Results for 'Task‐oriented dialog'

993 found
Order:
  1. Utility-Based Generation of Referring Expressions.Markus Guhe - 2012 - Topics in Cognitive Science 4 (2):306-329.
    This paper presents two cognitive models that simulate the production of referring expressions in the iMAP task—a task-oriented dialog. One general model is based on Dale and Reiter’s (1995)incremental algorithm, and the other is a simple template model that has a higher correlation with the data but is specifically geared toward the properties of the iMAP task. The property of the iMAP task environment that is modeled here is that the color feature is unreliable for identifying referents while other (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  2.  51
    Investigating Conversational Dynamics: Interactive Alignment, Interpersonal Synergy, and Collective Task Performance.Riccardo Fusaroli & Kristian Tylén - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (1):145-171.
    This study investigates interpersonal processes underlying dialog by comparing two approaches, interactive alignment and interpersonal synergy, and assesses how they predict collective performance in a joint task. While the interactive alignment approach highlights imitative patterns between interlocutors, the synergy approach points to structural organization at the level of the interaction—such as complementary patterns straddling speech turns and interlocutors. We develop a general, quantitative method to assess lexical, prosodic, and speech/pause patterns related to the two approaches and their impact on (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  3.  10
    A task-oriented taxonomy of visual completion.Carol Yin - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (6):780-781.
    Differences and similarities between modal and amodal completions can only be understood by considering the goals of visual completion: unity, shape, and perceptual quality. Pessoa et al. cannot reject representational accounts of vision because of flaws with isomorphic representations of perceptual quality: representations and processes for perceptual quality (modal completion) and most likely dissociable from those for unity and shape (nonmodal completions).
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  26
    Beyond intersubjectivity: Task orientation and first language use in foreign language discussions.Eric Hauser - 2013 - Pragmatics and Society 4 (3):285-316.
    One type of task interaction that students in a foreign language class may do is using the language they are studying for discussion. This paper analyzes interaction among Japanese university students participating in such discussions in English. The participants are interactionally competent; one source of resources they draw on to construct this competence is their first language, Japanese. Participants occasionally use Japanese to refer to Japanese things. They also use Japanese in the pursuit of intersubjectivity, such as using Japanese to (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  5. Moving from task‐oriented to learning‐oriented strategies on school excursions to museums.Janette Griffin & David Symington - 1997 - Science Education 81 (6):763-779.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  6.  11
    Variations in lecturer task orientation and student perceptions of course effectiveness.Allen J. Schuh - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 11 (3):193-194.
  7.  24
    An experimental study of the role of the ego in work. II. The significance of task-orientation in work.H. B. Lewis & M. Franklin - 1944 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 34 (3):195.
  8.  9
    An empirical assessment of a modified artificially intelligent device use acceptance model—From the task-oriented perspective.Yutao Yang, Jia Luo & Tian Lan - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Artificial intelligence is a cutting-edge technology that has been widely applied in tourism operations. To enhance tourists' experience, many tourism suppliers introduced AI devices to interact with tourists. Previous studies classified AI devices as task- and social- oriented based on their functions; however, current models that explain customers' intention to use AI devices did not reflect the discrepancy between the two different types. Therefore, this paper attempts to fill this gap by proposing a theoretical model for the use of task-oriented (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  6
    The Timing of Utterance Planning in Task-Oriented Dialogue: Evidence from a Novel List-Completion Paradigm.Barthel Mathias, Sauppe Sebastian, C. Levinson Stephen & S. Meyer Antje - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  10.  27
    Collective Contexts in Conversation: Grounding by Proxy.Arash Eshghi & Patrick G. T. Healey - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (2):299-324.
    Anecdotal evidence suggests that participants in conversation can sometimes act as a coalition. This implies a level of conversational organization in which groups of individuals form a coherent unit. This paper investigates the implications of this phenomenon for psycholinguistic and semantic models of shared context in dialog. We present a corpus study of multiparty dialog which shows that, in certain circumstances, people with different levels of overt involvement in a conversation, that is, one responding and one not, can (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  11.  3
    Mechanism design for automated negotiation, and its application to task oriented domains.Gilad Zlotkin & Jeffrey S. Rosenschein - 1996 - Artificial Intelligence 86 (2):195-244.
  12. Displaying Recipiency: Reactive Tokens in Mandarin Task-Oriented Interaction.[author unknown] - 2016
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  1
    Book review: Jun Xu, Displaying Recipiency: Reactive Tokens in Mandarin Task-Oriented Interaction. [REVIEW]Liangtao Lai - 2017 - Discourse Studies 19 (5):602-604.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14.  2
    Speakers’ orientations to directional terms in a map task.Anna Filipi - 2014 - Discourse Studies 16 (3):365-384.
    Drawing on a corpus of eight task-based interactions involving a map task ), this article seeks to focus on how speakers orient to directional terms in an interactional context, and in so doing, to add to features that can be deemed to characterize this speech exchange system. Analysis of the data using conversation analysis showed that features of the talk were confirmation and clarification checks, which provided an opportunity to check understanding and the accuracy of the instruction leading to a (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  29
    Attentional orienting and awareness: Evidence from a discrimination task.María Fernanda López-Ramón, Ana B. Chica, Paolo Bartolomeo & Juan Lupiáñez - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (3):745-755.
    We used several cue–target SOAs and three different degrees of cue predictability , to investigate the role of awareness of cue–target predictability on cueing effects. A group of participants received instructions about the informative value of the cue, while another group did not receive such instructions. Participants were able to extract the predictive value of a spatially peripheral cue and use it to orient attention, whether or not specific instructions about the predictive value of the cue were given, and no (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  16.  13
    Orienting task and study time in facial recognition.John H. Mueller, Michael Carlomusto & Alvin G. Goldstein - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 11 (5):313-316.
  17.  3
    Diagnosis of Oriental Ethics Education in the Middle School and Its Tasks. 이상호 - 2013 - Journal of Ethics: The Korean Association of Ethics 1 (92):1-18.
  18.  7
    Task Dependent Effects of Head Orientation on Perceived Gaze Direction.Tarryn Balsdon & Colin W. G. Clifford - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  15
    Orienting in virtual environments: How are surface features and environmental geometry weighted in an orientation task?Debbie M. Kelly & Walter F. Bischof - 2008 - Cognition 109 (1):89-104.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  20.  28
    Effects of orienting task, practice, and incentive on simultaneous incidental and intentional learning.Arnold Mechanic - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (4):393.
  21.  10
    Strategy Generalization Across Orientation Tasks: Testing a Computational Cognitive Model.Glenn Gunzelmann - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (5):835-861.
    Humans use their spatial information processing abilities flexibly to facilitate problem solving and decision making in a variety of tasks. This article explores the question of whether a general strategy can be adapted for performing two different spatial orientation tasks by testing the predictions of a computational cognitive model. Human performance was measured on an orientation task requiring participants to identify the location of a target either on a map (find‐on‐map) or within an egocentric view of a space (find‐in‐scene). A (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  22.  29
    A spatially oriented decision does not induce consciousness in a motor task.Bruce Bridgeman & Valerie Huemer - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (3):454-464.
    Visual information follows at least two branches in the human nervous system, following a common input stage: a cognitive ''what'' branch governs perception and experience, while a sensorimotor ''how'' branch handles visually guided behavior though its outputs are unconscious. The sensorimotor system is probed with an isomorphic task, requiring a 1:1 relationship between target position and motor response. The cognitive system, in contrast, is probed with a forced qualitative decision, expressed verbally, about the location of a target. Normally, the cognitive (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  23.  15
    Implicit attentional orienting in a target detection task with central cues.Scott A. Peterson & Tanja N. Gibson - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4):1532-1547.
    Studies using Posner’s spatial cueing paradigm have demonstrated that participants can allocate their attention to specific target locations based on the predictiveness of preceding cues. Four experiments were conducted to investigate attentional orienting processes operating in a high probability condition as compared to a low probability condition using various types of centrally-presented cues. Spatially-informative cues resulted in cueing effects for both probability conditions, with significantly larger CEs in the high probability conditions than the low probability conditions. Participants in the high (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  24.  23
    Association value and orienting task in incidental and intentional paired-associate learning.Frank W. Wicker & Alan L. Bernstein - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (2):308.
  25. Effects of orienting task and presentation modality on judged frequency.C. G. Penney - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (5):352-352.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  25
    Making a task difficult: Evidence that device-oriented steps are effortful and error-prone.Maartje Ga Ament, Anna L. Cox, Ann Blandford & Duncan P. Brumby - 2013 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 19 (3):195.
  27.  24
    Anxiety and orienting tasks in picture recognition.John H. Mueller, David J. Miller & Jeffrey L. Hutchings - 1979 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 13 (3):145-148.
  28.  24
    Effects of orienting tasks and instructions about associative structure on free recall and clustering.Robert E. Till, Carroll D. Johnston & James J. Jenkins - 1975 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 6 (4):349-351.
  29.  27
    When do semantic orienting tasks hinder recall?Eugene Winograd & Anderson D. Smith - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 11 (3):165-167.
  30. Reflexive and voluntary orienting in detection and discrimination tasks.R. Egly, R. D. Rafal & A. Henik - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (6):469-469.
  31.  12
    Effects of instructions, orienting task, and memory tests on memory for words and word frequency.James R. Erickson & Carol Renaud Gaffney - 1985 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 23 (4):377-380.
  32.  14
    Control of spatial orienting: Context-specific proportion cued effects in an exogenous spatial cueing task.Alex Gough, Jesse Garcia, Maryem Torres-Quesada & Bruce Milliken - 2014 - Consciousness and Cognition 30:220-233.
  33.  20
    A Study of the Mechanism of the Congruence of Leader–Follower Power Distance Orientation on Employees’ Task Performance.Yan Bao, Shudi Liao, Jianqiao Liao, Yucheng Zhang, Chuanjun Deng & Zhiwen Guo - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10:441710.
    Based on implicit leadership theory, we examine the congruence effect of leader-follower power distance orientation on follower trust in a supervisor and work engagement, which in turn influences employees’ task performance. Results of Polynomial regressions on 526 dyads supported the congruence effect hypothesis. The results show that (1) the congruence of leader-follower power distance orientation leads to better performance; (2) under the condition of congruence, Subordinate task performance is higher when leader-follower power distance orientation matching in low-low ratings congruence than (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  21
    A dialog set within a tower of faith above a city of power: Merian validus.Edward H. Sisson - unknown
    The Washington National Cathedral, set on the highest hill in the capital city of the world's greatest economic and military power, is an iconic location for an examination of the intersection of immaterial faith, material power, and human conscious experience. It is a location made even more symbolic due to the fact that surrounding the Cathedral on three sides are three private schools -- an elementary school (Beauvoir) to the east, a boys' school (St. Albans) to the south, and a (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  3
    Enhanced source memory for emotionally valenced sources: does an affective orienting task make the difference?Nikoletta Symeonidou & Beatrice G. Kuhlmann - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    Previous research on whether source memory is enhanced for emotionally valenced sources yielded inconclusive results. To identify potential boundary conditions, we tested whether encoding instructions that promote affective versus different types of non-affective item-source-processing foster versus hamper source-valence effects. In both experiments, we used neutral words as items superimposed on emotional (positive & negative) or neutral pictures as sources. Source pictures were selected based on valence and arousal ratings collected in a pre-study such that only valence varied across sources. Source (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  15
    Visual and pronouncing responses, and the relation between orienting task and presentations in incidental learning.Arnold Mechanic & Joanne D'Andrea - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (3):343.
  37.  20
    Studies in incidental learning: IV. The interaction of orienting tasks and stimulus materials.Leo Postman & Pauline Austin Adams - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 51 (5):329.
  38.  37
    Differential effects of effort and type of orienting task on recall and organization of highly associated words.Thomas S. Hyde - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 97 (1):111.
  39.  38
    Visibility Is Not Equivalent to Confidence in a Low Contrast Orientation Discrimination Task.Manuel Rausch & Michael Zehetleitner - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
  40.  43
    Attentional asymmetries in a visual orienting task are related to temperament.Kelly G. Garner, Paul E. Dux, Joe Wagner, D. R. Tarrant, Christopher D. Chambers & A. Mark - 2012 - Cognition and Emotion 26 (8):1508-1515.
    Spatial asymmetries are an intriguing feature of directed attention. Recent observations indicate an influence of temperament upon the direction of these asymmetries. It is unknown whether this influence generalises to visual orienting behaviour. The aim of the current study was therefore to explore the relationship between temperament and measures of spatial orienting as a function of target hemifield. An exogenous cueing task was administered to 92 healthy participants. Temperament was assessed using Carver and White's (1994) Behavioural Inhibition System and Behavioural (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  41.  8
    Autonomous vs. heteronomous mode of action control and task performance: The role of the situational context and action vs. state orientation.Romana Kadzikowska-Wrzosek - 2015 - Polish Psychological Bulletin 46 (3):433-446.
    The article presents the results of two experimental studies in which I investigated the effect of the situational context and action vs. state orientation on perseverance and efficacy in task performance. The results of Study 1 confirmed that in a context which supports autonomy - as opposed to one which induces external control - people are much more likely to be not only more persistent and effective in their actions but also much more interested in the performed task. Interest in (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  26
    Intersystemarer dialog in wissenschaftstheorie und -geschichte.Bardo Diehl - 1985 - Journal for General Philosophy of Science / Zeitschrift für Allgemeine Wissenschaftstheorie 16 (2):213-228.
    First of all some problems of a scientifical dialogue between the different systems in the East and the West are being presented. In such a dialogue symmetry and reflexivity as principles of orientation have an important function. These principles are close to reality in the field of the philosophy of science and history of science in the DDR. By the controversy between the philosophy of science and the history of science in the DDR and by the way of discussing the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  9
    Intersystemarer Dialog in Wissenschaftstheorie und -geschichte.Bardo Diehl - 1985 - Zeitschrift Für Allgemeine Wissenschaftstheorie 16 (2):213-228.
    First of all some problems of a scientifical dialogue between the different systems in the East and the West are being presented. In such a dialogue symmetry and reflexivity as principles of orientation have an important function. These principles are close to reality in the field of the philosophy of science and history of science in the DDR. By the controversy between the philosophy of science and the history of science in the DDR and by the way of discussing the (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44. Recognition memory and awareness: A large effect of study-test modalities on "know" responses following a highly perceptual orienting task.V. H. Gregg & John M. Gardiner - 1994 - European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 6:137-47.
  45.  17
    Attentional prioritization reconfigures novel instructions into action-oriented task sets.Carlos González-García, Silvia Formica, Baptist Liefooghe & Marcel Brass - 2020 - Cognition 194 (C):104059.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  46.  23
    Hand and Grasp Selection in a Preferential Reaching Task: The Effects of Object Location, Orientation, and Task Intention.Sara M. Scharoun, Kelly A. Scanlan & Pamela J. Bryden - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  47.  26
    Speech Planning at Turn Transitions in Dialog Is Associated With Increased Processing Load.Mathias Barthel & Sebastian Sauppe - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (7):e12768.
    Speech planning is a sophisticated process. In dialog, it regularly starts in overlap with an incoming turn by a conversation partner. We show that planning spoken responses in overlap with incoming turns is associated with higher processing load than planning in silence. In a dialogic experiment, participants took turns with a confederate describing lists of objects. The confederate’s utterances (to which participants responded) were pre‐recorded and varied in whether they ended in a verb or an object noun and whether (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  48.  53
    Argumentation and Explanation in Conceptual Change: Indications From Protocol Analyses of Peer‐to‐Peer Dialog.Christa S. C. Asterhan & Baruch B. Schwarz - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (3):374-400.
    In this paper we attempt to identify which peer collaboration characteristics may be accountable for conceptual change through interaction. We focus on different socio‐cognitive aspects of the peer dialog and relate these with learning gains on the dyadic as well as the individual level. The scientific topic that was used for this study concerns natural selection, a topic for which students’ intuitive conceptions have been shown to be particularly robust. Learning tasks were designed according to the socio‐cognitive conflict instructional (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  49.  13
    Effects of semantic and perceptual orienting tasks on preschool children’s memory.Marion Perlmutter, Edward J. Schork & Denise Lewis - 1982 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 19 (2):65-68.
  50.  6
    The influence of visual and vestibular orientation cues in a clock reading task.Nicolas Davidenko, Yeram Cheong, Amanda Waterman, Jacob Smith, Barrett Anderson & Sarah Harmon - 2018 - Consciousness and Cognition 64:196-206.
1 — 50 / 993