Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Productive Thinking. [REVIEW]R. M. Ogden - 1946 - Philosophical Review 55 (3):298-300.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   212 citations  
  • Productive Thinking.Max Wertheimer - 1946 - Philosophical Review 55 (3):298.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   116 citations  
  • Learning from worked‐out examples: A study on individual differences.Alexander Renkl - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (1):1-29.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Learning from Worked-Out Examples: A Study on Individual Differences.Alexander Renkl - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (1):1-29.
    The goal of this study was to investigate interindividual differences in learning from worked-out examples with respect to the quality of self-explanations. Restrictions of former studies (e.g., lacking control of time-on-task) were avoided and additional research questions (e.g., reliability and dimensionality of self-explanation characteristics) were addressed. An investigation with 36 university freshmen of education working in individual sessions was conducted. The domain was probability calculus. As predictors of learning, prior knowledge and the quality of self-explanations (thinking aloud protocols) were assessed. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Abstract Planning and Perceptual Chunks: Elements of Expertise in Geometry.Kenneth R. Koedinger & John R. Anderson - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (4):511-550.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • A study of the effects of verbalization on problem solving.Robert M. Gagné & Ernest C. Smith - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 63 (1):12.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge.Zoltan Dienes & Josef Perner - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (5):735-808.
    The implicit-explicit distinction is applied to knowledge representations. Knowledge is taken to be an attitude towards a proposition which is true. The proposition itself predicates a property to some entity. A number of ways in which knowledge can be implicit or explicit emerge. If a higher aspect is known explicitly then each lower one must also be known explicitly. This partial hierarchy reduces the number of ways in which knowledge can be explicit. In the most important type of implicit knowledge, (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   96 citations  
  • Self‐Explanations: How Students Study and Use Examples in Learning to Solve Problems.Michelene T. H. Chi, Miriam Bassok, Matthew W. Lewis, Peter Reimann & Robert Glaser - 1989 - Cognitive Science 13 (2):145-182.
    The present paper analyzes the self‐generated explanations (from talk‐aloud protocols) that “Good” and “Poor” students produce while studying worked‐out examples of mechanics problems, and their subsequent reliance on examples during problem solving. We find that “Good” students learn with understanding: They generate many explanations which refine and expand the conditions for the action parts of the example solutions, and relate these actions to principles in the text. These self‐explanations are guided by accurate monitoring of their own understanding and misunderstanding. Such (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   84 citations  
  • Self‐Explanations: How Students Study and Use Examples in Learning to Solve Problems.Michelene T. H. Chi, Miriam Bassok, Matthew W. Lewis, Peter Reimann & Robert Glaser - 1989 - Cognitive Science 13 (2):145-182.
    The present paper analyzes the self‐generated explanations (from talk‐aloud protocols) that “Good” and “Poor” students produce while studying worked‐out examples of mechanics problems, and their subsequent reliance on examples during problem solving. We find that “Good” students learn with understanding: They generate many explanations which refine and expand the conditions for the action parts of the example solutions, and relate these actions to principles in the text. These self‐explanations are guided by accurate monitoring of their own understanding and misunderstanding. Such (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  • Eliciting Self‐Explanations Improves Understanding.Michelene T. H. Chi, Nicholas Leeuw, Mei‐Hung Chiu & Christian Lavancher - 1994 - Cognitive Science 18 (3):439-477.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Eliciting Self-Explanations Improves Understanding.Michelene T. H. Chi, Nicholas De Leeuw, Mei-Hung Chiu & Christian Lavancher - 1994 - Cognitive Science 18 (3):439-477.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices.Michelene T. H. Chi, Paul J. Feltovich & Robert Glaser - 1981 - Cognitive Science 5 (2):121-52.
    The representation of physics problems in relation to the organization of physics knowledge is investigated in experts and novices. Four experiments examine the existence of problem categories as a basis for representation; differences in the categories used by experts and novices; differences in the knowledge associated with the categories; and features in the problems that contribute to problem categorization and representation. Results from sorting tasks and protocols reveal that experts and novices begin their problem representations with specifiably different problem categories, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   231 citations  
  • Placental Transfer and Synthesis of Hormones.John H. Holland - 1973
  • Induction: Processes of Inference, Learning, and Discovery.John H. Holland, Keith J. Holyoak, Richard E. Nisbett & Paul R. Thagard - 1991 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 42 (2):269-272.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   217 citations  
  • Learning from worked-out examples: The effects of example variability and elicited self-explanations.Robin Stark, Alexander Renkl, Hans Gruber & Heinz Mandl - unknown
    It was investigated to what extent example variability and the elicitation of sophisticated self-explanations foster the acquisition of applicable and transferable knowledge by learning from worked-out examples. To this end, we had 56 apprentices from a bank learn calculation of compound interest and real interest. The subjects were randomly assigned to the four conditions of a 2´2-factorial design (factor 1: uniform vs. multiple examples; factor 2: spontaneous vs. Elicited self-explanations). The learning results were measured by a post-test comprising application problems (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations