The compatibility of complex systems and reduction: A case analysis of memory research [Book Review]
Minds and Machines 11 (4):483-502 (2001)
Abstract
Some theorists who emphasize the complexity of biological and cognitive systems and who advocate the employment of the tools of dynamical systems theory in explaining them construe complexity and reduction as exclusive alternatives. This paper argues that reduction, an approach to explanation that decomposes complex activities and localizes the components within the complex system, is not only compatible with an emphasis on complexity, but provides the foundation for dynamical analysis. Explanation via decomposition and localization is nonetheless extremely challenging, and an analysis of recent cognitive neuroscience research on memory is used to illustrate what is involved. Memory researchers split between advocating memory systems and advocating memory processes, and I argue that it is the latter approach that provides the critical sort of decomposition and localization for explaining memory. The challenges of linking distinguishable functions with brain processes is illustrated by two examples: competing hypotheses about the contribution of the hippocampus and competing attempts to link areas in frontal cortex with memory processingAuthor's Profile
Reprint years
2004
DOI
10.1023/a:1011803931581
My notes
Similar books and articles
From reduction back to higher levels.William Bechtel & Adele Abrahamsen - 2008 - In B. C. Love, K. McRae & V. M. Sloutsky (eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 559--564.
Dispelling the magic: Towards memory without capacity.Niels A. Taatgen - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (1):147-148.
The Cognitive Neuroscience of Working Memory.Naoyuki Osaka, Robert H. Logie & Mark D'Esposito (eds.) - 2007 - Oxford University Press.
The contribution of long-term memory and the role of frontal-lobe systems in on-line processing.Jennifer D. Ryan & Neal J. Cohen - 2003 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26 (6):756-756.
Representation, reduction, and interdisciplinarity in the sciences of memory.John Sutton - 2004 - In Hugh Clapin (ed.), Representation in Mind. Elsevier. pp. 187--216.
Understanding neural complexity: A role for reduction. [REVIEW]John Bickle - 2001 - Minds and Machines 11 (4):467-481.
Analytics
Added to PP
2009-01-28
Downloads
293 (#41,241)
6 months
2 (#300,644)
2009-01-28
Downloads
293 (#41,241)
6 months
2 (#300,644)
Historical graph of downloads
Author's Profile
Citations of this work
Explanation: a mechanist alternative.William Bechtel & Adele Abrahamsen - 2005 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 36 (2):421-441.
The massive redeployment hypothesis and the functional topography of the brain.Michael L. Anderson - 2008 - Philosophical Psychology 21 (2):143-174.
The mechanisms of emergence.R. Keith Sawyer - 2004 - Philosophy of the Social Sciences 34 (2):260-282.
The Massive Redeployment Hypothesis and the Functional Topography of the Brain.Michael L. Anderson - 2007 - Philosophical Psychology 20 (2):143-174.
References found in this work
The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information.George A. Miller - 1956 - Psychological Review 63 (2):81-97.
What Might Cognition Be, If Not Computation?Tim Van Gelder - 1995 - Journal of Philosophy 92 (7):345 - 381.
The information available in visual presentations.George Sperling - 1960 - Psychological Monographs 74:1-29.
The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information.George A. Miller - 1956 - Psychological Review 101 (2):343-352.
The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science.Tim van Gelder - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (5):615-28.