Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Computing machinery and intelligence.Alan M. Turing - 1950 - Mind 59 (October):433-60.
    I propose to consider the question, "Can machines think?" This should begin with definitions of the meaning of the terms "machine" and "think." The definitions might be framed so as to reflect so far as possible the normal use of the words, but this attitude is dangerous, If the meaning of the words "machine" and "think" are to be found by examining how they are commonly used it is difficult to escape the conclusion that the meaning and the answer to (...)
    Direct download (18 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   997 citations  
  • Perceiving referential intent: Dynamics of reference in natural parent–child interactions.John C. Trueswell, Yi Lin, Benjamin Armstrong, Erica A. Cartmill, Susan Goldin-Meadow & Lila R. Gleitman - 2016 - Cognition 148 (C):117-135.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • The Pursuit of Word Meanings.Jon Scott Stevens, Lila R. Gleitman, John C. Trueswell & Charles Yang - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (S4):638-676.
    We evaluate here the performance of four models of cross-situational word learning: two global models, which extract and retain multiple referential alternatives from each word occurrence; and two local models, which extract just a single referent from each occurrence. One of these local models, dubbed Pursuit, uses an associative learning mechanism to estimate word-referent probability but pursues and tests the best referent-meaning at any given time. Pursuit is found to perform as well as global models under many conditions extracted from (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Principles of object perception.Elizabeth S. Spelke - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (1):29--56.
    Research on human infants has begun to shed light on early-developing processes for segmenting perceptual arrays into objects. Infants appear to perceive objects by analyzing three-dimensional surface arrangements and motions. Their perception does not accord with a general tendency to maximize figural goodness or to attend to nonaccidental geometric relations in visual arrays. Object perception does accord with principles governing the motions of material bodies: Infants divide perceptual arrays into units that move as connected wholes, that move separately from one (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   280 citations  
  • Knowledge as Process: Contextually Cued Attention and Early Word Learning.Linda B. Smith, Eliana Colunga & Hanako Yoshida - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (7):1287-1314.
    Learning depends on attention. The processes that cue attention in the moment dynamically integrate learned regularities and immediate contextual cues. This paper reviews the extensive literature on cued attention and attentional learning in the adult literature and proposes that these fundamental processes are likely significant mechanisms of change in cognitive development. The value of this idea is illustrated using phenomena in children's novel word learning.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • A computational study of cross-situational techniques for learning word-to-meaning mappings.Jeffrey Mark Siskind - 1996 - Cognition 61 (1-2):39-91.
  • False-belief understanding in infants.Zijing He Renée Baillargeon, Rose M. Scott - 2010 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (3):110.
  • When English proposes what Greek presupposes: The cross-linguistic encoding of motion events.Anna Papafragou, Christine Massey & Lila Gleitman - 2006 - Cognition 98 (3):B75-B87.
  • When English proposes what Greek presupposes: the cross-linguistic encoding of motion events.Anna Papafragou - 2006 - Cognition 98 (3):75-87.
    How do we talk about events we perceive? And how tight is the connection between linguistic and non-linguistic representations of events? To address these questions, we experimentally compared motion descriptions produced by children and adults in two typologically distinct languages, Greek and English. Our findings confirm a well-known asymmetry between the two languages, such that English speakers are overall more likely to include manner of motion information than Greek speakers. However, mention of manner of motion in Greek speakers' descriptions increases (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Maturational Constraints on Language Learning.Elissa L. Newport - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (1):11-28.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   203 citations  
  • The use of multiple frames in verb learning via syntactic bootstrapping.Letitia R. Naigles - 1996 - Cognition 58 (2):221-251.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Understanding how input matters: verb learning and the footprint of universal grammar.Jeffrey Lidz, Henry Gleitman & Lila Gleitman - 2003 - Cognition 87 (3):151-178.
  • Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind.".Alan M. Leslie - 1987 - Psychological Review 94 (4):412-426.
  • Do six-month-old infants perceive causality?Alan M. Leslie & Stephanie Keeble - 1987 - Cognition 25 (3):265-288.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   185 citations  
  • When English proposes what Greek presupposes: the cross-linguistic encoding of motion events.Lila Gleitman - 2006 - Cognition 98 (3):75-87.
    How do we talk about events we perceive? And how tight is the connection between linguistic and non-linguistic representations of events? To address these questions, we experimentally compared motion descriptions produced by children and adults in two typologically distinct languages, Greek and English. Our findings confirm a well-known asymmetry between the two languages, such that English speakers are overall more likely to include manner of motion information than Greek speakers. However, mention of manner of motion in Greek speakers' descriptions increases (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Human simulations of vocabulary learning.Jane Gillette, Henry Gleitman, Lila Gleitman & Anne Lederer - 1999 - Cognition 73 (2):135-176.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   81 citations  
  • Human simulations of vocabulary learning.Jane Gillette, Lila Gleitman, Henry Gleitman & Anne Lederer - 1999 - Cognition 73 (2):135-176.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • The Language of Thought. [REVIEW]Adam Morton - 1978 - Journal of Philosophy 75 (3):161-169.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   104 citations  
  • Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small.Jeffrey L. Elman - 1993 - Cognition 48 (1):71-99.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   230 citations  
  • From communication to language—a psychological perspective.J. S. Bruner - 1974 - Cognition 3 (3):255-287.
  • False-belief understanding in infants.Renée Baillargeon, Rose M. Scott & Zijing He - 2010 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (3):110-118.
  • Fast mapping word meanings across trials: Young children forget all but their first guess.Athulya Aravind, Jill de Villiers, Amy Pace, Hannah Valentine, Roberta Golinkoff, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, Aquiles Iglesias & Mary Sweig Wilson - 2018 - Cognition 177 (C):177-188.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The Language of Thought.J. A. Fodor - 1978 - Critica 10 (28):140-143.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1387 citations  
  • The Perception of Causality.A. Michotte, T. R. Miles & Elaine Miles - 1964 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 15 (59):254-259.
  • Hard Words.Lila R. Gleitman, Anna Papafragou & John C. Trueswell - unknown
    How do children acquire the meaning of words? And why are words such as know harder for learners to acquire than words such as dog or jump? We suggest that the chief limiting factor in acquiring the vocabulary of natural languages consists not in overcoming conceptual difficulties with abstract word meanings but rather in mapping these meanings onto their corresponding lexical forms. This opening premise of our position, while controversial, is shared with some prior approaches. The present discussion moves forward (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  • 6 Universal aspects of word learning.Lila Gleitman & Cynthia Fisher - 2005 - In James A. McGilvray (ed.), The Cambridge Companion to Chomsky. Cambridge University Press. pp. 123.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Review of V erbal Behavior. [REVIEW]Noam Chomsky - 1959 - Language 35 (1):26--58.
    I had intended this review not specifically as a criticism of Skinner's speculations regarding language, but rather as a more general critique of behaviorist (I would now prefer to say "empiricist") speculation as to the nature of higher mental processes. My reason for discussing Skinner's book in such detail was that it was the most careful and thoroughgoing presentation of such speculations, an evaluation that I feel is still accurate. Therefore, if the conclusions I attempted to substantiate in the review (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   211 citations  
  • Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories.Eleanor Rosch & Carolyn B. Mervis - 1975 - Cognitive Psychology 7 (4):573--605.
    Six experiments explored the hypothesis that the members of categories which are considered most prototypical are those with most attributes in common with other members of the category and least attributes in common with other categories. In probabilistic terms, the hypothesis is that prototypicality is a function of the total cue validity of the attributes of items. In Experiments 1 and 3, subjects listed attributes for members of semantic categories which had been previously rated for degree of prototypicality. High positive (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   392 citations  
  • Word and Object.Willard Van Orman Quine - 1960 - Les Etudes Philosophiques 17 (2):278-279.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2807 citations  
  • The structure of a semantic theory.Jerrold Katz & Jerry Fodor - 1963 - Language 39:170-210.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   154 citations