Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks.Robert D. Rogers & Stephen Monsell - 1995 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124 (2):207.
  • Thinking with portals: Revisiting kinematic cues to intention.Roland Pfister, Markus Janczyk, Robert Wirth, David Dignath & Wilfried Kunde - 2014 - Cognition 133 (2):464-473.
  • Effect-based action control with body-related effects: Implications for empirical approaches to ideomotor action control.Roland Pfister - 2019 - Psychological Review 126 (1):153-161.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • On the Influence of Reward on Action-Effect Binding.Paul S. Muhle-Karbe & Ruth M. Krebs - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
  • Habits without values.Kevin J. Miller, Amitai Shenhav & Elliot A. Ludvig - 2019 - Psychological Review 126 (2):292-311.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Context theory of classification learning.Douglas L. Medin & Marguerite M. Schaffer - 1978 - Psychological Review 85 (3):207-238.
  • Toward an instance theory of automatization.Gordon D. Logan - 1988 - Psychological Review 95 (4):492-527.
  • The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning.Bernhard Hommel, Jochen Müsseler, Gisa Aschersleben & Wolfgang Prinz - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (5):849-878.
    Traditional approaches to human information processing tend to deal with perception and action planning in isolation, so that an adequate account of the perception-action interface is still missing. On the perceptual side, the dominant cognitive view largely underestimates, and thus fails to account for, the impact of action-related processes on both the processing of perceptual information and on perceptual learning. On the action side, most approaches conceive of action planning as a mere continuation of stimulus processing, thus failing to account (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   340 citations  
  • Anticipatory Control of Approach and Avoidance: An Ideomotor Approach.Andreas B. Eder & Bernhard Hommel - 2013 - Emotion Review 5 (3):275-279.
    This article reviews evidence suggesting that the cause of approach and avoidance behavior lies not so much in the presence (i.e., the stimulus) but, rather, in the behavior’s anticipated future consequences (i.e., the goal): Approach is motivated by the goal to produce a desired consequence or end-state, while avoidance is motivated by the goal to prevent an undesired consequence or end-state. However, even though approach and avoidance are controlled by goals rather than stimuli, affective stimuli can influence action control by (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy.Edward Awh, Artem V. Belopolsky & Jan Theeuwes - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (8):437.
    Prominent models of attentional control assert a dichotomy between top-down and bottom-up control, with the former determined by current selection goals and the latter determined by physical salience. This theoretical dichotomy, however, fails to explain a growing number of cases in which neither current goals nor physical salience can account for strong selection biases. For example, equally salient stimuli associated with reward can capture attention, even when this contradicts current selection goals. Thus, although 'top-down' sources of bias are sometimes defined (...)
    Direct download (11 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   93 citations