroimaging perspectives. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2, 55–69. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Platek, S. M., and Kemp, S. M. (2009). Is family special to the brain? An event-related fMRI study of familiar, familial, and self-face recognition. Neuropsychologia 47, 849–858. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romund, L., Golde, S., Lorenz, R., Raufelder, D., Pelz, P., Gleich, T., et al. (2016). Neural correlates of the self-concept in adolescence-A focus on the significance of friends. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 987–996. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serbun, S. J., Shih, J. Y., and Gutchess, A. H. (2011). Memory for details with self-referencing. Memory 19, 1004–1014. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2011.626429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sims, J. P. (2016). Reevaluation of the influence of appearance and reflected appraisals for mixed-race identity: the role of consistent inconsistent racial perception. Sociol. Race Ethnicity 2, 569–583. doi: 10.1177/2332649216634740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snodgrass, S. E., Hecht, M. A., and Ploutz-Snyder, R. (1998). Interpersonal sensitivity: expressivity or perceptivity? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 238–249. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Symons, C. S., and Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 121, 371–394. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasetto, C., Mirisola, A., Galdi, S., and Cadinu, M. (2015). Parents’ math–gender stereotypes, children’s self-perception of ability, and children’s appraisal of parents’ evaluations in 6-year-olds. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 42, 186–198. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomova, L., Heinrichs, M., and Lamm, C. (2019). The other and me: effects of oxytocin on self-other distinction. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 136, 49–53. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism & Collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Google Scholar

Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Can. Psychol. 26, 1–12. doi: 10.1037/h0080017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Buuren, M., Walsh, R. J., Sijtsma, H., Hollarek, M., Lee, N. C., Bos, P. A., et al. (2020). Neural correlates of self- and other-referential processing in young adolescents and the effects of testosterone and peer similarity. Neuroimage 219:117060. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Cruijsen, R., Peters, S., Zoetendaal, K. P. M., Pfeifer, J. H., and Crone, E. A. (2019). Direct and reflected self-concept show increasing similarity across adolescence: a functional neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia 129, 407–417. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vazire, S. (2010). Who knows what about a person? The self–other knowledge asymmetry (SOKA) model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 281–300. doi: 10.1037/a0017908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallace, H. M., and Tice, D. M. (2012). Reflected Appraisal through a 21st-Century Looking Glass Handbook of Self and Identity, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: The Guilford Press, 124–140.

Google Scholar

Walters, G. D. (2016). Reflected appraisals and self-view in delinquency development: an analysis of retrospective accounts from members of the Racine birth cohorts. J. Crim. Justice 47, 100–107. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2016.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, B., Fu, Y., and Zhang, J. (2019). Influence of language and culture on retrieval-induced forgetting under the self-referential condition: evidence from the Han and the Mosuo. Acta Psychol. Sin. 51, 450–461. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00450

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, D. (2005). Explorations of Chinese Personality. China: SOCIAL SCIENCES ACADEMIC PRESS(CHINA).

Google Scholar

Xu, X., Chen, Y., Zhao, Y., and Yang, J. (2015). The neural evidence of the reflected appraisal process as a main path for learning about one’s self-knowledge of personality. Biol. Psychol. 110, 85–90. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yue, C., and Huang, X. (2012). A review of the properties of reflected self-appraisal. Psychol. Sci. 35, 735–739. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2012.03.037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Zhou, T., Zhang, J., Liu, Z., Fan, J., and Zhu, Y. (2006). In search of the Chinese self: an fMRI study. Sci. China Series C 49, 89–96. doi: 10.1007/s11427-004-5105-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, L., and Su, Y. (2008). The role of genuine intimacy in the lover-reference effect. Acta Psychol. Sin. 40, 487–495. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2008.00487

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, Y., and Zhang, L. (2002). An experimental study on the self-reference effect. Sci. China Series C Life Sci. 45, 120–128. doi: 10.1360/02yc9014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: reflected self-appraisals, self-referencing, memory, remember-know (R-K) judgments, self-knowledge

Citation: Yue C, Yang Y, He W, Yue T and Pan W (2020) The Memory Effect of Reflected Self-Appraisals on Different Types of Others. Front. Psychol. 11:553585. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.553585

Received: 19 April 2020; Accepted: 22 September 2020;
Published: 03 November 2020.

Edited by:

Xuebing Li, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), China

Reviewed by:

Fang Cui, Shenzhen University, China
Yu Luo, Institute of Psychology (CAS), China

Copyright © 2020 Yue, Yang, He, Yue and Pan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Weigang Pan, confessing@163.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.