Results for 'Hereditarian'

25 found
Order:
  1.  24
    Eugenics: hereditarian attitudes in American thought.B. S. Bosanquet - 1964 - The Eugenics Review 55 (4):231.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  5
    Benefits of hereditarian insights for mate choice and parenting.Geoffrey F. Miller - 2023 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46:e196.
    Madole & Harden develop some good ideas about how to understand genetic causality more clearly, but they frame the benefits of behavior genetics research at a largely collective level, focused on the pros and cons of different ways to engineer the gene pool or social behavior. This neglects the individual benefits of hereditarian insights for mate choice and parenting.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3. Race, IQ, and the search for statistical signals associated with so-called “X”-factors: environments, racism, and the “hereditarian hypothesis”.Jonathan Michael Kaplan - 2015 - Biology and Philosophy 30 (1):1-17.
    Some authors defending the “hereditarian” hypothesis with respect to differences in average IQ scores between populations have argued that the sorts of environmental variation hypothesized by some researchers rejecting the hereditarian position should leave discoverable statistical traces, namely changes in the overall variance of scores or in variance–covariance matrices relating scores to other variables. In this paper, I argue that the claims regarding the discoverability of such statistical signals are broadly mistaken—there is no good reason to suspect that (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  4. Dodging Darwin: Race, Evolution, and the Hereditarian Hypothesis.Jonny Anomaly - 2020 - Personality and Individual Differences 160.
  5. The Mendelian Revolution: The Emergence of Hereditarian Concepts in Modern Science and Society.Peter J. Bowler - 1989 - Journal of the History of Biology 24 (1):167-168.
  6.  19
    The Mendelian Revolution: The Emergence of Hereditarian Concepts in Modern Science and Society. Peter Bowler.Diane B. Paul - 1991 - Isis 82 (4):773-774.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  8
    Peter J. Bowler. The Mendelian Revolution: The Emergence of Hereditarian Concepts in Modern Science and Society. London: Athlone Press, 1989. Pp. viii + 207. ISBN 0-485-11375-9. £35.00. [REVIEW]Roger Smith - 1990 - British Journal for the History of Science 23 (4):495-496.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  13
    Gerald Sweeney. “Fighting for the Good Cause”: Reflections on Francis Galton’s Legacy to American Hereditarian Psychology. 136 pp., illus., bibl., index. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2001. [REVIEW]Mary Mosher Flesher - 2004 - Isis 95 (2):316-317.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  5
    The Mendelian Revolution: The Emergence of Hereditarian Concepts in Modern Science and Society by Peter Bowler. [REVIEW]Diane Paul - 1991 - Isis 82:773-774.
  10.  38
    Review of The Non-Darwinian Revolution: Reinterpreting a Historical Myth by Peter Bowler; and of The Mendelian Revolution: The Emergence of Hereditarian Concepts in Modern Science and Society by Peter J. Bowler. [REVIEW]Michael Ruse - 1993 - Philosophy of Science 60 (1):171-172.
  11.  11
    Gerald Sweeney, fighting for the good cause: Reflections on Francis galtons legacy to american hereditarian psychology. Transactions of the american philosophical society, 91, part 2. philadelphia: American philosophical society, 2001. Pp. X+136. Isbn 0-87169-912-5. $18.00. [REVIEW]John C. Waller - 2003 - British Journal for the History of Science 36 (2):247-248.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12. “If there is nothing beyond the organic...”: Heredity and Culture at the Boundaries of Anthropology in the Work of Alfred L. Kroeber.Maria E. Kronfeldner - 2009 - NTM Zeitschrift für Geschichte der Wissenschaften, Technik und Medizin 17 (2):107-133.
    Continuing Franz Boas' work to establish anthropology as an academic discipline in the US at the turn of the twentieth century, Alfred L. Kroeber re-defined culture as a phenomenon sui generis. To achieve this he asked geneticists to enter into a coalition against hereditarian thoughts prevalent at that time in the US. The goal was to create space for anthropology as a separate discipline within academia, distinct from other disciplines. To this end he crossed the boundary separating anthropology from (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  13. Philosophy of Science that Ignores Science: Race, IQ and Heritability.Neven Sesardic - 2000 - Philosophy of Science 67 (4):580-602.
    Philosophers of science widely believe that the hereditarian theory about racial differences in IQ is based on methodological mistakes and confusions involving the concept of heritability. I argue that this "received view" is wrong: methodological criticisms popular among philosophers are seriously misconceived, and the discussion in philosophy of science about these matters is largely disconnected from the real, empirically complex issues debated in science.
    Direct download (11 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  14.  15
    The Phenotype/Genotype Distinction and the Disappearance of the Body.Gabriel Gudding - 1996 - Journal of the History of Ideas 57 (3):525-545.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:The Phenotype/Genotype Distinction and the Disappearance of the BodyGabriel GuddingThe discipline of genetics has long been a rhetorical and heuristic locus for social and political issues. As such, the science has influenced culture through the avenues of law, medicine, warfare, social work, and even, since 1972 in California, the education of kindergarten students. It has affected how we view the body, morality, romance, biography, and agency—not to mention procreation (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  15. Mechanism, purpose and progress: Darwin and early American psychology.John D. Greenwood - 2008 - History of the Human Sciences 21 (1):103-126.
    Histories of psychology regularly celebrate the foundational role played in the development of early American psychology by Darwin's theory of evolution through natural selection, and in particular the development of functional psychology and behaviorism. In this article it is argued that although Darwin's theory did play an influential role, early American psychology did not generally reflect the hereditarian determinism of his theory of evolution by natural selection. However, early American psychologists did accept one critical implication of Darwin's theory, which (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  16.  29
    Karl pearson's mathematization of inheritance: From ancestral heredity to Mendelian genetics (1895–1909).M. Eileen Magnello - 1998 - Annals of Science 55 (1):35-94.
    Summary Long-standing claims have been made for nearly the entire twentieth century that the biometrician, Karl Pearson, and his colleague, W. F. R. Weldon, rejected Mendelism as a theory of inheritance. It is shown that at the end of the nineteenth century Pearson considered various theories of inheritance (including Francis Galton's law of ancestral heredity for characters underpinned by continuous variation), and by 1904 he ?accepted the fundamental idea of Mendel? as a theory of inheritance for discontinuous variation. Moreover, in (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  17.  1
    At a Glance:” The Role of Diagrammatic Representations in Eugenics Appropriations of the “Infamous Juke Family.Andrea Ceccon - forthcoming - Journal of the History of Biology:1-37.
    The case of the Juke family is one of the most notable episodes of the history of eugenics in the USA. The Jukes were initially brought to the fore in the 1870s by a famous investigation that aimed at estimating the interplay of heredity and environment in determining the problems of poverty and crime. This inquiry triggered a harsh confrontation between two polar interpretations of the study, an “environmentalist” one and a “hereditarian” one. It was with the later reassessment (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  28
    Biology and the emergence of the Anglo-American eugenics movement.Edward J. Larson - 2010 - In Denis Alexander & Ronald L. Numbers (eds.), Biology and Ideology From Descartes to Dawkins. London: University of Chicago Press.
    In the late 1800s, Charles Darwin and other naturalists supported a blending view of inheritance whereby offspring possess a middling mix of their parents' traits. Many of these naturalists also argued that individuals pass at least some of their acquired characteristics to their descendants. Darwin proposed that acquired characteristics and other environmentally induced changes in a parent's hereditary material account in large part for the inheritable variations that drove evolution. Inspired by the evolutionary theories of his first cousin, Darwin, Francis (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  3
    The complicated legacy of E. O. Wilson with respect to genetics and human behavior.Manuel Lerdau - 2022 - Bioessays 44 (5):2200034.
    Over the arc of his career, E. O. Wilson first embraced, then popularized, and finally rejected an extreme genetical hereditarian view of human nature. The controversy that ensued during the period of popularization (largely in the 1970s and 1980s) obscured the fact that empirical and theoretical research during this time undercut the assumptions necessary for this view. By the end of his career, Wilson accepted the fact that individual/kin selection models were insufficient to explain human behavior and society, and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  5
    “Not by a Decree of Fate:” Ellen Richards, Euthenics, and the Environment in the Progressive Era.David P. D. Munns - 2023 - Journal of the History of Biology 56 (3):525-557.
    In 1904, Ellen Richards introduced “euthenics.” By 1912, Lewellys Barker, director of medicine and physician-in-chief at Johns Hopkins Hospital, would tell the _New York Times_ that the “task of eugenics” and the “task of euthenics” was the “Task for the Nation.” Alongside the emergence of hereditarian eugenics, where fate was firmly rooted in heredity, this article places euthenics into the same Progressive Era demands for the scientific management over environmental issues like life and labor, health and hygiene, sewage and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  17
    Development and Adaptation: Evolutionary Concepts in British Morphology, 1870–1914.Peter J. Bowler - 1989 - British Journal for the History of Science 22 (3):283-297.
    Bernard Norton's research concentrated on the Biometrical school of Darwinism and the social implications of the hereditarian ideas that began to gain popularity in the closing years of the nineteenth century. In this article I want to look at the previous generation of evolutionists, the evolutionary morphologists against whom the Biometricians (and their great rivals, the early Mendelians) were reacting. Despite the prominence of evolutionary morphology in the post-Darwinian era, comparatively little historical work has been done on it. In (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  22.  62
    The bell curve case for heredity.Max Hocutt & Michael Levin - 1999 - Philosophy of the Social Sciences 29 (3):389-415.
    City College of New York The hereditarian theory of race differences in IQ was briefly revived with the appearance of The Bell Curve but then quickly dismissed. The authors attempt a defense of it here, with an eye to conceptual and logical issues of special interests to philosophers, such as alleged infirmities in the heritability concept. At the same time, some relevant post-Bell Curve empirical data are introduced.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  23.  21
    Nature and Nurture in French Ethnography and Anthropology, 1859-1914.Martin S. Staum - 2004 - Journal of the History of Ideas 65 (3):475-495.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Nature and Nurture in French Ethnography and Anthropology, 1859-1914Martin StaumThe adaptability of non-European peoples to "civilization" was a critical issue deriving from the perennial nature-nurture question that haunted debates in the human sciences in late nineteenth-century France.1 The emerging scholarly disciplines of anthropology and ethnography helped provide a scientific veneer that bolstered existing cultural prejudices concerning the innate limitations or retarded development of non-Europeans. Certainly there were many other (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  24.  7
    Parnellites, Playboys, and Pathology: Irish Modernist Drama and the Politics of Sexual Health.Lloyd Houston - 2022 - Journal of Medical Humanities 44 (1):43-59.
    The personal, political, and aesthetic ideals that Irish modernists found embodied in the figure of Charles Stewart Parnell—independence, self-mastery, and a capacity for radical self-fashioning—have been well attested in Irish literary historiography. What has been less often noted is the centrality of sexual health to the conception, articulation, and emulation of those virtues, particularly when attempting to translate Parnell’s public persona to the stage. This essay addresses this lacuna by tracing how a medicalized and politicized conception of sex informed Irish (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  35
    Regression to the Mean: More than a Statistic.Robert J. Smith - 2005 - Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology 25 (1):106-115.
    This article looks at Galton's regression to the mean from several traditionally unrelated but interwoven venues: as a law of trait heredity; as a statistical artifact infiltrating careless research designs; as an illustration of cognitive bias. Hereditarians argue for the first of these, disputed by biogeneticists, who view R to M as a mere correlate of generational traits decline. Research designers busy themselves with the second perspective, but to explain the concept, cavalierly adduce various organismic states that sum as "error (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark