Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 April 2022
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.839340

Longitudinal Influences of DRD4 Polymorphism and Early Maternal Caregiving on Personality Development and Problem Behavior in Middle Childhood and Adolescence

  • 1Institute of Psychology, Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Wuppertal, Wuppertal, Germany
  • 2Institute of Psychology, Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg, Erlangen, Germany

Most studies examining gene-environment effects on self-regulation focus on outcomes early childhood or adulthood. However, only a few studies investigate longitudinal effects during middle childhood and adolescence and compare two domains of early caregiving. In a longitudinal follow-up with a sample of N = 87, we studied the effects of differences in the DRD4 tandem repeat polymorphisms and two domains of early maternal caregiving quality on children’s personality development using Block’s California Child Q-Set (CCQ) at age six and age 12 and on problem behavior at ages six and seven. Early maternal regulation quality predicted later ego-resiliency and aggressiveness. In addition, significant gene-environment interactions revealed that children with the 7+ DRD4 tandem repeat polymorphism and poor maternal regulation quality in infancy showed lower scores in ego-resiliency and higher scores in ego-undercontrol and CCQ aggressiveness. In contrast, children who had experienced effective maternal regulation in infancy showed a comparable level in personality traits and problem behavior as the DRD4 7- group independent of the levels of maternal regulatory behavior. Similarly, longitudinal caregiving × DRD4 interactions were found for behavior problems in middle childhood, especially for oppositional-aggression, inattentive-hyperactivity, and social competence. Early caregiving effects were only found for maternal regulation quality, but not for maternal responsiveness. Effective early maternal regulation in infancy can moderate the negative effect of DRD4 7+ on children’s self-regulation in middle childhood and adolescence. However, maternal responsiveness has no comparable effects. It seems relevant to consider several dimensions of early caregiving and to also measure the environment in more detail in gene-environment studies.

Introduction

Although attachment theory emphasizes the importance of caregivers’ emotional availability and appropriate support for child development, Bowlby’s concept of the development of emotionally stable personality traits was an interaction between genetic dispositions and caregiving experiences (Bowlby, 1973). He included Waddington’s epigenetic model (Waddington, 1942) in attachment theory and suggested that an emotionally stable person develops as the outcome of continuing genetic and supportive environmental interactions. In a transactional perspective (Sameroff, 2010), differences in personality characteristics (e.g., emotional stability, self-regulation,) consequently influence the probability of individual adjustment or maladjustment when facing later adversities or challenges during the life course (Bowlby, 1988).

Attachment, Caregiving, and Personality Characteristics

Research on social development and specifically attachment development has repeatedly demonstrated that secure attachment and supporting parenting are associated with personality characteristics that assess emotional stability and socially responsible behavior. Ego-control and ego-resiliency (Block and Block, 2006) are such personality traits. Ego-control describes the habitual tendency to control one’s impulses and emotions, ranging from poor delay of gratification and impulsiveness even when inappropriate (ego-undercontrol) to inhibition even when not necessary in that specific situation (ego-overcontrol). Ego-resiliency describes the situation-appropriate modulation of ego-control (Block and Block, 2006), i.e., the ability to adapt the level of ego-control to the requirements of a specific context or situation. Both personality characteristics are relatively stable over time, already starting in early childhood (Block and Block, 2006; Chuang et al., 2006; Taylor et al., 2014). Studies repeatedly showed associations of both ego-resiliency and ego-undercontrol with concurrent and later adaptation, psychopathology, and social or academic success (Rothbaum and Weisz, 1994; Robins et al., 1996; van Aken et al., 2002; Martel et al., 2007; Syed et al., 2020).

Secure attachment in infancy, middle childhood, and adolescence is associated with higher levels of ego-resiliency and moderate levels of ego-control (Arend et al., 1979; Kobak and Sceery, 1988; Suess et al., 1992; Zimmermann et al., 1996; Zimmermann, 1999; Kersten-Alvarez et al., 2010; Caldwell and Shaver, 2012; Zimmermann and Scheuerer-Englisch, 2013). Infant attachment security is also longitudinally associated with low neuroticism, high agreeableness, and conscientiousness in adulthood (Young et al., 2019), specifically those Big Five domains that also characterize ego-resilient individuals (Scholte et al., 2005).

Moreover, also caregiving sensitivity is associated with ego-resiliency. Stams et al. (2001) reported that higher caregiver sensitivity towards infants as a result of an experimental intervention was longitudinally associated with higher ego-resiliency at age 7 (for girls), whereas maltreatment or parental intrusiveness was associated with lower ego-resiliency (Kim et al., 2009; Taylor et al., 2013). Moreover, support of children’s emotional needs and supportive guidance during exploration and problem-solving (Block et al., 1998; Kremen and Block, 1998) as well as early child-care (Wessels et al., 1997) are associated with higher ego-resiliency scores. Thus, there is some empirical evidence for concurrent and longitudinal effects of caregiving and attachment security on the development of personality characteristics that specifically assess adaptive self-regulation (e.g., ego-resiliency).

Effects of Gene × Environment Interaction on Personality Development

Despite the many studies that examine gene × environment interactions on signs of dysregulation in developmental psychopathology (Moffitt et al., 2006; Thapar et al., 2007a; Manuck and McCaffery, 2014; Pinto et al., 2015; King et al., 2016) or effortful control in childhood and adolescence (Cho et al., 2016; Ganiban et al., 2021) only few studies examined gene × environment interaction on ego-resiliency development. Taylor et al. (2014) examined the longitudinal influences of early maternal caregiving behavior and two variants of the serotonin transporter polymorphism on the development of ego-resiliency from toddlerhood to middle childhood. They reported two main effects. Early sensitivity at the age of 18 months was associated with concurrent ego-resiliency (but not with ego-resiliency in middle childhood) and in addition, the haplotype of the two variants of the serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-httlpr and SERT intron 2) was associated with higher ego-resiliency scores in early childhood, but again not with ego-resiliency during middle childhood. However, they did not find evidence for a gene × environment interaction using a composite score of maternal sensitivity, warmth, intrusiveness, and control as caregiving index.

In contrast, studies focusing on genetic polymorphisms affecting the functionality of the dopamine system, specifically the DRD4 polymorphism, report direct effects as well as the moderation of genetic effects by environmental factors on developmental outcomes in the domain of self-regulation, impulsivity, and externalizing behavior. The DRD4 gene is one of those candidate genes that repeatedly but not consistently is associated with specific aspects of temperament and personality (Savitz and Ramesar, 2004). Associations between a variant of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (the 7-repeat allele of the 48 base pair repeat sequence; DRD4 7+) have been found already with infant temperament, sensation seeking, and attention deficit disorder (Ebstein et al., 1998, 2000; Lakatos et al., 2003; Faraone et al., 2005; Birkas et al., 2006). The DRD4 gene is functionally associated with the signal transmission of the dopamine system regulating many executive functions like control and inhibition of attention and action. There is ample evidence that the DRD4 7+ variant is associated with increased difficulties in self-control, executive functioning, or signs of ADHD in children (Schmidt et al., 2001; DiLalla et al., 2009; Pappa et al., 2015), but also for DRD4 × environment interaction effects (Martel et al., 2011). King et al. (2016) reported that infants carrying the DRD4 7+ allele showed increased rates of externalizing behavior when mothers showed low sensitivity. Other studies report similar interaction effects between DRD4 polymorphism status and low maternal sensitivity on later externalizing problems and ADHD for older children and adolescents (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn, 2006; Nikitopoulos et al., 2014). In a detailed analysis, Elam and DiLalla (2018) showed that, even during a short mother-child interaction of 10 min, children carrying the DRD4 7+ allele became less responsive towards their mothers and mothers became less sensitive. The interaction effect of maternal responsiveness and DRD4 7+ on the CBCL dysregulation profile in middle childhood was only obvious when infants additionally showed early regulatory problems (Poustka et al., 2015). Moreover, as shown in the large NICHD-study, young children who are carriers of the DRD4 7+ variant showed less delay of gratification and more inattention or impulsivity when experiencing more hours of daycare compared to DRD4 7- carriers (Berry et al., 2013). However, Propper et al. (2007) also reported that African American children with the short polymorphism of the DRD4 showed less externalizing behavior when their mothers showed more warmth. They emphasize the relevance of different effects of specific parenting behaviors. Specifically, maltreatment may play a crucial role. Thibodeau et al. (2015) reported that children with specific genetic variations of dopaminergic genes showed a higher environmental sensitivity for the development of impulsivity (i.e., ego-undercontrol) and consequently developed more anti-social behavior when experiencing maltreatment. Therefore, it seems reasonable to expect effects of genetic differences that affect the dopamine system on those personality characteristics that directly assess behavior tendencies of self-control as well as capacities for self-regulation and modulation of self-control, as described by Block and Block (1980) with their concept of ego-control and ego-resiliency. Moreover, also other individual differences in self-regulation (e.g., aggression, attention problems) might well be influenced by variants of the DRD4 gene and moderated by specific aspects of caregiving.

Caregiving and Self-Regulation: Timing and Domain-Specific Effects

Research on the association between caregiving and children’s self-regulation has shown mixed results (Karreman et al., 2006; Bridgett et al., 2015). The timing of caregiving experiences (i.e., early childhood vs. late childhood) and differences in specific caregiving behaviors can play a decisive role in the development of self-regulation and related behavior problems.

The relevance of timing in caregiving has become obvious in the EARA study on differential effects of early deprivation and later adoption (O’Connor et al., 2003). From childhood until early adulthood, the later adoption group (after the age of 6 months) showed increased and enduring attentional problems compared to the early adoption group (Sonuga-Barke et al., 2017). Early deprivation or early maltreatment has long-lasting effects on self-regulation at the physiological level (Gunnar and Pollak, 2007; Gunnar and Quevedo, 2007) and at the level of information processing (Pollak and Sinha, 2002; Wismer Fries et al., 2005). These studies suggest the importance of early caregiving experiences for self-regulatory processes in the area of attention, social behavior, emotional and social cognition. Similarly, early intervention programs focusing on changing the parent-child interaction seem to be more effective in influencing self-regulation or executive functions compared to later interventions (Hentges et al., 2020).

The relevance of a specific parenting domain is another source contributing to the differences in research results on the association between caregiving and self-regulation (Belsky, 1984). In a meta-analysis, Karreman et al. (2006) only found small effect sizes for the general association between parenting and self-regulation in childhood. Moreover, Grusec and Davidov (2010) have pointed out that different parenting domains have different outcomes in child development. In their review of the socialization literature, maternal warmth was associated with children’s felt security but not necessarily with their self-regulation. We, therefore, assume that specifically experiences of effective emotional regulation, autonomy support, and limit-setting contribute to the development of self-regulation (Kochanska et al., 2000; Bernier et al., 2010).

Differential effects of specific parental behaviors can also be found in the case of maternal sensitivity which includes the processes of perception, correct interpretation, prompt reaction, and regulation of the infant’s emotions or needs (Ainsworth, 1967). Although these components are interrelated, especially the regulation of the infant’s needs is predictive of a low rate of crying, whereas the perception or prompt reaction (i.e., responsiveness) is not (Lohaus et al., 2004).

The role of specific aspects of sensitive caregiving on self-regulation is also obvious in gene × environment studies. Sheese et al. (2007) showed that high parenting quality in infancy moderates the effect of the DRD4 repeat allele (7+) on sensation seeking (i.e., activity, high pleasure, impulsivity), however not on effortful control (of attention). Similarly, maternal responsiveness but not maternal effective regulation moderated the effect of 5-httlpr polymorphism on attachment disorganization (Spangler et al., 2009).

Thus, the experience of external regulation, but not the fast caregiver reaction is a predictor of a child’s effective self-regulation. This is especially important in infancy when individuals rely on external emotion regulation provided by the parents (Thompson, 1993). The effective regulation of the infant’s attachment needs and also of the increasingly salient need for autonomy and exploration are of special importance for the development of self-regulation.

Aims of the Study

Given these previous findings, the present study aimed at examining the interplay between early social caregiving experiences and allelic variations of the DRD4 gene in the longitudinal development of self-regulatory competencies. We tested this specifically for: (1) ego-resiliency and ego-control as personality characteristics assessing self-regulation, (2) aggressiveness and anxiety as personality characteristics that represent dysregulation but not yet clinical symptoms, and (3) problem behavior as clinical indicators of enduring dysregulation.

Following the hypothesis that maternal regulatory behavior and not only maternal responsiveness to the infant’s signals is influential for the development of self-regulation, we separately examined the effect of the two maternal caregiving variables: (1) maternal regulation of the infant’s emotions and (2) maternal responsiveness to the infant’s needs.

From a developmental perspective, we additionally wanted to test whether the effects of gene × environment interaction or personality development are depending on age. Savitz and Ramesar (2004) conclude that effects of the DRD4 polymorphism are more prominent at a younger age. In addition, Reiss and Leve (2007) emphasize that the activation of genes might be age-specific and that early genetic effects might influence later gene-environment interaction. Moreover, Schmidt et al. (2001) showed DRD4 effects on the stability of attention problems. Thus, the analysis of age effects was included. Finally, we also examined possible effects of gene × environment correlations or evocative processes (Moffitt et al., 2006; Taylor and Kim-Cohen, 2007).

In sum, we had three main aims:

(1) to study gene-environment interaction of DRD4 polymorphisms and early caregiving on the longitudinal development of self-regulation capacities, (a) at the level of personality characteristics for self-regulation at age six and age 12 (ego-resiliency and ego-control), (b) individual differences in dysregulation at age six and age 12 (aggression and anxiety), and (c) behavior problems at age six and seven,

(2) to examine whether maternal regulatory caregiving predicted children’s self-regulation better compared to maternal responsiveness, and

(3) to examine age differences in these effects on personality.

Method

Participants and Procedure

The original sample consists of 106 healthy German, Caucasian, low-risk infants (53 girls/53 boys) and their mothers stemming from a wide range of socioeconomic statuses. Families included in the study at the time of recruitment were all two-parent families, only children with gestation age between 38 and 42 months, no pre-term birth, no handicaps or severe illnesses, or long hospitalization during the first year of life. The mothers were the infant’s primary caretakers in all families except one.We obtained informed consent from the parents at each assessment period. For more details, see Spangler and Schieche (1998).

At the age of 12 months, we observed mother-infant interaction during a 30-min competing demands free play session. Mothers answered a questionnaire while feeling free to respond to the infant, as they usually would do. In a follow-up assessment at age six with 97 subjects, mothers were asked to describe their children’s personality by means of the California Child Q-sort (CCQ) and their children’s problem behavior at age six and again at age seven by a standardized questionnaire (VBV-EL). In a follow-up assessment at age 12, we collected cheek cells from 95 of the original 106 children and from 96 of their mothers for genetic analyses. In addition, mothers again provided a description of the children’s personality using the CCQ for 95 children. For the longitudinal gene-environment analysis 87 subjects had complete data.

Data Analyses and Measures

Maternal Behavior at 12 Months

The quality of maternal behavior was analyzed from the videotaped free play sessions. In an event-sampling approach, the mothers’ perceptions of the infant’s signals, as well as the promptness and appropriateness of the responses to these signals as components of maternal sensitivity, were coded. Infant signals were defined as any instance of vocalization, negative facial expression, and behavior directed to the mother (e.g., approaching, looking at mother for at least 3 s, offering an object, grasping for her questionnaire). Maternal behavior was coded regarding the two dimensions maternal responsiveness and maternal regulation quality with three single variables for each maternal caregiving dimension. Maternal responsiveness was assessed based on: (1) the proportion of maternal reactions to the infant’s signals (e.g., signals followed by any maternal responses, ranging from short glances or behavioral breaks indicating attention to obvious infant-directed behavior), (2) the proportion of responded signals followed by an infant-directed active behavior going beyond short glances or looks, and (3) the proportion of prompt responses (within 3 s). Maternal regulation quality was assessed based on (4) the proportion of appropriate responses (agreeing with the child’s wish or need, e.g., mother providing a wanted object or comforting the infant when distressed), (5) the proportion of emotionally positive responses (characterized by affectionate, respectful, and sensitive behavior), and (6) proportion of episodes with sustained regulation (mother finally comforting the infant; mother allowing the infant to play with a pencil as long as he/she wants).

Reliability was examined over 12 play situations. The rater agreement for the detection of infant signals was 81%. Kappa scores for the single maternal behavior categories ranged between 0.76 and 1.0. The three variables for maternal responsiveness as well as the three variables for maternal regulation quality showed high within domain correlations (ranging from 0.81 to 0.92). However, correlations between the two maternal caregiving domains were lower (ranging from 0.36 to 0.62). We computed separate composite scores based on z-transformed scores for maternal responsiveness and for maternal regulation quality, respectively.

Maternal responsiveness represents the mean proportion of the infant’s signals that the mother perceived and reacted to promptly, while maternal regulation quality represents the mean proportion of appropriate, emotionally positive, and sustained regulation of the infant. For statistical analysis, both maternal caregiving domains were dichotomized by the median split.

Children’s Personality

The California Child-Q-sort (CCQ; Block and Block, 1980) was used to assess the children’s personality traits. At age six, we used the German 54-item short version (Göttert and Asendorpf, 1989) of the original 100-items California Child-Q-sort (CCQ; Block and Block, 1980) to reduce the participant’s workload.The correlations between each child’s Q-sort and the prototypes for ego-resiliency and ego-under control (self-regulation personality characteristics; provided by Block and Block, 1980), as well as aggressiveness and anxiety (dysregulatory personality characteristics; Zimmermann et al., 2009; Zimmermann and Scheuerer-Englisch, 2013) are scores for the prototypicalities of each child for each of these dimensions. Ego-control describes the degree of habitual control of impulses and emotions ranging from a low delay of gratification and impulsivity to enduring inhibition. Ego-resiliency describes the ability to modulate the level of control appropriately depending on the situation. Aggressiveness assesses the salience of attacking others directly or indirectly, and anxiety the salience of fear and withdrawal within the personality profile of the child. The prototypes for aggressiveness and anxiety were composites of Q-sorts provided by clinical and developmental psychologists with a reliability of r = 0.87, and r = 0.83, respectively.

At age 12 we used the original 100-item German version of the CCQ as the standard procedure and as the short CCQ version had only been validated for preschool children. For the specific longitudinal analysis and in order to control for possible methodological differences between the original long and the short version, we only used the 54 items of the short version for the calculation of the prototypicalities at age 12. However, correlations between the prototypicalities of the short and the long version of the CCQ at age 12 were r = 0.98, 0.89, 0.97, and 0.94 for ego-resiliency, ego-undercontrol, aggressiveness, and anxiety, respectively.

Children’s Problem Behavior

Mothers rated their children’s problem behavior during home visits when children were six and seven years old using the VBV-EL (“Verhaltensbeobachtungsbogen für Vorschulkinder”; Döpfner et al., 1993). The VBV-EL is a 53-item standardized German checklist to assess externalizing and internalizing problem behavior and personal resources during the preschool period. It consists of four subscales, including social-emotional competence, oppositional-aggressive behavior, attention-deficit/hyperactivity, and affective problems. The VBV-EL has been applied to clinical and non-clinical samples (Sarimski, 1997; Laucht et al., 2000) and has shown high internal consistencies (alpha = 0.71–0.92), and good test-retest reliability (range r = 0.48–0.78) in clinical samples (Renner et al., 2004). In the present sample, the one-year stabilities of the VBV-EL subscales were good (ranging from r = 0.61 to 0.69). In order to ensure high reliability, we used the mean scale scores of the assessments at age six and seven for further statistical analysis.

Molecular-Genetic Analyses

Genotyping for children and mothers was performed at the Institute of Psychiatry, University of Regensburg (Germany) for the DRD4 exon III repeat polymorphism. Genomic DNA was isolated from buccal swabs using published procedures (Freeman et al., 1997).

For exon III 48-bp VNTR polymorphism in DRD4 primers were 5’ GCG ACT ACG TGG TCT ACT CG 3’ and 5’ AGG ACC CTC ATG GCC TTG 3’. PCR cycling conditions were 15 min for 95°C followed by 35 cycles of 45 s at 95°C (denaturation), 30 s at 50°C (annealing), and 30 s at 72°C (elongation) with a final extension for 7 min at 72°C using a Multicycler PTC 200 gradient machine (Biozym Diagnostik, Germany). PCR products were separated by 2.0% agarose gel electrophoresis and stained with ethidium bromide for UV visualization (Schoots and van Tol, 2003). For the purpose of present analyses and based on previous findings regarding the specific role of the 7-repeat polymorphism, a dichotomous measure of the DRD4 polymorphism was defined. Children were grouped in one group who at least had one 7-repeat allele (DRD4 7+) and another group who did not have any 7-repeat allele (DRD4 7-).

The allele-wise distribution of the children’s DRD4 polymorphism was comparable to European and Middle East populations (Chang et al., 1996). While the most frequent variant was the 4-repeat (67.9%), the frequency of the 7-repeat was 13.2%, and of the 2-repeat was 9.5%. The remaining rare alleles summed up to a frequency of 9.6%. Due to small cell counts for specific DRD4 genotypes, Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium was tested only for combinations of presence and absence of the 7-repeat polymorphisms, which were in the equilibrium, χ2 (2, N = 96) = 0.32, p = 0.85. There was no significant effect of infant sex on the distribution of the DRD4 polymorphisms.

Examining Gene-Environment Interactions

We examined the longitudinal effects of genetic dispositions (DRD4 (7+ vs. 7-) and early caregiving experiences (maternal regulation quality, maternal responsiveness, high vs. low, respectively) on the development of personality and problem behavior. We, therefore, categorized the child variables into three variable groups: self-regulation personality characteristics (ego-resiliency, ego undercontrol), dysregulatory personality measures (aggressiveness, anxiety), and problem behavior (oppositional-aggression, inattentiveness/hyperactivity, emotional problems, and social competence). For each of the groups, a multivariate analysis of variance was conducted with DRD4 (7- vs. 7+) and maternal regulation (low vs. high) as independent factors. For the personality measures, we used age (6 vs. 12 years) as a repeated measures factor in addition. We similarly conducted a MANOVA using maternal responsiveness instead of maternal regulation. In the case of significant overall effects, separate univariate MANOVAs were conducted. To disentangle the interaction effects between DRD4 and maternal behavior, separate maternal regulation × age MANOVAs were conducted for the two DRD4 groups, and separate DRD4 × age MANOVAs were conducted for the two maternal behavior groups, as post-hoc tests.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

We first report the distribution of the children’s DRD4 polymorphisms and examine potential associations of maternal caregiving behavior with their own or their children’s variations of the DRD4 polymorphism.

Twenty-three of the 87 children (26%) with a complete data set possessed at least one DRD4 7-repeat polymorphism (7+), while 74% did not (7-). This is comparable to the total sample, where 24 (25%) of the 95 children showed the DRD4 7+ polymorphism. A MANOVA showed that maternal regulation quality and maternal responsiveness did not differ as a function of children’s DRD4 status (7+ vs. 7-; F(2,91) = 0.72, p = 0.489), indicating that maternal caregiving was not influenced by the DRD4 polymorphisms status of the child. Similarly, maternal caregiving was not related to her own DRD4 polymorphism status (F(2,92) = 0.19, p = 0.828). Thus, differences in mothers’ responsiveness and regulation quality were not associated with genetic differences of the DRD4 polymorphisms of the children or the mothers.

Analyses of gender differences did not show significant effects for CCQ personality measures neither for self-regulation nor for dysregulation. However, boys as compared to girls scored significantly higher on oppositional-aggression (t(97) = 2.2, p = 0.023) and significantly lower on social competence (t(97) = −2.9, p = 0.005). Therefore, gender was included as a covariate for the analyses regarding problem behavior.

Concurrent and Longitudinal Correlations Between Personality Measures and Problem Behavior Measures

Next, we examined the concurrent convergence between the CCQ self-regulation self personality characteristics and the CCQ dysregulation variables for each age group. The correlations between the CCQ-personality variables separately for age six and age 12 (see Table 1) indicate the expected negative association between ego-resiliency and ego-undercontrol, CCQ aggressiveness, as well as CCQ anxiety both at age six and 12. Moreover ego-undercontrol and CCQ aggressiveness were positively associated whereas ego-undercontrol and CCQ anxiety did not correlate significantly at both age six and 12. The correlation between ego-undercontrol and CCQ aggressiveness was significantly higher than the correlation between ego-undercontrol and CCQ anxiety aggressiveness at age six (z = 9.56, p < 0.001) and at age 12 (z = 9.82, p < 0.001). CCQ aggressiveness and CCQ anxiety were not significantly associated.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Correlations between CCQ personality variables at ages six and 12.

Next, we examined the concurrent associations between CCQ personality measures at age six and problem behavior scales at age six (see Table 2). Ego-resiliency was significantly positively associated with social competence and negatively with all forms of problem behavior, while high ego-undercontrol and CCQ aggressiveness were significantly related to low social competence, high oppositional aggressiveness, and inattentiveness/hyperactivity in the problem questionnaire. In addition, CCQ anxiety was significantly positively associated with affective problems in the problem behavior questionnaire.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Concurrent correlations between children’s CCQ personality variables and problem behavior at age six.

An analysis of the stability of the CCQ personality variables over time showed significant and moderately high differential stability over six years (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Longitudinal correlations among CCQ-personality variables at ages six and twelve.

G x E Effects on Self-Regulation: Ego-Resiliency and Ego-Undercontrol

The DRD4 × maternal regulation × age tree-way MANOVA for ego-resiliency and ego-undercontrol revealed a main effect for maternal regulation (F(2,82) = 3.47, p = 0.036, η2 = 0.078), a two-way interaction between maternal regulation and DRD4 (F(2,82) = 3.89, p = 0.024, η2 = 0.087) and a three-way interaction between DRD4, maternal regulation, and age (F(2,82) = 4.73, p = 0.011, η2 = 0.103).

Univariate analyses for ego-resiliency resulted in a significant main effect for maternal regulation (F(1,83) = 5.76, p = 0.019, η2 = 0.065) and a significant interaction between maternal regulation and DRD4 (F(1,83) = 5.52, p = 0.021, η2 = 0.062). Separate maternal regulation × age MANOVAs for the two DRD4 groups did not show significant effects for the DRD4 7- group, but a significant main effect for maternal regulation in children with DRD4 7+ (F(1,20) = 8.6, p = 0.008, η2 = 0.301) indicating lower ego-resiliency in children of mothers with low emotional regulation in the DRD4 7+ group (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Children’s Means (and SE) in ego-resiliency and ego-undercontrol according to DRD4 polymorphism and maternalregulation.

Similarly, separate DRD4 × age MANOVAs for the two maternal regulation groups did not show effects for the group with high maternal regulation quality, but a main effect of DRD4 for children of the low maternal regulation group (F(1,41) = 5.7, p = 0.021, η2 = 0.123), indicating lower ego-resiliency scores for DRD4 7+ children than DRD4 7- children in this group. Thus, as can be seen from Figure 1, both at six and at 12 years, low ego-resiliency mean scores were found for children with DRD4 7+, when they had experienced low maternal regulation, while the other three groups showed comparably high levels of ego-resiliency.

Univariate MANOVAs for ego-undercontrol also revealed an interaction between maternal regulation and DRD4 (F(1,83) = 4.96, p = 0.029, η2 = 0.056). Separate maternal regulation × age MANOVAs for the two DRD4 groups showed a main effect for maternal regulation quality in children with DRD4 7+ (F(1,20) = 6.05, p = 0.032, η2 = 0.232), while separate DRD4 × age MANOVAs for the two maternal regulation groups resulted in a main effect of DRD4 for the group of children with low maternal regulation (F(1,41) = 4.26, p = 0.045, η2 = 0.094). As can be seen from Figure 1, children with DRD4 7+ who experienced low maternal regulation show more ego-undercontrol (at 6 years and at 12 years) than the other three groups exhibiting a moderate amount of ego-undercontrol.

Next, we considered effects of maternal responsiveness. The DRD4 × maternal responsiveness × age three-way MANOVA for ego-resiliency and ego-undercontrol revealed a significant three-way interaction between DRD4, maternal responsiveness, and age (F(2,82) = 4.12, p = 0.020, η2 = 0.091). Univariate MANOVAs did not show a corresponding effect for ego-undercontrol but showed a significant three-way interaction for ego-resiliency (F(1,83) = 8.34, p = 0.005, η2 = 0.091). Separate follow-up ANOVAs revealed a significant interaction between DRD4 and responsiveness at age 12 (F(1,83) = 6.83, p = 0.011). According to post-hoc t-tests, children with less responsive mothers in the DRD4 7+ group at 12 years had lower ego-resiliency scores (M = 0.32) than the DRD4 7- group (M = 0.51), T(41) = 2.36, p = 0.02).

G x E Effects on Dysregulation: Aggressiveness and Anxiety

The DRD4 × maternal regulation × age three-way MANOVA for CCQ aggressiveness and CCQ anxiety revealed significant main effects for maternal regulation (F(2,82) = 3.18, p = 0.050, η2 = 0.071) and age (F(2,82) = 6.91, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.144), a marginal main effect for DRD4 (F(2,82) = 3.05, p = 0.053, η2 = 0.069), and a significant three-way interaction between DRD4, maternal regulation and age (F(2,82) = 5.19, p = 0.008, η2 = 0.075). Univariate analyses for CCQ aggressiveness resulted in a significant main effect for maternal regulation (F(1,83) = 4.46, p = 0.038, η2 = 0.051) and a significant interaction between DRD4 and maternal regulation (F(1,83) = 5.73, p = 0.019, η2 = 0.065), while for CCQ anxiety there were significant main effects for age (F(1,83) = 9.94, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.107) and DRD4 (F(1,83) = 5.36, p = 0.023, η2 = 0.061), and a significant three-way-interaction between DRD4, maternal regulation, and age (F(1,83) = 6.41, p = 0.013, η2 = 0.072).

Regarding aggressiveness, separate maternal regulation × age MANOVAs for the two DRD4 did not show differences in CCQ aggressiveness depending on maternal regulation for the DRD4 7- group, but indicated significantly higher aggressiveness scores for children having experienced low as compared to high maternal regulation in the DRD4 7+ group (F(1,20) = 7.68, p = 0.012, η2 = 0.277). Separate DRD4 × age MANOVAs for the two maternal regulation groups did not show significant effects. Thus, as can be seen in Figure 2, the effect of maternal regulation on CCQ aggressiveness was only found in the DRD4 7+ group. Both at 6 and at 12 years, CCQ aggressiveness of children with DRD4 7+ and low maternal regulation quality was higher in comparison to their counterparts with high quality of maternal regulation, while no differences regarding maternal regulation quality were found in DRD4 7- children.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Children’s Means (and SE) in anxiety and aggressiveness according to DRD4 polymorphism and maternal regulation (Scores are lineary transformed by adding 1 to improve readability).

For CCQ anxiety, separate follow-up ANOVAs for the two age periods revealed a significant main effect for DRD4 (F(1,83) = 5.16, p = 0.026) and a significant interaction between DRD4 and maternal regulation quality (F(1,83) = 4.13, p = 0.045) at age 12 but not at age six. Post-hoc t-tests revealed that at age 12 among the children of mothers with low maternal regulation quality the DRD4 7+ group had higher CCQ anxiety scores than the DRD4 7- group (T(41) = 3.35, p = 0.002, see Figure 2).

The DRD4 × maternal responsiveness × age three-way MANOVA for CCQ aggressiveness and CCQ anxiety revealed a main effect for age (F(2,82) = 6.29, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.133) and a three-way interaction between DRD4, maternal regulation, and age (F(2,82) = 3.31, p = 0.041, η2 = 0.075). Univariate analyses did not show effects for CCQ aggressiveness. However, for CCQ anxiety, there were significant main effects for age (F(1,83) = 9.83, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.106) and DRD4 (F(1,83) = 5.28, p = 0.024, η2 = 0.060) and a significant three-way-interaction between DRD4, maternal regulation, and age (F(1,83) = 5.08, p = 0.027, η2 = 0.056). Separate follow-up ANOVAs for the two age periods only revealed a significant main effect for DRD4 (F(1,83) = 4.98, p = 0.028) at age 12 but not at age six, indicating that DRD4 7+ children had higher CCQ anxiety scores (M = 0.83) than DRD4 7- children (M = 0.76) at age 12.

G x E Effects on Behavior Problems in Middle Childhood

The DRD4 × maternal regulation MANOVA (with gender as a covariate) for the four scales of the problem behavior questionnaire revealed a significant multivariate interaction effect between DRD4 and maternal regulation quality (F(4,83) = 2.71, p = 0.036, η2 = 0.117). Univariate follow-up analyses showed a significant interaction effect for social competence (F(1,85) = 9.97, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.105) and a statistical trend of the interaction for inattentiveness/hyperactivity (F(1,85) = 3.01, p = 0.087, η2 = 0.034), and oppositional-aggression (F(1,85) = 3.72, p = 0.057, η2 = 0.042). Post hoc comparisons of the problem behaviors of children of mothers with low or high maternal regulation quality, conducted separately for the two DRD4 groups and controlling for gender effects revealed a significant effect of maternal regulation on social competence (F(1,19) = 6.87, p = 0.017) indicating lower social competence in children of mothers with maternal low regulation in the DRD4 7+ group. Comparing the two DRD4 groups separately, an analysis for the two maternal regulation groups revealed a significant DRD4 7+ effect on oppositional-aggression (F(1,43) = 4.49, p = 0.040) and social competence (F(1,43) = 9.59, p = 0.003) in the low maternal regulation quality group, indicating more oppositional aggression and less social competence in DRD4 7+ children experiencing low maternal regulation quality already in infancy (see Figure 3). In addition, the children with DRD4 7+ and low maternal regulation compared to the group with high maternal regulation quality showed significantly lower social competence scores (F(1,20) = 11.2, p = 0.003), significantly higher scores in inattentiveness/hyperactivity (F(1,20) = 4.5, p = 0.047), and, as a statistical trend, a higher mean score in oppositional-aggression (F(1,20) = 3.9, p = 0.06).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Children’s Means (and SE) in social competence, oppositional-aggression, inattentiveness/hyperactivity, and affective problems according to DRD4 polymorphism and maternal regulation.

In contrast, the DRD4 × maternal responsiveness two-way MANOVA for the four scales of the problem behavior questionnaire did not reveal significant main or interaction effects.

Discussion

The main objectives of this study were to investigate the effects of early maternal caregiving and molecular genetic polymorphisms of the dopamine receptor gene (DRD4) on self-regulation in middle childhood and adolescence. We examined effects on personality characteristics assessing self-regulation and dysregulation in middle childhood and adolescence, and in addition problem behavior in middle childhood. As the participating families had not experienced any peri-natal or post-natal complications early risk factors influencing later self-regulation at the time of first assessment will not explain differences in self-regulation (Feldman, 2009; Bersted and DiLalla, 2016).

No Genetic Influences on Maternal Sensitivity

We first examined whether differences in maternal sensitivity could be explained by evocative processes based on the children’s genetic dispositions or influenced by maternal genetic variations (Mills-Koonce et al., 2007; Reiss and Leve, 2007), suggesting a gene-environment-correlation (Rutter, 2006). We did not find any effects of mothers’ or children’s DRD4 polymorphisms on maternal sensitivity variables. Thus, the effects of early maternal regulation on the children’s later personality in this study cannot be explained by gene-environment-correlation (Rutter, 2006), neither as an epi-phenomenon of the mothers’ nor the children’s DRD4 polymorphisms. This is in line with other studies examining the effect of genetic dopaminergic polymorphisms reporting no empirical evidence for a gene-environment correlation of supportive parenting and DRD4 polymorphisms (Cho et al., 2016), maternal sensitivity and maternal DRD1 or DRD2 A1 polymorphisms (Mills-Koonce et al., 2007; Mileva-Seitz et al., 2012). Maternal sensitivity as the appropriate adjustment and regulation of one’s own parenting behaviors to children’s signals might be less affected by typical dopaminergic functions (i.e., regulation of attention, inhibition, or reaction to reward). However, the frequency of single, specific parenting behaviors (e.g., orientation to child) might well be influenced by genetic differences of the dopaminergic system (Mileva-Seitz et al., 2012) and other aspects of parenting may show systematic variations depending on polygenetic scores (Wertz et al., 2019). However, other factors explaining differences in maternal sensitivity may play a role (e.g., mother’s attachment history or maternal depression; Pederson et al., 1998; Grossmann et al., 2008; Bernard et al., 2018) or other genetic differences (Mileva-Seitz et al., 2011). In general, the results offer no empirical evidence that the two dimensions of maternal sensitivity assessed in this study, maternal regulation quality and maternal responsiveness, are a result of specific differences in mothers’ DRD4 7+ vs. 7-polymorphisms or an evoked reactive reaction based on DRD4 variation of the child. Thus, we interpret maternal effective regulation and responsiveness as independent individual psychological factors influencing later child development.

Ego-Resiliency and Ego-Undercontrol: Continuity and Associations With Dysregulation and Maladjustment

The study shows a moderate stability of ego-resiliency and ego-control in this sample which is comparable to the original study by Block and Block (2006) and a Swedish longitudinal study (Chuang et al., 2006; Syed et al., 2020) and is close to the mean stability of personality traits at that age as reported in a meta-analysis (Roberts and DelVecchio, 2000). The moderate differential stability suggests some changes in rank order of all personality characteristics. Thus, we still find differential developmental trajectories in personality development with both growth and decline in self-regulation and dysregulation between middle childhood and early adolescence which may also be due to other reasons like differences in the onset of puberty within the sample (Block and Block, 2006).

Moreover, the study shows that ego-resiliency in middle childhood and in adolescence as a sign of flexible self-regulation is clearly associated with lower scores in aggressiveness and anxiety at age six and age twelve (i.e., personality traits assessing dysregulation) and also with fewer problem behaviors and more social competence in middle childhood. These findings corroborate the relevance of ego-resiliency as a fundamental dimension of effective self-regulation and adjustment (Block and Block, 2006; Taylor et al., 2014; Syed et al., 2020). Ego-resiliency is a marker of effective and situation-appropriate adjustment of self-control and goes along with both low internalizing and low externalizing symptoms from early childhood to adolescence (Scholte et al., 2005; Martel and Nigg, 2006; Hofer et al., 2010; Meier and Zimmermann, 2018; Zimmermann et al., 2022).

Interestingly, ego-undercontrol shows a differential pattern of associations with concurrent signs of externalizing (i.e., aggressiveness, inattention-hyperactivity) and internalizing (i.e., anxiety). Ego-undercontrol is positively associated with concurrent CCQ aggressiveness at age six and age twelve. Moreover, at age six it is also significantly correlated with concurrent oppositional aggression and inattentive-hyperactivity. Thus, children who immediately express or act according to their current needs, emotions, or goals, regardless of context, and who demonstrate poor delay of gratification show more externalizing symptoms and are rated low in social competence. In contrast, ego-undercontrol is negatively associated with CCQ anxiety at age six and negatively but not significantly at age twelve. The significantly different correlations of ego-undercontrol with aggressiveness compared to anxiety are in line with other research (Krueger et al., 1996) and can already be found in early childhood (Zimmermann et al., 2022). It characterizes the specific theoretical concept of ego-control as a continuum from undercontrol to overcontrol (Funder and Block, 1989; Block, 2002; Block and Block, 2006). Thus, anxiety, assessed as a personality trait here, may not only be a sign of hyperactivating emotion regulation (i.e., low self-regulation) but also a sign of constant overcontrol. Recent research supports Block’s theoretical perspective by showing a U-shaped association between self-control and mental health problems (Hassan and Schmidt, 2021) where children with low levels of self-control and also children with high levels of self-control show increased internalizing and externalizing problems. Self-regulation and self-control are sometimes conceptually confounded in research but may have different implications for adjustment or clinical symptoms (Nigg, 2017).

Gene-Environment Effects

In the present study, two main results of the gene x environment effects on personality development are of special interest. First, there are main effects for early maternal caregiving and a moderation of the genetic disposition associated with the long (7+) variant of the DRD4 polymorphism by early maternal caregiving. This is the case for all three proposed levels of self-regulation capacities, personality characteristics assessing self-regulation, personality characteristics assessing dysregulation, and also the third level of enduring problems with self-regulation for most mental health problem domains and social competence. Second, specifically effective maternal regulation quality but not maternal responsiveness is a moderator of DRD4 polymorphism in these gene-environment interactions.

The general pattern of results shows that effective early maternal regulation in infancy predicts variation in self-regulation (specifically ego-resiliency) and in dysregulation (specifically aggressiveness). The significant interaction effects found for personality differences in self-regulation and dysregulation and also for problem behavior (i.e., all three levels of regulatory capacity) suggest that, for the group of children with the DRD4 7+ variant, effective maternal regulation experienced already in infancy can compensate for the negative genetic disposition associated with the DRD4 7+ variant on flexible self-regulation as a personality trait. This is in concordance with other studies on gene × environment interaction for DRD4 polymorphisms (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn, 2006; Sheese et al., 2007; Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn, 2011; Martel et al., 2011) and also for DRD2 A1 polymorphisms (Mills-Koonce et al., 2007; Waldman, 2007). However, some studies assessing other parenting variables and other aspects of children’s self-regulation do not report similar results. This can be seen in the study by Sheese et al. (2007), where parenting quality was assessed with an aggregated score of supportive presence, autonomy support, cognitive stimulation, and low hostility. Here, low (aggregate) parenting quality and DRD4 7+ disposition resulted in a low level of sensation seeking (defined as impulsive, cheerful activity) but the study showed no gene-environment interaction for effortful control (an aggregate of attention regulation and inhibitory control) as a sign of self-control. We conclude that not all parenting variables may function as moderators of genetic predispositions for all self-control variables. As a consequence, we need to differentiate specific aspects of parenting or parental sensitivity that may help infants to develop enduring self-regulation from other domains of parental sensitivity that do not have the same effect. Many studies on gene-environment-interaction only use a global or aggregated score of sensitivity, parenting, or caregiving, often only because the single caregiving variables are correlated. However, correlations between different measures of sensitivity often are only modest or rather modest (Lohaus et al., 2004; Bohr et al., 2018). Future studies should have a closer look at the differential functions of caregiving variables and contexts for the development of attachment and self-regulation in gene-environment studies (Golds et al., 2020; Picardi et al., 2020).

Direct effects of the DRD4 polymorphisms on children’s personality only appeared for CCQ anxiety, specifically at age twelve. Thus, although the personality variables chosen in this study are all related to differences in functions of the dopamine system (inhibition and regulation of impulses, emotions, and behavior) a direct main effect of DRD4 does not explain the developmental pathway of personality development for all of these variables. As we do not have data on the mental health problems or problem behavior at age 12 we cannot examine whether the results on affective problems at age six also would replicate at age 12. This warrants further longitudinal assessments.

In sum, the results suggest that children with the DRD4 7+ variant and without early effectively regulating parenting already in infancy will develop less effective self-regulatory abilities. For these children it is highly relevant to experience early effective external regulation to learn the ability to flexibly control and modify their behavioral and emotional reactions in early caregiver-child interaction. The results seem to support the notion of children’s differences in susceptibility for environmental conditions, specifically effective parenting (Belsky and Pluess, 2013; Belsky and van IJzendoorn, 2017). Thus, while the personality development of children with the 7+ variant of the DRD4 polymorphism depends on specific qualities of maternal caregiving (i.e., early effective regulation) children with the 7- variant seem not to be affected by differences in such early maternal caregiving. This is in line with other studies on gene × environment effects (Caspi et al., 2002). The role of the dopaminergic system in the development of self-regulation in childhood may be especially seen rather early in development (Posner et al., 2007). According to Posner and Rothbart (2022), dopamine is a specific modulator of the executive function domain of the attention network, where DRD4 polymorphisms already have an early functional impact during development. The dopamine pathways are involved in motor control and planning, cognitive processes, and reward processing and therefore influence the regulation of attention but also of emotions and motivation (Nieoullon and Coquerel, 2003). Animal studies suggest, that specifically the flexibility and efficiency of self-regulation are impaired. Some research suggests that the DRD4 7+ variant is associated with reduced dopaminergic signaling leading to reduced learning from external stimuli or caregiving (Tripp and Wickens, 2008). Therefore, by reducing the child’s emotional arousal, effective maternal regulation may be more effective in fostering children’s early self-regulation than fast maternal responding.

Besides the children’s susceptibility, effective maternal regulation in infancy was more influential than maternal responsiveness in this study. While effective maternal regulation showed a repeated and systematic effect on different personality measures at different ages maternal responsiveness only showed an effect on ego-resiliency at age 12. This suggests that the caregiver’s ability to effectively regulate the infant’s negative emotions as well as to assist the child effectively during exploration seems to help the child to develop effective and adaptive patterns of self-regulation (Thompson, 1993; Spangler et al., 1994; Grossmann et al., 2008; Bernier et al., 2010). In contrast, other aspects of sensitivity like vigilant and fast reactivity to children’s signals may not always be a sign of supportive caregiving if combined with problems in effectively regulating the child’s emotional states or if combined with actively dysregulating the child by being intrusive. Thus, prompt maternal reactions alone do not necessarily support children during emotion regulation processes and children consequently do not learn effective self-regulation even when parents are highly vigilant to their emotional expressions. Similarly, Lohaus et al. (2004) reported that reactions appropriate to infants’ needs, but not signal perception of infants’ expression was predictive of a low rate of infant crying, a sign of effective self-regulation (Lohaus et al., 2004). Moreover, parental sensitivity and parental intrusiveness may differentially influence the intercept and slope of self-control development (Geeraerts et al., 2021). For other genetic predispositions and also for other developmental outcomes besides self-regulation, specific aspects of sensitivity or caregiving by both mothers and fathers or attachment security may well play an important role (Spangler et al., 2009; Zimmermann et al., 2009; Davies et al., 2015; Zimmermann and Spangler, 2016; Baptista et al., 2017; Neppl et al., 2020; Lee et al., 2021).

The effect of early maternal regulation for the development of self-regulation and for problem behavior in childhood and early adolescence suggests the importance of early sensitive experiences on personality development. Following the model of stage-salient issues (Sroufe, 1989), effective regulation of the infant’s negative emotions is the earliest stage-salient issue with potential effects on later stage-salient issues influencing subsequent personality development and adjustment (Cicchetti and Toth, 1992; Toth and Manly, 2019). Studies on long-term effects of early sensitivity provide support for this assumption (Beckwith et al., 1992; Grossmann et al., 2002). However, given the gene × environment interaction effects of DRD4 and maternal regulation found in this study, the long-term effects of early maternal regulation might specifically help children with a genetic disposition for problems to adjust their behavioral expressions of own emotions, needs, or goals to the affordances of the current situation, obvious as behavioral dysregulation.

For problem behavior, we found a main effect for maternal regulation quality and a similar interaction effect between maternal regulation quality and DRD4 polymorphism. Again, early regulation of the children’s emotional and exploratory needs helps children with the 7+ variant of the DRD4 polymorphism to reach a comparably high level of social competence and a comparably low level of oppositional aggression and attention-hyperactivity problems similar to the children with the DRD4 7- variant. A similar compensatory effect of maternal sensitivity for carriers of the DRD4 7+ variant on externalizing behavior in preschool children was reported by Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn (2006). Many studies have shown that the DRD4 7+ polymorphism increases the risk for the development of ADHD (Thapar et al., 2007b; Bonvicini et al., 2020) and contributes to its stability over time (El-Faddagh et al., 2004). However, the base rate of children diagnosed with ADHD showing the DRD4 7+ polymorphism is seldom higher than 50%. The present study suggests that even a biological predisposition for attention-hyperactivity problems and oppositional aggression might well be compensated quite early in life, leading to at least an average ability for self-regulation that can be helpful for adjustment and adaptive self-regulation at a non-clinical level. Thus, social influences on the development of attention problems and hyperactivity should be considered more closely as a second developmental pathway showing equifinality (Carlson et al., 1995; Pauli-Pott et al., 2018) when maternal regulation is low and given the DRD4 7 repeat polymorphism. In future research, the timing of caregiving effects on self-regulation needs to be explored in more detail (Weeland et al., 2015).

The present study has some limitations that need to be considered. The self-regulation variables and the dysregulation variables were assessed with the same instrument, potentially increasing their shared variability. The effect sizes for the main effects for maternal regulation and the interaction effects of DRD4 polymorphisms and maternal caregiving are relatively small and also the number of children with the 7+ polymorphism, although comparable to other studies published in the field. In addition, the effects of cumulative environmental risks (Caspi et al., 2003; El-Faddagh et al., 2004; Reiner and Spangler, 2011) or the interaction with other genetic variations (Schmidt et al., 2007; Wertz et al., 2019) could offer even more insights in developmental processes. Moreover, a GWAS approach might be more representative of the complex interplay of genetic variations on personality development and adjustment. However, tandem repeats seem to have more impact on the development of self-regulation or mental health problems than expected earlier (Xiao et al., 2021). Thus, replications are required as well as research designs with additional assessment waves to identify developmental processes of self-regulation and dysregulation and to examine the relevance of early compared to later caregiving or other social experiences more closely.

Conclusion

We conclude that especially early effective maternal regulation of infants’ distress can help children with the DRD4 7+ variant to develop adaptive self-regulation in middle childhood and adolescence. We suggest that research on gene × environment effects not only should seek for all possible genetic variations influencing self-regulation but simultaneously also should more closely examine the environment and the existing variations in caregiving behaviors to better understand developmental processes in the interplay of social and genetic influences on personality development and (mal-) adjustment.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of the German Psychological Association. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

Both authors contributed equally to the study design, the statistical analysis, and the writing and editing of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research has been supported by the Koehler-Stiftung (Munich, Germany) and the German Research Foundation (SP 312/16-1 and ZI 511/13-1). We acknowledge support from the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Wuppertal.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant Care and the Growth of Love. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Google Scholar

Arend, R., Gove, F. L., and Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Continuity of individual adaptation from infancy to kindergarten: a predictive study of ego-resiliency and curiosity in preschoolers. Child Dev. 50, 950–959. doi: 10.2307/1129319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2006). Gene-environment interaction of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) and observed maternal insensitivity predicting externalizing behavior in pre-schoolers. Dev. Psychobiol. 48, 406–409. doi: 10.1002/dev.20152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to rearing environment depending on dopamine-related genes: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Dev. Psychopathol. 23, 39–52. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baptista, J., Belsky, J., Mesquita, A., and Soares, I. (2017). Serotonin transporter polymorphism moderates the effects of caregiver intrusiveness on ADHD symptoms among institutionalized preschoolers. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 26, 303–313. doi: 10.1007/s00787-016-0890-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckwith, L., Rodning, C., and Cohen, S. (1992). Preterm children at early adolescence and continuity and discontinuity in maternal responsiveness from infancy. Child Dev. 63, 1198–1208. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01689.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: a process model. Child Dev. 55, 83–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1984.tb00275.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J., and Pluess, M. (2013). Beyond risk, resilience and dysregulation: phenotypic plasticity and human development. Dev. Psychopathol. 25, 1243–1261. doi: 10.1017/S095457941300059X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2017). Genetic differential susceptibility to the effects of parenting. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 15, 125–130. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernard, K., Nissim, G., Vaccaro, S., Harris, J. L., and Lindhiem, O. (2018). Association between maternal depression and maternal sensitivity from birth to 12 months: a meta-analysis. Attach. Hum. Dev. 20, 578–599. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1430839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., and Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Dev. 81, 326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, D., Deater-Deckard, K., McCartney, K., Wang, Z., and Petrill, S. A. (2013). Gene-environment interaction between dopamine receptor D4 7-repeat polymorphism and early maternal sensitivity predicts inattention trajectories across middle childhood. Dev. Psychopathol. 25, 291–306. doi: 10.1017/S095457941200106X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bersted, K. A., and DiLalla, L. F. (2016). The influence of DRD4 genotype and perinatal complications on preschoolers’ negative emotionality. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 42, 71–79. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2015.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birkas, E., Horváth, J., Lakatos, K., Nemoda, Z., Sasvari-Szekely, M., Winkler, I., et al. (2006). Association between dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene polymorphisms and novelty-elicited auditory event-related potentials in preschool children. Brain Res. 1103, 150–158. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Block, J. (2002). Personality as an Affect-Processing System: Toward an Integrative Theory. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Block, J. H., and Block, J. (1980). “The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the organization of behavior,” in Development of Cognition, Affect and Social Relations: The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, (Vol. 13), ed W. A. Collins (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum).

Google Scholar

Block, J., and Block, J. H. (2006). Venturing a 30-year longitudinal study. Am. Psychol. 61, 315–327. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.4.315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Block, J., Kogan, N., and Block, J. H. (1998). Parental teaching strategies and children’s cognitive style. Int. J. Edu. Res. 29, 187–204. doi: 10.1016/S0883-0355(98)00024-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohr, Y., Putnick, D. L., Lee, Y., and Bornstein, M. H. (2018). Evaluating caregiver sensitivity to infants: measures matter. Infancy 23, 730–747. doi: 10.1111/infa.12248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonvicini, C., Cortese, S., Maj, C., Baune, B. T., Faraone, S. V., and Scassellati, C. (2020). DRD4 48 bp multiallelic variants as age-population-specific biomarkers in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Transl. Psychiatry 10:70. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-0755-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and Loss: Volume II: Separation, Anxiety and Anger. London: Hogarth Press.

Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1988). Developmental psychiatry comes of age. Am. J. Psychiatry 145, 1–10. doi: 10.1176/ajp.145.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bridgett, D. J., Burt, N. M., Edwards, E. S., and Deater-Deckard, K. (2015). Intergenerational transmission of self-regulation: A multidisciplinary review and integrative conceptual framework. Psychol. Bull. 141, 602–654. doi: 10.1037/a0038662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldwell, J. G., and Shaver, P. R. (2012). Exploring the cognitive-emotional pathways between adult attachment and ego-resiliency. Individ. Diff. Res. 10, 141–152.

Google Scholar

Carlson, E. A., Jacobvitz, D., and Sroufe, L. A. (1995). A developmental investigation of inattentiveness and hyperactivity. Child Dev. 66, 37–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00854.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science 297, 851–854. doi: 10.1126/science.1072290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., et al. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 301, 386–389. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, F. M., Kidd, J. R., Livak, K. J., Pakstis, A. J., and Kidd, K. K. (1996). The worldwide distribution of allele frequencies at the human dopamine D4 receptor locus. Hum. Genet. 98, 91–101. doi: 10.1007/s004390050166

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, J., Kogan, S. M., and Brody, G. H. (2016). Genetic moderation of transactional relations between parenting practices and child self-regulation. J. Fam. Psychol. 30, 780–790. doi: 10.1037/fam0000228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chuang, S. S., Lamb, M. E., and Hwang, C. P. (2006). Personality development from childhood to adolescence: a longitudinal study of ego-control and ego-resiliency in Sweden. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 30, 338–343. doi: 10.1177/0165025406072795

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cicchetti, D., and Toth, S. L. (1992). The role of developmental theory in prevention and intervention. Dev. Psychopathol. 4, 489–493. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400004831

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, P., Cicchetti, D., and Hentges, R. F. (2015). Maternal unresponsiveness and child disruptive problems: the interplay of uninhibited temperament and dopamine transporter genes. Child Dev. 86, 63–79. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DiLalla, L. F., Elam, K. K., and Smolen, A. (2009). Genetic and gene-environment interaction effects on preschoolers’ social behaviors. Dev. Psychobiol. 51, 451–464. doi: 10.1002/dev.20384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Döpfner, M., Berner, W., Fleischmann, T., and Schmidt, M. (1993). VerhaltensBeurteilungsbogen für Vorschulkinder (VBV 3–6). Weinheim: Beltz.

Google Scholar

Ebstein, R. P., Benjamin, J., and Belmaker, R. H. (2000). Personality and polymorphisms of genes involved in aminergic neurotransmission. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 410, 205–214. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(00)00852-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebstein, R. P., Levine, J., Geller, V., Auerbach, J., Gritsenko, I., and Belmaker, R. H. (1998). Dopamine D4 receptor and serotonin transporter promoter in the determination of neonatal temperament. Mol. Psychiatry 3, 238–246. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elam, K. K., and DiLalla, L. F. (2018). Minute-to-minute trajectories of child unresponsiveness and parent sensitivity in parent-child interactions: the role of DRD4. Soc. Dev. 27, 952–966. doi: 10.1111/sode.12308

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El-Faddagh, M., Laucht, M., Maras, A., Vohringer, L., and Schmidt, M. H. (2004). Associations of dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in a high-risk community sample: a longitudinal study from birth to 11 years of age. J. Neural Transm. (Vienna) 111, 883–889. doi: 10.1007/s00702-003-0054-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., Perlis, R. H., Doyle, A. E., Smoller, J. W., Goralnick, J. J., Holmgren, M. A., et al. (2005). Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1313–1323. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2009). The development of regulatory functions from birth to 5 years: insights from premature infants. Child Dev. 80, 544–561. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01278.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, B., Powell, J., Ball, D., Hill, L., Craig, I., and Plomin, R. (1997). DNA by mail: an inexpensive and noninvasive method for collecting DNA samples from widely dispersed populations. Behav. Genet. 27, 251–257. doi: 10.1023/a:1025614231190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funder, D. C., and Block, J. (1989). The role of ego-control, ego-resiliency and IQ in delay of gratitication in adolescence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1041–1050. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.57.6.1041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganiban, J. M., Liu, C., Zappaterra, L., An, S., Natsuaki, M. N., Neiderhiser, J. M., et al. (2021). Gene × environment interactions in the development of preschool effortful control and its implications for childhood externalizing behavior. Behav. Genet. 51, 448–462. doi: 10.1007/s10519-021-10073-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geeraerts, S. B., Endendijk, J. J., Deković, M., Huijding, J., Deater-Deckard, K., and Mesman, J. (2021). Inhibitory control across the preschool years: developmental changes and associations with parenting. Child Dev. 92, 335–350. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golds, L., De Kruiff, K., and MacBeth, A. (2020). Disentangling genes, attachment and environment: a systematic review of the developmental psychopathology literature on gene-environment interactions and attachment. Dev. Psychopathol. 32, 357–381. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419000142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Göttert, R., and Asendorpf, J. (1989). Eine deutsche version des California-child-Q-sort: Kurzform. (German short version of the California child Q-Set). Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie 21, 70–82.

Google Scholar

Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Kindler, H., and Zimmermann, P. (2008). “A wider view of attachment and exploration: the influence of mothers and fathers on the development of psychological security from infancy to young adulthood,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications, 2nd Edn., eds J. Cassidy, and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 857–879.

Google Scholar

Grossmann, K. E., Grossmann, K., Winter, M., and Zimmermann, P. (2002). “Attachment relationships and appraisal of partnership: from early experience of sensitive support to later relationship representation,” in Paths to Successful Development, eds L. Pulkkinen, and A. Caspi (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 73–105. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511489761.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grusec, J. E., and Davidov, M. (2010). Integrating different perspectives on socialization theory and research: a domain-specific approach. Child Dev. 81, 687–709. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01426.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunnar, M., and Pollak, S. D. (2007). Supporting parents so that they can support their internationally adopted children: the larger challenge lurking behind the fatality statistics. Child Maltreat. 12, 381–382. doi: 10.1177/1077559507306719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunnar, M., and Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 145–173. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hassan, R., and Schmidt, L. A. (2021). Inhibitory control, dyadic social behavior and mental health difficulties in preschoolers. Child Dev. . [Online ahead of print]. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hentges, R. F., Krug, C. M. W., Shaw, D. S., Wilson, M. N., Dishion, T. J., and Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2020). The long-term indirect effect of the early family check-Up intervention on adolescent internalizing and externalizing symptoms via inhibitory control. Dev. Psychopathol. 32, 1544–1554. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419001482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofer, C., Eisenberg, N., and Reiser, M. (2010). The role of socialization, effortful control and ego resiliency in French adolescents’ social functioning. J. Res. Adolesc. 20, 555–582. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00650.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karreman, A., van Tuijl, C., van Aken, M. A. G., and Dekovic, M. (2006). Parenting and self-regulation in preschoolers: a meta-analysis. Infant Child Dev. 15, 561–579. doi: 10.1002/icd.478

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kersten-Alvarez, L. E., Hosman, C. M. H., Riksen-Walraven, J. M., Van Doesum, K. T. M., and Hoefnagels, C. (2010). Long-term effects of a home-visiting intervention for depressed mothers and their infants. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 51, 1160–1170. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02268.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., and Manly, J. T. (2009). Child maltreatment and trajectories of personality and behavioral functioning: implications for the development of personality disorder. Dev. Psychopathol. 21, 889–912. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, A. P., Muzik, M., Hamilton, L., Taylor, A. B., Rosenblum, K. L., and Liberzon, I. (2016). Dopamine receptor gene DRD4 7-repeat allele X maternal sensitivity interaction on child externalizing behavior problems: independent replication of effects at 18 months. PLoS One 11:e0160473. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobak, R. R., and Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: working models, affect regulation and representations of self and others. Child Dev. 59, 135–146. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb03201.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., and Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: continuity and change, antecedents and implications for social development. Dev. Psychol. 36, 220–232. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kremen, A. M., and Block, J. (1998). The roots of ego-control in young adulthood: links with parenting in early childhood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75, 1062–1075. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.4.1062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., White, J., and Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1996). Delay of gratification, psychopathology and personality: is low self-control specific to externalizing problems? J. Pers. 64, 107–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1996.tb00816.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakatos, K., Nemoda, Z., Birkas, E., Ronai, Z., Kovacs, E., Ney, K., et al. (2003). Association of D4 dopamine receptor gene and serotonin transporter promoter polymorphisms with infants’ response to novelty. Mol. Psychiatry 8, 90–97. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001212

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laucht, M., Esser, G., and Schmidt, M. H. (2000). Längsschnittforschung zur Entwicklungsepidemiologie psychischer Störungen: Zielsetzung, Konzeption und zentrale Ergebnisse der Mannheimer Risikolängsschnittstudie. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie: Forschung und Praxis 29, 246–262. doi: 10.1026//0084-5345.29.4.246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. K., Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J., and Beauchaine, T. P. (2021). Serotonergic sensitivity alleles moderate relations between attachment security at age three and socioemotional competence at age five. Dev. Psychobiol. 63, 698–712. doi: 10.1002/dev.22042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lohaus, A., Keller, H., Ball, J., Voelker, S., and Elben, C. (2004). Maternal sensitivity in interactions with three- and 12-month-old infants: stability, structural composition and developmental consequences. Infant Child Dev. 13, 235–252. doi: 10.1002/icd.351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manuck, S. B., and McCaffery, J. M. (2014). Gene-environment interaction. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 41–70. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martel, M. M., and Nigg, J. T. (2006). Child ADHD and personality/temperament traits of reactive and effortful control, resiliency and emotionality. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 1175–1183. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01629.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martel, M. M., Nigg, J. T., Wong, M. M., Fitzgerald, H. E., Jester, J. M., Puttler, L. I., et al. (2007). Childhood and adolescent resiliency, regulation and executive functioning in relation to adolescent problems and competence in a high-risk sample. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 541–563. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martel, M. M., Nikolas, M., Jernigan, K., Friderici, K., Waldman, I., and Nigg, J. T. (2011). The dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4) moderates family environmental effects on ADHD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 39, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9439-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meier, S. A., and Zimmermann, P. (2018). Personality assessment as contribution to diagnostic differentiation between ADHD and RAD in middle childhood. Prax. Kinderpsychol. Kinderpsychiatr. 67, 510–529. doi: 10.13109/prkk.2018.67.6.510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mileva-Seitz, V., Fleming, A. S., Meaney, M. J., Mastroianni, A., Sinnwell, J. P., Steiner, M., et al. (2012). Dopamine receptors D1 and D2 are related to observed maternal behavior. Genes Brain Behav. 11, 684–694. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00804.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mileva-Seitz, V., Kennedy, J., Atkinson, L., Steiner, M., Levitan, R., Matthews, S. G., et al. (2011). Serotonin transporter allelic variation in mothers predicts maternal sensitivity, behavior and attitudes toward 6-month-old infants. Genes Brain Behav. 10, 325–333. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00671.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mills-Koonce, W. R., Propper, C. B., Gariepy, J.-L., Blair, C., Garrett-Peters, P., and Cox, M. J. (2007). Bidirectional genetic and environmental influences of mother and child behavior: the family system as the unit of analyses. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 1073–1087. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., and Rutter, M. (2006). Measured gene-environment interactions in psychopathology: concepts, research strategies and implications for research, intervention and public understanding of genetics. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1, 5–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00002.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neppl, T. K., Jeon, S., Diggs, O., and Donnellan, M. B. (2020). Positive parenting, effortful control and developmental outcomes across early childhood. Dev. Psychol. 56, 444–454. doi: 10.1037/dev0000874

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieoullon, A., and Coquerel, A. (2003). Dopamine: a key regulator to adapt action, emotion, motivation and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 16, S3–S9. doi: 10.1097/00019052-200312002-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigg, J. T. (2017). On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 58, 361–383. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nikitopoulos, J., Zohsel, K., Blomeyer, D., Buchmann, A. F., Schmid, B., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., et al. (2014). Are infants differentially sensitive to parenting? early maternal care, DRD4 genotype and externalizing behavior during adolescence. J. Psychiatric Res. 59, 53–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, T. G., Marvin, R. S., Rutter, M., Olrick, J. T., and Britner, P. A. (2003). Child-parent attachment following early institutional deprivation. Dev. Psychopathol. 15, 19–38. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pappa, I., Mileva-Seitz, V. R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Tiemeier, H., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). The magnificent seven: a quantitative review of dopamine receptor d4 and its association with child behavior. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 57, 175–186. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pauli-Pott, U., Schloß, S., and Becker, K. (2018). Maternal responsiveness as a predictor of self-regulation development and attention-deficit/hyperactivity symptoms across preschool ages. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 49, 42–52. doi: 10.1007/s10578-017-0726-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pederson, D. R., Gleason, K. E., Moran, G., and Bento, S. (1998). Maternal attachment representations, maternal sensitivity and the infant-mother attachment relationship. Dev. Psychol. 34, 925–933. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.34.5.925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Picardi, A., Giuliani, E., and Gigantesco, A. (2020). Genes and environment in attachment. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 112, 254–269. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, R. Q., Soares, I., Carvalho-Correia, E., and Mesquita, A. R. (2015). Gene-environment interactions in psychopathology throughout early childhood: a systematic review. Psychiatric Genet. 25, 223–233. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0000000000000106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollak, S. D., and Sinha, P. (2002). Effects of early experience on children’s recognition of facial displays of emotion. Dev. Psychol. 38, 784–791. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., and Rothbart, M. K. (2022). “Genetic and experiential factors in brain development,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Development, eds O. Houdé and G. Borst (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 105–121.

Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., and Tang, Y. (2007). The anterior cingulate gyrus and the mechanism of self-regulation. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 391–395. doi: 10.3758/cabn.7.4.391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poustka, L., Zohsel, K., Blomeyer, D., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Schmid, B., Trautmann-Villalba, P., et al. (2015). Interacting effects of maternal responsiveness, infant regulatory problems and dopamine D4 receptor gene in the development of dysregulation during childhood: a longitudinal analysis. J. Psychiatr. Res. 70, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Propper, C., Willoughby, M., Halpern, C. T., Carbone, M. A., and Cox, M. (2007). Parenting quality, DRD4 and the prediction of externalizing and internalizing behaviors in early childhood. Dev. Psychobiol. 49, 619–632. doi: 10.1002/dev.20249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiner, I., and Spangler, G. (2011). Dopamine D4 receptor exon III polymorphism, adverse life events and personality traits in a nonclinical German adult sample. Neuropsychobiology 63, 52–58. doi: 10.1159/000322291

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiss, D., and Leve, L. D. (2007). Genetic expression outside the skin: clues to mechanisms of genotype × environment interaction. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 1005–1027. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000508

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renner, G., Zenglein, C., and Krampen, G. (2004). Reliability and validity of the “Verhaltensbeurteilungsbogen für Vorschulkinder” (VBV 3-6), a German checklist for behavior problems and behavior disorders at preschool age. Diagnostica 50, 78–87. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924.50.2.78

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, B. W., and DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychol. Bull. 126, 3–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robins, R. W., John, O. P., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., and Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1996). Resilient, overcontrolled and undercontrolled boys: three replicable personality types. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 157–171. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.70.1.157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothbaum, F., and Weisz, J. R. (1994). Parental caregiving and child externalizing behavior in nonclinical samples: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 116, 55–74. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.1.55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutter, M. (2006). Genes and Behavior: Nature-Nurture Interplay Explained. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

Google Scholar

Sameroff, A. (2010). A unified theory of development: a dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Dev. 81, 6–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01378.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarimski, K. (1997). Behavioural phenotypes and family stress in three mental retardation syndromes. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 6, 26–31. doi: 10.1007/BF00573637

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savitz, J. B., and Ramesar, R. S. (2004). Genetic variants implicated in personality: a review of the more promising candidates. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 131B, 20–32. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., and Hamer, D. H. (2007). Evidence for a gene-gene interaction in predicting children’s behavior problems: association of serotonin transporter short and dopamine receptor D4 long genotypes with internalizing and externalizing behaviors in typically developing 7-year-olds. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 1105–1116. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Perez-Edgar, K., Hu, S., and Hamer, D. H. (2001). Association of DRD4 with attention problems in normal childhood development. Psychiatr. Genet. 11, 25–29. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200103000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholte, R. H., van Lieshout, C. F., de Wit, C. A., and van Aken, M. A. (2005). Adolescent personality types and subtypes and their psychosocial adjustment. Merrill-Palmer Q. 51, 258–286. Available online at: https://www.jstor.org/stable/23096038

Google Scholar

Schoots, O., and van Tol, H. H. M. (2003). The human dopamine D4 receptor repeat sequences modulate expression. Pharmacogenomics J. 3, 343–348. doi: 10.1038/sj.tpj.6500208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheese, B. E., Voelker, P. M., Rothbart, M. K., and Posner, M. I. (2007). Parenting quality interacts with genetic variation in dopamine receptor D4 to influence temperament in early childhood. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 1039–1046. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Kennedy, M., Kumsta, R., Knights, N., Golm, D., Rutter, M., et al. (2017). Child-to-adult neurodevelopmental and mental health trajectories after early life deprivation: the young adult follow-up of the longitudinal English and Romanian Adoptees study. Lancet 389, 1539–1548. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30045-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spangler, G., Johann, M., Ronai, Z., and Zimmermann, P. (2009). Genetic and environmental influence on attachment disorganization. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 952–961. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02054.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spangler, G., and Schieche, M. (1998). Emotional and adrenocortical responses of infants to the strange situation: the differential function of emotional expression. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 22, 681–706. doi: 10.1080/016502598384126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spangler, G., Schieche, M., Ilg, U., Maier, U., and Ackermann, C. (1994). Maternal sensitivity as an external organizer for biobehavioral regulation in infancy. Dev. Psychobiol. 27, 425–437. doi: 10.1002/dev.420270702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sroufe, L. A. (1989). “Relationships, self and individual adaptation,” in Relationship Disturbances in Early Childhood. A Developmental Approach, eds A. J. Sameroff and R. N. Emde (New York: Basic Books), 70–94.

Google Scholar

Stams, G.-J. J. M., Juffer, F., van IJzendoorn, M. H., and Hocksbergen, R. C. (2001). Attachment-based intervention in adoptive families in infancy and children’s development at age 7: two follow-up studies. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 19, 159–180. doi: 10.1348/026151001166010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suess, G. J., Grossmann, K. E., and Sroufe, L. A. (1992). Effects of infant attachment to mother and father on quality of adaptation in preschool: from dyadic to individual organisation of self. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 15, 43–65. doi: 10.1177/016502549201500103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Syed, M., Eriksson, P. L., Frisén, A., Hwang, C. P., and Lamb, M. E. (2020). Personality development from age 2 to 33: stability and change in ego resiliency and ego control and associations with adult adaptation. Dev. Psychol. 56, 815–832. doi: 10.1037/dev0000895

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, Z. E., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., and Widaman, K. F. (2013). Longitudinal relations of intrusive parenting and effortful control to ego-resiliency during early childhood. Child Dev. 84, 1145–1151. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, A., and Kim-Cohen, J. (2007). Meta-analysis of gene-environment interactions in developmental psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 1029–1037. doi: 10.1017/S095457940700051X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, Z. E., Sulik, M. J., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Silva, K. M., Lemery-Chalfant, K., et al. (2014). Development of ego-resiliency: relations to observed parenting and polymorphisms in the serotonin transporter gene during early childhood. Soc. Dev. 23, 433–450. doi: 10.1111/sode.12041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thapar, A., Harold, G., Rice, F., Langley, K., and O’Donovan, M. (2007a). The contribution of gene-environment interaction to psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 989–1004. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thapar, A., Langley, K., Asherson, P., and Gill, M. (2007b). Gene-Environment interplay in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and the importance of a developmental perspective. Br. J. Psychiatry 190, 1–3. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.027003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thibodeau, E. L., Cicchetti, D., and Rogosch, F. A. (2015). Child maltreatment, impulsivity and antisocial behavior in African American children: moderation effects from a cumulative dopaminergic gene index. Dev. Psychopathol. 27, 1621–1636. doi: 10.1017/S095457941500098X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, R. A. (1993). Socioemotional development: enduring issues and new challenges. Dev. Rev. 13, 372–402. doi: 10.1006/drev.1993.1018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toth, S. L., and Manly, J. T. (2019). Developmental consequences of child abuse and neglect: implications for intervention. Child Dev. Perspect. 13, 59–64. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12317

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tripp, G., and Wickens, J. R. (2008). Dopamine transfer deficit: a neurobiological theory of altered reinforcement mechanisms in ADHD. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49, 691–704. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01851.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Aken, M. A. G., van Lieshout, C. F. M., Scholte, R. H. J., Haselager, G. J. T., Pulkkinen, L., and Caspi, A. (2002). “Personality types in childhood and adolescence: Main effects and person-relationship transactions,” in Paths to Successful Development: Personality in the Life Course, eds L. Pulkkinen, and A. Caspi (New York: Cambridge University Press), 129–156‥

Google Scholar

Waddington, C. H. (1942). Canalization of development and the inheritance of acquired characters. Nature 150, 563–565.

Google Scholar

Waldman, I. D. (2007). Gene-environment interactions re-examined: does mother’s marital stability interact with the dopamine receptor D2 gene in the aetiology of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 1117–1128. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weeland, J., Overbeek, G., de Castro, B. O., and Matthys, W. (2015). Underlying mechanisms of gene-environment interactions in externalizing behavior: a systematic review and search for theoretical mechanisms. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 18, 413–442. doi: 10.1007/s10567-015-0196-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wertz, J., Belsky, J., Moffitt, T. E., Belsky, D. W., Harrington, H., Avinun, R., et al. (2019). Genetics of nurture: a test of the hypothesis that parents’ genetics predict their observed caregiving. Dev. Psychol. 55, 1461–1472. doi: 10.1037/dev0000709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wessels, H., Lamb, M. E., Hwang, C. P., and Broberg, A. G. (1997). Personality development between 1 and 8 years of age in Swedish children with varying child care experiences. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 21, 771–794.

Google Scholar

Wismer Fries, A. B., Ziegler, T. E., Kurian, J. R., Jacoris, S., and Pollak, S. D. (2005). Early experience in humans is associated with changes in neuropeptides critical for regulating social behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 102, 17237–17240. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504767102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, X., Zhang, C. Y., Zhang, Z., Hu, Z., Li, M., and Li, T. (2021). Revisiting tandem repeats in psychiatric disorders from perspectives of genetics, physiology and brain evolution. Mol. Psychiatry . [Online ahead of print]. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01329-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, E. S., Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Huelsnitz, C. O., and Fleck, C. (2019). Childhood attachment and adult personality: a life history perspective. Self Identity 18, 22–38. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2017.1353540

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann, P. (1999). Structure and functions of internal working models of attachment and their role for emotion regulation. Attach. Hum. Dev. 1, 291–306. doi: 10.1080/14616739900134161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann, P., Gleason, M. M., Hellwig, S., Podewski, F., and Iwanski, A. (2022). Psychometric properties of the German version of the Early Childhood Screening Assessment. J. Child Fam. Stud. 31, 484–495. doi: 10.1007/s10826-021-02167-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann, P., Gliwitzky, J., and Becker-Stoll, F. (1996). Attachment and friendship in adolescence. Psychol. Erziehung Unterricht 43, 141–154.

Google Scholar

Zimmermann, P., Mohr, C., and Spangler, G. (2009). Genetic and attachment influences on adolescents’ regulation of autonomy and aggressiveness. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 1339–1347. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02158.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann, P., and Scheuerer-Englisch, H. (2013). Differences in attachment and personality in children from child guidance centers, child psychiatry units and control families. Prax. Kinderpsychol. Kinderpsychiatr. 62, 5–29. doi: 10.13109/prkk.2013.62.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann, P., and Spangler, G. (2016). Effects of gene × attachment interaction on adolescents’ emotion regulation and aggressive hostile behavior towards their mothers during a computer game. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:254. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: DRD4, dopamine, maternal sensitivity, personality development, gene-environment (GxE) interaction, ego-resiliency, ego-undercontrol, aggressiveness

Citation: Zimmermann P and Spangler G (2022) Longitudinal Influences of DRD4 Polymorphism and Early Maternal Caregiving on Personality Development and Problem Behavior in Middle Childhood and Adolescence. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:839340. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.839340

Received: 19 December 2021; Accepted: 04 March 2022;
Published: 14 April 2022.

Edited by:

Anna Buchheim, University of Innsbruck, Austria

Reviewed by:

Susumu Yokota, Kyushu University, Japan
Roger Kobak, University of Delaware, United States

Copyright © 2022 Zimmermann and Spangler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Peter Zimmermann, pzimmermann@uni-wuppertal.de

Download