Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 07 February 2019
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Processes in Individual and Collective Creativity: A Cross-Cultural Perspective View all 22 articles

Computational Simulation of Team Creativity: The Benefit of Member Flow

\r\nChong ZuChong Zu1Hui Zeng,Hui Zeng2,3Xiang Zhou,*Xiang Zhou1,3*
  • 1Department of Social Psychology, Nankai University, Tianjin, China
  • 2School of Economics, Nankai University, Tianjin, China
  • 3Collaborative Innovation Center for China Economy, Tianjin, China

This study simulates the team cognition model through NetLogo 6.0.2 to view a dynamic changing of team creativity during knowledge sharing when the team members perform problem-solving tasks. A hypothesis is proposed: (a) when people possess various characteristics, members who own high-level normal knowledge and have high communication frequency are suited to perform problem construction process and members who own high-level creative knowledge and have less communication frequency are suited to perform divergent exploration process; (b) member flow that old-timer is replaced by a new member, can improve the team creativity and keep it more stable. The team cognition model is based on the social network of the team, where members are assigned cognition tasks. Also, the simulation experiments are conducted in 6 conditions and each condition has one situation including “MemberFlow” procedure, and one excluding “MemberFlow” procedure. Each experiment contains 500 repetitive experiments and in each repetition, there are 100 steps of “GO” procedure are performed. The results show that the team creativity is maximal and stable in the condition of hypothesis (a), and member flow can optimize the team creativity.

Introduction

Many influential factors of creativity, on the individual and team level, have been researched, such as openness of personality, intrinsic motivation, social characteristics, knowledge sharing, cognition processes and so on (Hennessey, 2015; Jiang et al., 2018; Lee, 2018; Xu et al., 2018). Most studies are focused on a particular factor to examine their correlations. Comparing with other methods, computational simulation can integrate several influential factors in one experiment to observe the team creativity in the environment approaching the reality. In this study, the team cognition model is simulated through NetLogo 6.0.2, using Logo language, where knowledge is shared during the team communication, and the team members generate the solutions for a specific problem through problem-solving cognitive processes. In this computational simulation, team creativity can be evaluated through how many creative solutions can be found in all solutions that can solve this problem. And the subjects in the virtual experiments are the agents, and the set of agents called “agentset.” One of the advantages that computational simulation has is that many potential characteristics can be considered in the simulation experiments, like knowledge structure and communication frequency, which are the important characters that agents possess in the simulation.

Since 1950, researchers have started to turn their interests on creativity (Guilford, 1950). After years of efforts, the essence of creativity could be understood through multi-levels, and psychologists reveal personalities, thinking modes, emotion, cognition, social characteristics can affect creativity on the individual level and organizational level, in order to provide more creative productions and better life quality. Creativity is an ability that can produce novel and useful achievements (Sternberg and Lubart, 1996). The achievements can be creative ideas, solutions for the particular problems, results from task accomplishment, and products of variety kinds of arts and et al. Team can be defined as two or more members possessing distinct characteristics and knowledge who interact dynamically, dependently and adaptively with each other to accomplish a general and valuable task, where every one is distributed specific task to perform with limited time as a member of the team (Salas, 1992). Although individual creativity of members is the foundation of the team creativity, synergistic interaction among members is crucial, so that generating creative productions are base on the individual knowledge, and knowledge sharing through communication can also affect the team creativity.

Knowledge is the base-stone of individual creativity. People cannot create new things surpass their knowledge. Therefore, when the creativity is discussed and researched, knowledge structure is a crucial characteristic in the study. In the simulation, agents need to conduct their problem-solving processes according to the knowledge pool so that their knowledge structure may affect their performance. Beyond that, many other individual characteristics, like the openness of personality, intrinsic motivation, social characteristics, can make an effect to the creativity for a creative production (Feist, 1998; Wolfradt and Pretz, 2001; Sacchetti and Tortia, 2013). These attributes can give individuals a proper intrinsic environment to generate creative achievements based on their knowledge. Besides, in terms of social characteristics, the change of social position and tie strength caused by team communication can affect the efficiency of information acquisition (Perry-Smith and Shalley, 2003; Perry-Smith, 2006). Therefore, the hypothesis has been proposed that member flow, which means in this case that using new member who is willing to communicate to ones who contains the various knowledge, would improve the team creativity and keep it more stable. In this case, member flow can be defined as that old-timer shifts out, and a new member joins this team alternatively.

All the characteristics that can affect individual creativity can be seen as a mediator between their knowledge and their performance in the cognitive processes. Thus, they are simplified in the computational simulation into efficiency variable, which represents how is the performance that people generate creative productions based on their knowledge. In the simulation, the efficiency variable is controlled as a control variable. In addition to the personal level, people collaborate with others as a team or group for a specific task through communication, which forms a social network. Simonton (2000) once suggested that successful research on creativity should place creative individuals among social network; therefore social characteristics play an important role on individual creativity, which contains the strength of the relationship, position in the social network. In terms of strength of the relationship, according to the strength-of-weak-ties theory from Granovetter (1973), weak ties interspersing among social network, which means low frequency and short time interaction and limited intimacy in the relationship, could improve on generating creative ideas. In addition, for the reason that new ideas produce from the interrelationship of previous ideas, creative ideas need an amount of information and knowledge as the basis of competence to generate creative ideas. Hence, the quality and efficiency of information acquisition are crucial. Individuals with weak ties in the social network, who can only get information less repeatedly, would get less redundant information and knowledge thereby improving the efficiency of information acquisition, compared to the ones with strong ties (Granovetter, 1973). Another social characteristic is the position including Centrality and Peripherality. Approaching the central position may increase individual creativity while surpassing a general level can impede developing creativity. That is because that people in central position would experience more relationship conflicts causing anxiety, thus strangling one’s creativity (Podolny et al., 1997). Thus, how is an individual’s strength of the relationship can be manipulated through team communication in the simulation, which is defined as the frequency of member who shares their knowledge to other members. Meanwhile, the position of the member in the team is a potential characteristic, which is an outcome of communication. In this case, this kind of communication is the knowledge sharing process.

In the individual level, the brain generates creativity, which influenced by personality, intrinsic motivation and social characteristics and other properties; in the team level, communication, and cognition compose to the brain of the team. Cooke brought about Interactive team cognition, considering that team cognition produces from the interaction among members, where people generate dynamic emergence of team cognition through interaction, negotiation, decision, and other mutual actions (Cooke et al., 2013; Cooke, 2015). Consequently, team cognition, as a complicated dynamic model, emerges from simple communication among members, which can be considered as neural connections in the brain. In many measurements, the condition of achieving tasks, producing ideas and works can be evaluated as the criteria for creativity, the processes of which need to integrate all competencies of members and knowledge, where the team communication plays a crucial role. Communication cannot only integrate productions derived from cognitive actions, but communication is also a precondition for knowledge sharing. The knowledge structure is of importance to generate new novel productions, so that knowledge diversity is one of the influence factors (Pelled et al., 1999). When the team executes cognitive activities, performs tasks, members communicate with each other for knowledge sharing to generate creative ideas and works, or solve problems, accomplish tasks. In this research, the knowledge of members is shared among the team and with the other team.

If team communication could be compared to neural connections, team cognition might be viewed as the structure of the brain. Team cognition processes in problem-solving had been extended from individual level (Reiter-Palmon et al., 2008). The problem-solving task is the research that can be solved by various methods and solutions, based on the information stored in the memory (Chiew and Wang, 2004). Many researchers developed their own models to overview this cognitive process. For instance, Bransford and Stein (1984) designed the problem-solving model as problem identification, problem representation, strategy selection, strategy application, and result evaluation. For computational simulation, an integrated team cognition model can be summarized as problem construction, divergent exploration, evaluation and conclusion. Problem construction can be valued through problem restatements, and a good problem construction can produce creative solutions with high quality (Reiter-Palmon et al., 1997; Reiter-Palmon and Robinson, 2009). In the model of Runco and Chand (1995), it is a problem finding process, aiming to decide the properties of the problem and strategy selection. In this research, agents can construct the range of the problem for subsequent divergent exploration. In the divergent exploration process, the team uses divergent thinking to explore novel and useful solutions for the problem. After exploration, the team combines scattered opinions from members to generate preliminary solutions through communication. With regard to the whole team, the solutions that generate from members independently are, inevitably, resembling or even repetitive. Therefore, in this step, integrating these solutions effectively also will be included. In the last process, evaluation and conclusion, the solutions need to be evaluated if they are useful and novel, in order to measure team creativity. In the simulation, problem construction and divergent exploration are considered and manipulated through agents, who would conduct these procedures based on their knowledge and their efficiency, which can control how many problem restatements and various solutions that they can explore. The evaluation and conclusion part can be developed in the subsequent researches.

This study builds a team cognition model to describe how the members work as a team, and then a computational simulation is programmed through NetLogo 6.0.2 and virtual experiments are performed in the BehaviorSpace in the NetLogo. Knowledge and Efficiency are the characteristics included for each team member agent, and they can share their knowledge, based on which agents execute their corresponding problem-solving processes. The aim of simulation is that a dynamic changing can be viewed in different parameters of agents and problem, and beyond that, a hypothesis can be disclosed which is member flow can improve the team creativity.

Materials and Methods

Team Cognition Model

The model is constructed before the simulation to conduct each of the team members what cognitive activities would proceed when they solve a specific problem and the team formation as a social network. The social network is formed from the interaction of members who are belonged to this network and then individual social characteristics are given to every member, like their tie strength and position, which may affect their performance in the team. Besides, knowledge is an important characteristic that can affect the team problem-solving results. Therefore, there are the hypotheses:

(a) When people possess various knowledge structure and communication style, members who have more normal knowledge and communicate with others more frequently, are suited to perform problem construction process; members who have more creative knowledge and communicate with others less frequently, are suited to perform divergent exploration process. This arrangement can optimize team creativity.

(b) When member flows at intervals, team creativity would be more stable and higher.

The team is constituted by two or more members who possess divergent knowledge and members who are distributed the same problem-solving task form a group: supervision group need to perform problem construction task; exploration group need to execute divergent exploration task; evaluation group is in charge of evaluation and conclusion part. In this study, the effect of evaluation and conclusion procedure to the team creativity is not considered. Meanwhile, many other properties of individuals are simplified to the efficiency that is described as a mediator element represented to how efficient members are exchanging knowledge into results of each problem-solving process. The efficiency variation as one of the individual characters is controlled as a control variable, which is affected by personality, motivation and other factors.

According to Figure 1, Team Cognition Model, the team is a social network; the lines connect with other members represent the interaction, including information and knowledge sharing and normal communication. And the members separate into three groups, depending on their creativity, social characteristic and responsibility, to execute the corresponding cognitive process. While members’ social character will change along with executing cognition tasks, team creativity would change adaptively, which will conduct the computational simulation.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The number of members is adjustable and they communicate with each other in the team. According to the social position in the team, central members form supervision group to execute Problem Construction process, showing with red balls; Peripheral members form exploration group to generate various solutions in Divergent Exploration process, showing with yellow balls; Other members form the evaluation group to perform Evaluation and Conclusion process to integrate scattered solutions and evaluate whether final solution is creative or not, showing with orange balls.

Methods

The simulation is implemented by NetLogo 6.0.2 (Wilensky, 1999), which is a multi-agent programmable simulation tool. The agents and their characteristics and the elements of the world can be set up in the “Setup” procedure. In the “World” (Figure 2), every agent and their behaviors can be observed. Also, their behaviors can be manipulated in the “Go” procedure. On the interface, variables can be adjusted in the certain range, and the results can be displayed through “World” and “Plot” function can show the variation of the variable at interest. Meanwhile, virtual experiments can be performed through BehaviorSpace, where the times of “Go” procedure executed in once experiment and the times of experiments can be regulated.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. It is the “World” that can observe the member agents (triangle figures) and the rabbit agents (write round figures). The yellow part represents the normal knowledge area (normal solution area) and the purple part represents the creative knowledge area (creative solution area); the blue part represents the solutions that the team generates. When the blue part overlaps with rabbit agents, it means a feasible solution.

The psychological course is complicated and circuitous so that it is difficult to simulate all the mental processes and consider all the individual characters. Thus, some processes and characters are set up as the control variables, and in this case, they are evaluation and conclusion process and efficiency, which represents to individual characteristics that can affect the efficiency of transforming knowledge into solutions.

In the team, there are three Agentsets: Supervision Group Agentset, Exploration Group Agentset and Evaluation Group Agentsets. Based on the team cognition model that mentioned before, agents’ characteristics are knowledge structure, the frequency of communication and efficiency. Knowledge is divided into normal knowledge and creative knowledge; the ratio of them can be adjusted, and also the frequency and efficiency can be modulated. In the experiments, the characteristics of agents in the Evaluation Group Agentset and Efficiency variables of all agents are controlled at a moderate level. The problem in the simulation is a series of coordinates of rabbits who are distributed in the normal knowledge area and creative knowledge area in the “World”. The size of the “World” is based on the range of total Knowledge Pool (KP) who is a global array variable where every item of knowledge pool of all the agents are picked stochastically. The main variables are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. There are three Agentsets of the team members: Supervision Group Agentset, Exploration Group Agentset, Evaluation Group Agentset, and a Rabbits Agentset, whose coordinates (RCoordinate) compose the given problem need to be solved.

Meanwhile, the simulation is a stochastic process so that the study discards the results of the first five steps because the random numbers that are set up in the “Setup” procedure would generate the noise.

The team creativity is calculated through:

tc = NScreativeNScreative + NSnormal

In the equation, tc represents the team creativity, which means the ratio of the number of creative solutions in the total solutions the team got, where NScreative is the number of creative solutions and NSnormal is the number of normal solutions.

The aim of simulation is to optimize the team creativity, finding a situation that can make tc maximal and stable, and the hypothesis is when members are arranged in the suitable cognition process group, and member flows at intervals, the team creativity can be maximum and stability.

Simulation and Results

Simulation Procedures

In the “Setup” procedure, Agentsets are bred and every variable is announced, and as well the “World” is created according to the range of total knowledge pool. All the numbers in the normal knowledge pool array compose into the normal problem area, and ones in the creative knowledge pool array compose into the creative problem area. The area formed by numbers’ combination of both knowledge pool is the redundant area, where rabbits would not generate.

Then the “Go” procedure is performed, which includes providing the problem, Knowledge sharing process, and problem construction process, divergent exploration process, and member flow process and team creativity measurement. The simulation experiments are conducted by BehaviorSpace function. In the simulation, 500 times repetitive experiments are performed under the same condition of the experiment (A, B, C, D, E, F) and in each repetition 100 steps of “Go” procedures are conducted. For optimizing the team creativity, 6 pair experiments are conducted, and each pair of experiment containing “MemberFlow” procedure and excluding “MemberFlow” procedure, which are A/a, B/b, C/c, D/d, E/e, F/f (e. g., A experiment contains “MemberFlow”, an experiment excludes “MemberFlow”). The specific setup can be seen in Table 2 and the Supplementary Data Sheets S1S12 are the output files from BehaviorSpace experiments (1 and 2 for A/a, 3 and 4 for B/b, 5 and 6 for C/c, 7 and 8 for D/d, 9 and 10 for E/e, 11 and 12 for F/f), which are analyzed in the Results section.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. A/a, B/b, C/c, D/d, E/e, F/f are 6 pair of experiments.

Results

In terms of all experiments, the initial knowledge pool arrays of members are limited in 10 items, the ratio of the size of normal knowledge and creative knowledge can be adjusted through variable SKNnormal, SKNcreative, EKNnormal, EKNcreative. Thus, in the A/a experiments, agents in the supervision group agentset own the high level of normal knowledge and high level of communication frequency; on the contrary, agents in the exploration group agentset possess the high level of creative knowledge and low level of communication frequency. The variables in the experiments B/b are set up at the moderate level. Comparing with A/a experiments, the C/c experiments have the different setup in communication frequency; D/d experiments have the different setup in members’ knowledge structure. The E/e and F/f experiments perform with the low creative knowledge level of all supervision and exploration group agents and with different setup of communication frequency.

The result of team creativity (tc) is collected and analyzed, which is shown in Table 3. In once experiment, each step can generate a value of tc, so that tcmean represents to the general team creativity of this experiment, which is the ratio of creative solutions among all solutions so that the range of tc is [0, 1]. When tc > 0.5 means that the virtual team generates more creative solutions and less normal solutions. In the study, a high probability of the mean of team creativity who reaches to 0.8 means that most repetitions of the experiment in certain condition can get a mean above 0.8 in 100 steps, which represents as Pmean. P0.95 means the probability of repetitive experiments whose values of team creativity in 95% steps are above a certain value so that higher probability means a more stable team in these repetitions of the experiments. Pmean shows the team stability in the experiment, and P0.95 shows the team stability in all the repetitions of the experiments.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. tc represents to team creativity, whose range is [0, 1]; tcmean represents to the mean of team creativity in once repetition of each condition of experiments.

Individual Knowledge Structure

In the F, D, A experiments (Figure 3), the communication frequency of both supervision group and exploration group are the same. There are both low level of creative knowledge in both groups in the F experiment, whose mean of team creativity (tcmean = 0.272) of the whole experiment is lowest; in the D experiment, members in the supervision group own high level of creative knowledge, and on the contrary, the exploration group own low level of creative knowledge, whose mean of team creativity is 0.559; a high value of tcmean (0.820) shows in experiment A, whose members in the supervision group own relatively low level of creative knowledge and members in the exploration group possess the high level of it. Also, in the A experiment, most repetitions and steps in the same repetition generate a high value of team creativity, and most of the time the team creates more normal solutions in the F experiment.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The variations of team creativity in the A, D, F experiment are illustrated.

Therefore, members who own a high level of creative knowledge are suited in the exploration group and ones who own a high level of normal knowledge are suited in the supervision group. This arrangement can optimize team creativity.

Communication Frequency

When the members are arranged according to their knowledge structure becomingly, such as the A, C experiment. While in the C experiment, members in the supervision group have a low level of communication frequency and members in the exploration group are willing to communicate with others, which leads to a relatively low mean of team creativity (tcmean = 0.559) and nearly 30% of repetitive experiments generate an average of team creativity under 0.5, which means more normal solutions, in the A experiment, members in the exploration group have a low level of communication frequency and members in the supervision group are more willing to communicate with other ones, which leads to a relatively high tcmean (0.820) and, in most repetitive experiments and steps in once repetition, team creativity can reach above 0.7 (Pmean = 0.968, P0.95 = 0.846) (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The variations of team creativity in the A, C and E, F experiment are illustrated.

In the condition that all members of the team own low level of creative knowledge, when members in the supervision group are more willing to communicate, as the performance in the F experiment, tcmean (0.272) is slightly less than the mean of team creativity in the E experiment whose members in the exploration group are more willing to communicate.

Therefore, members who have a high level of communication frequency are suited in the supervision group and members who have a low level of communication frequency are suited in the exploration group. This arrangement can optimize team creativity.

Member Flow

Comparing to the A, C, D, E, F experiment, the different variable is that “MemberFlow” procedure is excluded in the a, c, d, e, f experiments (Figure 5), and the result of tcmean are lower than the corresponding experiments who include “MemberFlow” procedure. To the Pmean and P0.95, the probability of team creativity in the high-level range is higher in the experiments who include “MemberFlow” procedure, such as A, C, D, E, F experiment. However, the improvement is unobserved in the B/b experiment, so that the member flow moderating effect is higher when characteristics of members are divergent.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The variations of team creativity in the A/a, B/b, C/c, D/d. E/e, F/f experiment are illustrated.

Therefore, member flow has a positive moderating effect when the team wants to optimize their team creativity.

Discussion and Conclusion

The results display that the member arrangement proposed in the hypothesis can optimize team creativity and member flow can moderate the team creativity in most situation. Many previous pieces of research show that changing membership can improve team creativity. Rotating randomly a subset of group members when they performed divergent exploration tasks can enhance group creativity (Choi and Thompson, 2005). Social identity is one reason for the members of an original group to accept rotating member from a different place, and which can increase knowledge stock of this group, hence enhancing their performance (Kane et al., 2005). In accordance with other laboratory studies, the result from the computational simulation is reasonable. In terms of transactive memory system (TMS) of the group, partial membership change creates inefficient TMS processes because TMS structure that new members rely on is developed by old-timers in their original group (Lewis et al., 2007), and anticipating of membership change can make transactive memory more difficult to build (Blanchet and Michinov, 2017). Consequently, in the future, transactive memory system and cognitive conflict should be considered during simulation for a more comprehensive study about member flow, except for the knowledge structure and communication frequency of members in the team. In addition to member flow, many other adaptive changing can affect team creativity that researchers cannot examine through traditional laboratory experiments or questionnaires. The computational simulation may be considered as another method to explore team variations through problem-solving processes or other cognition processes. Moreover, people in the peripheral position, who possess more external interaction with other social networks, have a higher level of creativity; correspondingly, people in the central position, who possess less external interaction, also can develop a higher level of creativity (Perry-Smith and Shalley, 2003; Perry-Smith, 2006). Thus, external knowledge sharing can be considered in the next step to simulate. Besides, the evaluation group and efficiency variable are controlled in this study, which can be considered in the following researches.

In addition to many other factors that can affect the team creativity, also the creativity has been researched in various methods and developed various interpretations. In terms of neuroimaging method, insight, as a kind of creative cognition, which is defined as a process that people can solve a problem from the state of not knowing to knowing abruptly, and the ‘Aha!’ experiences occurred in insight problem-solving have a high positive correlation with positive affect and fluent cognition which can improve individual creative thinking (Shen et al., 2016). This insight process also has been examined through this method and many relative studies have found consistently that anterior cingulate cortex and prefrontal areas are related to insight (Dietrich and Kanso, 2010) and right hemispheric dominance theory of creative thinking also applies to creative insight (Shen et al., 2013). Therefore, if the structure of the team and the interaction among members could be considered as the brain that may construct the team creativity and then generate creative achievements efficiently, the insight problem-solving process may be possible to be found in the team creative problem-solving process, which can be considered in the next stage of computational simulation. Besides and Dietrich (2018) proposed a new theoretical framework of creativity to separate this concept into three modes, and among them the flow mode concept can be made a analogy with the member flow in the team.

In conclusion, the preponderance of computational simulation can be seen in this study. This method can integrate all contents of researches that psychologists concern, to observe variations of team creativity qualitatively through simulating a series of cognition processes and considering various individual characteristics instead of focusing on one of them, although this method cannot obtain a sufficient external validity like other laboratory experiments.

In terms of the process of simulation, before which a team cognition model has been set up including members’ responsibility distribution: supervision group, exploration group, evaluation group; besides agent behaviors are included: knowledge sharing and the problem solving cognitive processes: problem construction, divergent exploration, evaluation and conclusion. According to previous researches, knowledge structure and social characteristics can affect the team creativity, so that these properties are put into the simulation; other individual characteristics are also important but they are controlled in the simulation experiments as efficiency variable (SE, EE). Then, the team cognition model is simulated through NetLogo 6.0.2. The results show that knowledge structure and communication frequency can affect the team creativity and when people gain the various characters in both, a suitable arrangement can optimize the team creativity and it can be more stable and higher when member flows.

Author Contributions

CZ designed this study, collected and analyzed the data with the assistance of XZ, and wrote the manuscript. XZ and HZ provided the critical emendations.

Funding

This research was supported by Key Program of the Tianjin Philosophy and Social Science Research Project of China (TJJX16-001), Key Program of the National Natural Science Foundation of China (71532005), Key Program of Tianjin Science and Technology Development Strategic Research Project of China (15ZLZLZF00770), National Social Science Foundation of China (12BSH053), and the Major Social Science Project of Tianjin Municipal Education Commission of China (2018JWZD41).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00188/full#supplementary-material

DATA SHEETS S1, S2 | The outputs of A/a experiments through BehaviorSpace function.

DATA SHEETS S3, S4 | The outputs of B/b experiments through BehaviorSpace function.

DATA SHEETS S5, S6 | The outputs of C/c experiments through BehaviorSpace function.

DATA SHEETS S7, S8 | The outputs of D/d experiments through BehaviorSpace function.

DATA SHEETS S9, S10 | The outputs of E/e experiments through BehaviorSpace function.

DATA SHEETS S11, S12 | The outputs of F/f experiments through BehaviorSpace function.

References

Blanchet, C., and Michinov, E. (2017). Impact of the anticipation of membership change on transactive memory and group performance. Curr. Res. Soc. Psychol. 25, 1–10.

Google Scholar

Bransford, J. D., and Stein, B. S. (1984). The Ideal Problem Solver: A Guide for Improving Thinking, Learning, and Creativity. New York, NY: WH Freeman and Company.

Google Scholar

Chiew, V., and Wang, Y. X. (2004). “Formal description of the cognitive process of problem solving,” in Proceedings of the Third IEEE International Conference on Cognitive Informatics, (Victoria, BC: IEEE), 74–83. doi: 10.1109/COGINF.2004.1327461

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, H. S., and Thompson, L. (2005). Old wine in a new bottle: impact of membership change on group creativity. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 98, 121–132. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooke, N. J. (2015). Team Cognition as interaction. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24, 415–419. doi: 10.1177/0963721415602474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooke, N. J., Gorman, J. C., Myers, C. W., and Duran, J. L. (2013). Interactive team cognition. Cogn. Sci. 37, 255–285. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietrich, A. (2018). Types of creativity. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 1–12. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1517-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Dietrich, A., and Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and insight. Psychol. Bull. 136, 822–848. doi: 10.1037/a0019749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2, 290–309. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0204_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Granovetter, M. S. (1973). The strength of weak ties. Am. J. Sociol. 78, 1360–1380. doi: 10.1086/225469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. Am. psychol. 5, 444–454. doi: 10.1037/h0063487

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hennessey, B. A. (2015). Creative behavior, motivation, environment and culture: the building of a systems model. J. Creat. Behav. 49, 194–210. doi: 10.1002/jocb.97

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, H., Zhang, Q. P., and Zhou, Y. (2018). Dynamic Creative interaction networks and team creativity evolution: a longitudinal study. J. Creat. Behav. 52, 168–196. doi: 10.1002/jocb.141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kane, A. A., Argote, L., and Levine, J. M. (2005). Knowledge transfer between groups via personnel rotation: effects of social identity and knowledge quality. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. process. 96, 56–71. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. (2018). The effects of knowledge sharing on individual creativity in higher education institutions: socio-technical view. Adm. Sci. 8, 1–16. doi: 10.3390/admsci8020021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, K., Belliveau, M., Herndon, B., and Keller, J. (2007). Group cognition, membership change, and performance: investigating the benefits and detriments of collective knowledge. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. process. 103, 159–178. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2007.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelled, L. H., Eisenhardt, K. M., and Xin, K. R. (1999). Exploring the black box: an analysis of work group diversity, conflict and performance. Adm. Sci. Q. 44, 1–28. doi: 10.2307/2667029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry-Smith, J. E. (2006). Social yet creative: the role of social relationships in facilitating individual creativity. Acad. Manag. J. 49, 85–101. doi: 10.5465/amj.2006.20785503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry-Smith, J. E., and Shalley, C. E. (2003). The social side of creativity: a static and dynamic social network perspective. Acad. Manag. 28, 89–106.

Google Scholar

Podolny, J. M., Baron, J. N., Krogh, G. V., Roos, J., Venzin, M., and Engwall, L. (1997). Resources and relationships: social networks and mobility in the workplace. Am. Sociol. Rev. 62, 673–693. doi: 10.2307/2657354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiter-Palmon, R., Herman, A. E., and Yammarino, F. J. (2008). Creativity and cognitive processes: multi-level linkages between individual and team cognition. Multi-level Issues Creat. Innov. 7, 203–267. doi: 10.1016/S1475-9144(07)00009-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiter-Palmon, R., Mumford, M. D., O’Connor Boes, J., and Runco, M. A. (1997). Problem construction and creativity: the role of ability, cue consistency, and active processing. Creat. Res. J. 10, 9–23. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1001_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiter-Palmon, R., and Robinson, E. J. (2009). Problem identification and construction: what do we know, what is the future? Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 3, 43–47. doi: 10.1037/a0014629

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runco, M. A., and Chand, I. (1995). Cognition and creativity. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 7, 243–267. doi: 10.1007/BF02213373

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sacchetti, S., and Tortia, E. C. (2013). Satisfaction with creativity: a study of organizational characteristics and individual motivation. J. Happiness Stud. 14, 1789–1811. doi: 10.1007/s10902-012-9410-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salas, E. (1992). Toward an understanding of team performance and training. Teams Train. Perform. 3–29.

Shen, W., Liu, C., Zhang, X., Zhao, X., Zhang, J., Yuan, Y., et al. (2013). Right hemispheric dominance of creative insight: an event-related potential study. Creat. Res. J. 25, 48–58. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2013.752195

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, W., Yuan, Y., Liu, C., and Luo, J. (2016). In search of the ‘Aha!’experience: elucidating the emotionality of insight problem-solving. Br. J. Psychol. 107, 281–298. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simonton, D. K. (2000). Creativity: cognitive, personal, developmental, and social aspects. Am. psychol. 55, 151–158. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J., and Lubart, T. I. (eds) (1996). The Concept of Creativity: Prospects and Paradigms. Cambridge: University of Cambridge.

Google Scholar

Wilensky, U. (1999). NetLogo. Center for Connected Learning and Computer-Based Modeling. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University.

Google Scholar

Wolfradt, U., and Pretz, J. E. (2001). Individual differences in creativity: personality, story writing, and hobbies. Eur. J. Pers. 15, 297–310. doi: 10.1002/per.409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, S., Jiang, X., and Walsh, I. J. (2018). The influence of openness to experience on perceived employee creativity: the moderating roles of individual trust. J. Creat. Behav. 52, 142–155. doi: 10.1002/jocb.138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: team creativity, member flow, social network, computational simulation, knowledge sharing, NetLogo

Citation: Zu C, Zeng H and Zhou X (2019) Computational Simulation of Team Creativity: The Benefit of Member Flow. Front. Psychol. 10:188. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00188

Received: 29 September 2018; Accepted: 21 January 2019;
Published: 07 February 2019.

Edited by:

Wangbing Shen, Hohai University, China

Reviewed by:

Antonio Francesco Corno, University of Leicester, United Kingdom
Xuexin Zhang, Fudan University, China

Copyright © 2019 Zu, Zeng and Zhou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xiang Zhou, zhouxiang@nankai.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.