Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Neural correlates of the first-person perspective.Kai Vogeley & Gereon R. Fink - 2003 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 (1):38-42.
  • Similarities and differences in visual and spatial perspective-taking processes.Andrew Surtees, Ian Apperly & Dana Samson - 2013 - Cognition 129 (2):426-438.
  • Windows to the soul: Children and adults see the eyes as the location of the self.Christina Starmans & Paul Bloom - 2012 - Cognition 123 (2):313-318.
  • What is a perspective problem? Developmental issues in belief ascription and dual identity.Josef Perner, Johannes L. Brandl & Alan Garnham - 2003 - Facta Philosophica 5 (2):355-378.
    We develop a criterion for telling when integrating two pieces of information, e.g. two pictures or statements requires an understanding of perspective. Problems that require such an understanding are perspective problems. With this criterion we can show that understanding false beliefs vis-à-vis reality pose a perspective problem, so does understanding spatial descriptions given from different viewing points (a classical example of what is commonly seen as a problem of perspective) and individuating objects with different sortals (naming objects). We use the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • The two forms of visuo-spatial perspective taking are differently embodied and subserve different spatial prepositions.Klaus Kessler - 2010 - Frontiers in Psychology 1.
  • The embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: Embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference.Klaus Kessler & Lindsey Anne Thomson - 2010 - Cognition 114 (1):72-88.
  • Self, world and space: The meaning and mechanisms of ego- and allocentric spatial representation.Rick Grush - 2000 - Brain and Mind 1 (1):59-92.
    b>: The problem of how physical systems, such as brains, come to represent themselves as subjects in an objective world is addressed. I develop an account of the requirements for this ability that draws on and refines work in a philosophical tradition that runs from Kant through Peter Strawson to Gareth Evans. The basic idea is that the ability to represent oneself as a subject in a world whose existence is independent of oneself involves the ability to represent space, and (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • The two-visual-systems hypothesis and the perspectival features of visual experience.Robert T. Foley, Robert L. Whitwell & Melvyn A. Goodale - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 35:225-233.
  • Imagining physically impossible self-rotations: geometry is more important than gravity.Sarah H. Creem, Maryjane Wraga & Dennis R. Proffitt - 2001 - Cognition 81 (1):41-64.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Bilateral symmetry and behavior.M. C. Corballis & I. L. Beale - 1970 - Psychological Review 77 (5):451-464.
  • Where exactly am I? Self-location judgements distribute between head and torso.Adrian J. T. Alsmith & Matthew R. Longo - 2014 - Consciousness and Cognition 24:70-74.
  • Dissociating contributions of head and torso to spatial reference frames: The misalignment paradigm.Adrian J. T. Alsmith, Elisa R. Ferrè & Matthew R. Longo - 2017 - Consciousness and Cognition 53:105-114.
  • Language, praxis, and the right hemisphere: Clues to some mechanisms of consciousness.Michael S. Gazzaniga, J. E. LeDoux & David H. Wilson - 1977 - Neurology 27:1144-1147.