Kinesthetic Unity as Motivated Association

Gestalt Theory 42 (3):271-286 (2020)
  Copy   BIBTEX

Abstract

Summary Within Husserl’s theory of perception, the role attributed to kinesthetic sensations determines a phase of the perceptive constitution that marks the boundary between pure receptivity and a first form of self-determination of consciousness. Kinesthetic experiences are, in fact, characterized not just as acts that are performed but rather that can be performed, albeit according to predetermined paths. This primitive form of ‘instinctive’ spontaneity of the Ego (linked to primal impulses) as realization of pre-established potentialities, characterizes what Husserl defines the ‘ idiopsychic’ dimension of consciousness (Husserl, 1952, p. 135). However, although this level of consciousness unity presupposes a spontaneous activity, it can be investigated according to the ‘causal’ laws of motivation. The phenomenon of motivation was notoriously introduced by Husserl in §56 of Ideen II, as a specific law of spiritual life. However, there are two possible forms of motivation, one in which the Ego is actively involved, and a second one, called “associative motivation.” The latter basically indicates the passive tendency of creating associations between unities of the immanent sphere. In other terms, Husserl acknowledges the existence of “motivated relations” within the immanent sphere of mental acts which do not necessarily call for an active participation of the Ego. In this sense, the relation between motivating factors and motivated elements could be considered a kind of conditioning of the form “because-therefore,” in which the two elements arrange themselves in a succession of experiences. This work aims to show that this very kind of association is the same that pre-determines the unfolding possibility of kinesthetic chains.

Links

PhilArchive



    Upload a copy of this work     Papers currently archived: 90,616

External links

Setup an account with your affiliations in order to access resources via your University's proxy server

Through your library

Similar books and articles

Husserl’s Concept of Motivation: The Logical Investigations and Beyond.Philip J. Walsh - 2013 - History of Philosophy & Logical Analysis 16 (1):70-83.
Constituting the mind: Kant, Davidson, and the unity of consciousness.Jeff Malpas - 1999 - International Journal of Philosophical Studies 7 (1):1-30.
Geometric and Intuitive Space in Husserl.Vincenzo Costa - 2017 - In Felice Masi & Maria Catena (eds.), The Changing Faces of Space. Cham: Springer Verlag.
Characteristics of visual and kinesthetic memory codes.Michael I. Posner - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 75 (1):103.
The Ontological Status of Essences in Husserl’s Thought.Andrea Zhok - 2011 - New Yearbook for Phenomenology and Phenomenological Philosophy 11:96-127.

Analytics

Added to PP
2020-12-23

Downloads
10 (#1,026,208)

6 months
2 (#670,035)

Historical graph of downloads
How can I increase my downloads?

Citations of this work

No citations found.

Add more citations

References found in this work

Phénoménologie de la perception.M. Merleau-Ponty - 1949 - Revista Portuguesa de Filosofia 5 (4):466-466.
The Senses and the Intellect.Alexander Bain - 1855 - D. Appleton and Company.
An introduction to Husserlian phenomenology.Rudolf Bernet, Iso Kern & Eduard Marbach - 1993 - Evanston, Ill.: Northwestern University Press. Edited by Iso Kern & Eduard Marbach.
Body image and body schema: A conceptual clarification.Shaun Gallagher - 1986 - Journal of Mind and Behaviour 7 (4):541-554.

View all 11 references / Add more references