Results for 'neural representation'

989 found
Order:
  1. Neural representations not needed - no more pleas, please.Daniel D. Hutto & Erik Myin - 2014 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 13 (2):241-256.
    Colombo (Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 2012) argues that we have compelling reasons to posit neural representations because doing so yields unique explanatory purchase in central cases of social norm compliance. We aim to show that there is no positive substance to Colombo’s plea—nothing that ought to move us to endorse representationalism in this domain, on any level. We point out that exposing the vices of the phenomenological arguments against representationalism does not, on its own, advance the case for (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  2. Neural Representations Observed.Eric Thomson & Gualtiero Piccinini - 2018 - Minds and Machines 28 (1):191-235.
    The historical debate on representation in cognitive science and neuroscience construes representations as theoretical posits and discusses the degree to which we have reason to posit them. We reject the premise of that debate. We argue that experimental neuroscientists routinely observe and manipulate neural representations in their laboratory. Therefore, neural representations are as real as neurons, action potentials, or any other well-established entities in our ontology.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  3. Investigating neural representations: the tale of place cells.William Bechtel - 2016 - Synthese 193 (5):1287-1321.
    While neuroscientists often characterize brain activity as representational, many philosophers have construed these accounts as just theorists’ glosses on the mechanism. Moreover, philosophical discussions commonly focus on finished accounts of explanation, not research in progress. I adopt a different perspective, considering how characterizations of neural activity as representational contributes to the development of mechanistic accounts, guiding the investigations neuroscientists pursue as they work from an initial proposal to a more detailed understanding of a mechanism. I develop one illustrative example (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  4.  35
    Neural Representations Beyond “Plus X”.Vivian Cruz & Alessio Plebe - 2018 - Minds and Machines 28 (1):93-117.
    In this paper we defend structural representations, more specifically neural structural representation. We are not alone in this, many are currently engaged in this endeavor. The direction we take, however, diverges from the main road, a road paved by the mathematical theory of measure that, in the 1970s, established homomorphism as the way to map empirical domains of things in the world to the codomain of numbers. By adopting the mind as codomain, this mapping became a boon for (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  5.  5
    The Neural Representation of a Repeated Standard Stimulus in Dyslexia.Sara D. Beach, Ola Ozernov-Palchik, Sidney C. May, Tracy M. Centanni, Tyler K. Perrachione, Dimitrios Pantazis & John D. E. Gabrieli - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    The neural representation of a repeated stimulus is the standard against which a deviant stimulus is measured in the brain, giving rise to the well-known mismatch response. It has been suggested that individuals with dyslexia have poor implicit memory for recently repeated stimuli, such as the train of standards in an oddball paradigm. Here, we examined how the neural representation of a standard emerges over repetitions, asking whether there is less sensitivity to repetition and/or less accrual (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  6.  63
    Neural Representations Beyond “Plus X”.Alessio Plebe & Vivian M. De La Cruz - 2018 - Minds and Machines 28 (1):93-117.
    In this paper we defend structural representations, more specifically neural structural representation. We are not alone in this, many are currently engaged in this endeavor. The direction we take, however, diverges from the main road, a road paved by the mathematical theory of measure that, in the 1970s, established homomorphism as the way to map empirical domains of things in the world to the codomain of numbers. By adopting the mind as codomain, this mapping became a boon for (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  7.  23
    Neural Representations in Context.Alessio Plebe & Vivian M. De La Cruz - 2019 - In Antonino Pennisi & Alessandra Falzone (eds.), The Extended Theory of Cognitive Creativity: Interdisciplinary Approaches to Performativity. Springer Verlag. pp. 285-300.
    In recent years, a number of different disciplines have begun to investigate the fundamental role context appears to play in a number of cognitive phenomena. Traditionally, linguistics, and the fields of communication and pragmatics in particular, have been the areas that have focused the most on contextual effects. Context has increasingly been studied for its role in influencing mental concepts, for some scholars being considered constitutive for most – if not all – concepts. Cognitive neuroscience is now starting to consider (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  93
    Neural representations unobserved—or: a dilemma for the cognitive neuroscience revolution.Marco Facchin - 2023 - Synthese 203 (1):1-42.
    Neural structural representations are cerebral map- or model-like structures that structurally resemble what they represent. These representations are absolutely central to the “cognitive neuroscience revolution”, as they are the only type of representation compatible with the revolutionaries’ mechanistic commitments. Crucially, however, these very same commitments entail that structural representations can be observed in the swirl of neuronal activity. Here, I argue that no structural representations have been observed being present in our neuronal activity, no matter the spatiotemporal scale (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9. Neural representation and neural computation.Patricia Smith Churchland & Terrence J. Sejnowski - 1990 - Philosophical Perspectives 4:343-382.
  10.  31
    Explicit neural representations, recursive neural networks and conscious visual perception.Daniel A. Pollen - 2003 - Cerebral Cortex 13 (8):807-814.
  11. The neural representation of the social world.Paul M. Churchland - 1998 - In The Digital Phoenix. Cambridge: Blackwell.
  12.  13
    Neural Representations of Task Context and Temporal Order During Action Sequence Execution.Danesh Shahnazian, Mehdi Senoussi, Ruth M. Krebs, Tom Verguts & Clay B. Holroyd - 2022 - Topics in Cognitive Science 14 (2):223-240.
    Routine action sequences critically rely on neural mechanisms maintaining contextual and temporal information to disambiguate similar tasks (e.g. making coffee or tea). In this study we show the involvement of areas in temporal and lateral prefrontal cortices in maintaining temporal and contextual information for the execution of hierarchically‐organized action sequences.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  13.  18
    Reconstructing neural representations of tactile space.Luigi Tamè, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Renata Sadibolova, Martin I. Sereno & Matthew R. Longo - 2021 - NeuroImage 229.
    Psychophysical experiments have demonstrated large and highly systematic perceptual distortions of tactile space. Such a space can be referred to our experience of the spatial organisation of objects, at representational level, through touch, in analogy with the familiar concept of visual space. We investigated the neural basis of tactile space by analysing activity patterns induced by tactile stimulation of nine points on a 3 × 3 square grid on the hand dorsum using functional magnetic resonance imaging. We used a (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  14. Neural representation and neural computation.Patricia S. Churchland & Terrence J. Sejnowski - 1989 - In L. Nadel (ed.), Philosophical Perspectives. MIT Press. pp. 343-382.
  15.  14
    Neural Representation. A Survey-Based Analysis of the Notion.Oscar Vilarroya - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
  16.  29
    The Neural Representation of Relational- and Collective-Self: Two Forms of Collectivism.Yingcan Zheng, Zilun Xiao, Luqing Wei & Hong Chen - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  17. Capgras Syndrome: A Novel Probe for Understanding the Neural Representation of the Identity and Familiarity of Persons.William Hirstein & V. S. Ramachandran - 1997 - Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 264:437-444.
  18.  82
    Decoding the Brain: Neural Representation and the Limits of Multivariate Pattern Analysis in Cognitive Neuroscience.J. Brendan Ritchie, David Michael Kaplan & Colin Klein - 2019 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 70 (2):581-607.
    Since its introduction, multivariate pattern analysis, or ‘neural decoding’, has transformed the field of cognitive neuroscience. Underlying its influence is a crucial inference, which we call the decoder’s dictum: if information can be decoded from patterns of neural activity, then this provides strong evidence about what information those patterns represent. Although the dictum is a widely held and well-motivated principle in decoding research, it has received scant philosophical attention. We critically evaluate the dictum, arguing that it is false: (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  19. Fictionalism about Neural Representations.Mark Sprevak - 2013 - The Monist 96 (4):539-560.
    This paper explores a novel form of Mental Fictionalism: Fictionalism about talk of neural representations in cognitive science. This type of Fictionalism promises to (i) avoid the hard problem of naturalising representations, without (ii) incurring the high costs of eliminating useful representation talk. In this paper, I motivate and articulate this form of Fictionalism, and show that, despite its apparent advantages, it faces two serious objections. These objections are: (1) Fictionalism about talk of neural representations ultimately does (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  20. Neural representations used to specify action.Silvia A. Bunge & Michael J. Souza - 2008 - In Silvia A. Bunge & Jonathan D. Wallis (eds.), Neuroscience of rule-guided behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.
  21. Decoding the Brain: Neural Representation and the Limits of Multivariate Pattern Analysis in Cognitive Neuroscience.J. Brendan Ritchie, David Michael Kaplan & Colin Klein - 2016 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science:axx023.
    Since its introduction, multivariate pattern analysis, or ‘neural decoding’, has transformed the field of cognitive neuroscience. Underlying its influence is a crucial inference, which we call the decoder’s dictum: if information can be decoded from patterns of neural activity, then this provides strong evidence about what information those patterns represent. Although the dictum is a widely held and well-motivated principle in decoding research, it has received scant philosophical attention. We critically evaluate the dictum, arguing that it is false: (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  22.  21
    Neural representation of sensory data.Jonathan Polimeni & Eric Schwartz - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (2):207-208.
    In the target article Pylyshyn revives the spectre of the “little green man,” arguing for a largely symbolic representation of visual imagery. To clarify this problem, we provide precise definitions of the key term “picture,” present some examples of our definition, and outline an information-theoretic analysis suggesting that the problem of addressing data in the brain requires a partially analogue and partially symbolic solution. This is made concrete in the ventral stream of object recognition, from V1 to IT cortex.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23. Cognition Without Neural Representation: Dynamics of a Complex System.Inês Hipólito - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    This paper proposes an account of neurocognitive activity without leveraging the notion of neural representation. Neural representation is a concept that results from assuming that the properties of the models used in computational cognitive neuroscience must literally exist the system being modelled. Computational models are important tools to test a theory about how the collected data has been generated. While the usefulness of computational models is unquestionable, it does not follow that neurocognitive activity should literally entail (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  24. The neural representation of Arabic digits in visual cortex.Lien Peters, Bert De Smedt & Hans P. Op de Beeck - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  25.  49
    The neural representation of spatial predicate-argument structures in sign language.Bencie Woll - 2003 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26 (3):300-301.
    Evidence from studies of the processing of topographic and classifier constructions in sign language sentences provides a model of how a mental scene description can be represented linguistically, but it also raises questions about how this can be related to spatial linguistic descriptions in spoken languages and their processing. This in turn provides insights into models of the evolution of language.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  5
    Neural Representation of the English Vowel Feature [High]: Evidence From /ε/ vs. /ɪ.Yan H. Yu & Valerie L. Shafer - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15:629517.
    Many studies have observed modulation of the amplitude of the neural index mismatch negativity (MMN) related to which member of a phoneme contrast [phoneme A, phoneme B] serves as the frequent (standard) and which serves as the infrequent (deviant) stimulus (i.e., AAAB vs. BBBA) in an oddball paradigm. Explanations for this amplitude modulation range from acoustic to linguistic factors. We tested whether exchanging the role of the mid vowel /ε/ vs. high vowel /ɪ/ of English modulated MMN amplitude and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  14
    The neural representation of the gender of faces in the primate visual system: A computer modeling study.Thomas Minot, Hannah L. Dury, Akihiro Eguchi, Glyn W. Humphreys & Simon M. Stringer - 2017 - Psychological Review 124 (2):154-167.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  23
    Neural representation of the sensorimotor speech–action-repository.Cornelia Eckers, Bernd J. Kröger, Katharina Sass & Stefan Heim - 2013 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7.
  29.  31
    What Are Neural Representations? A Cummins Functions Approach.Ori Hacohen - 2022 - Philosophy of Science 89 (4):701-720.
    This paper introduces the Cummins Functions Approach to neural representations, which aims to capture the notion of representation that is relevant to contemporary neuroscientific practice. CFA shares the common view that “to be a representation of X” amounts to “having the function of tracking X,” but maintains that the relevant notion of function is defined by Robert Cummins’s account. Thus, CFA offers a notion of neural representation that is dependent on explanatory context. I argue that (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  30.  23
    Neural representation of three-dimensional acoustic space in the human temporal lobe.Xiaolu Zhang, Qingtian Zhang, Xiaolin Hu & Bo Zhang - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  31.  70
    Memory search and the neural representation of context.Sean M. Polyn & Michael J. Kahana - 2008 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12 (1):24-30.
  32.  26
    Exploring the Neural Representation of Novel Words Learned through Enactment in a Word Recognition Task.Manuela Macedonia & Karsten Mueller - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  33.  76
    Explaining social norm compliance. A plea for neural representations.Matteo Colombo - 2014 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 13 (2):217-238.
    How should we understand the claim that people comply with social norms because they possess the right kinds of beliefs and preferences? I answer this question by considering two approaches to what it is to believe (and prefer), namely: representationalism and dispositionalism. I argue for a variety of representationalism, viz. neural representationalism. Neural representationalism is the conjunction of two claims. First, what it is essential to have beliefs and preferences is to have certain neural representations. Second, (...) representations are often necessary to adequately explain behaviour. After having canvassed one promising way to understand what neural representations could be, I argue that the appeal to beliefs and preferences in explanations of paradigmatic cases of norm compliance should be understood as an appeal to neural representations. (shrink)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  34. Multivariate pattern analysis and the search for neural representations.Bryce Gessell, Benjamin Geib & Felipe De Brigard - 2021 - Synthese 199 (5-6):12869-12889.
    Multivariate pattern analysis, or MVPA, has become one of the most popular analytic methods in cognitive neuroscience. Since its inception, MVPA has been heralded as offering much more than regular univariate analyses, for—we are told—it not only can tell us which brain regions are engaged while processing particular stimuli, but also which patterns of neural activity represent the categories the stimuli are selected from. We disagree, and in the current paper we offer four conceptual challenges to the use of (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  35.  26
    Neurofeedback and the Neural Representation of Self: Lessons From Awake State and Sleep.Andreas A. Ioannides - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12.
  36.  11
    A task-independent neural representation of subjective certainty in visual perception.Johannes Heereman, Henrik Walter & Hauke R. Heekeren - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  37.  38
    What is an (abstract) neural representation of quantity?Manuela Piazza & Veronique Izard - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3-4):348-349.
    We argue that Cohen Kadosh & Walsh's (CK&W's) definitions of neural coding and of abstract representations are overly shallow, influenced by classical cognitive psychology views of modularity and seriality of information processing, and incompatible with the current knowledge on principles of neural coding. As they stand, the proposed dichotomies are not very useful heuristic tools to guide our research towards a better understanding of the neural computations underlying the processing of numerical quantity in the parietal cortex.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  27
    Mental or Neural Representations: Justifying the terminology used in cognitive neuroscience.Fabiana Mesquita Carvalho & Nara M. Figueiredo - 2020 - Veritas – Revista de Filosofia da Pucrs 65 (2):e35387.
    In this paper we investigate whether one of the most common uses of the concept of representation is justifiable by suggesting the conditions under which it can be accepted and how it can be related to mental states. We present mental states in terms of private experiences and public events. We argue that a representation is a relation involving three main elements as well as the user of the representation, and defend that the conditions in which we (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  51
    Efficiency, information theory, and neural representations.Joseph T. Devlin, Matt H. Davis, Stuart A. McLelland & Richard P. Russell - 2000 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (4):475-476.
    We contend that if efficiency and reliability are important factors in neural information processing then distributed, not localist, representations are “evolution's best bet.” We note that distributed codes are the most efficient method for representing information, and that this efficiency minimizes metabolic costs, providing adaptive advantage to an organism.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  2
    Reconstruction of the neural representations of the tactile space.Luigi Tamè, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Renata Sadibolova, Martin I. Sereno & Matthew R. Longo - unknown
    We examined the neural basis of tactile distance perception by analyzing activity patterns induced by tactile stimulation of nine points on a 3 x 3 square grid on the hand dorsum using functional magnetic resonance (fMRI). We used a searchlight approach within pre-defined regions of interests (ROIs) to compute the pairwise Euclidean distances between the activity patterns elicited by tactile stimulation. Then, we used multidimensional scaling (MDS) to reconstruct skin space at the neural level and compare it with (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  50
    A Multitude of Neural Representations Behind Multisensory “Social Norm” Processing.Felipe Pegado, Michelle H. A. Hendriks, Steffie Amelynck, Nicky Daniels, Jessica Bulthé, Haemy Lee Masson, Bart Boets & Hans Op de Beeck - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12.
  42.  4
    Dispositional Self-Construal Modulates Neural Representation of Self: An ERP Study.Jie Chen, Panpan Yuan, Yaohan Cai, Cuihong Liu & Wenjie Li - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  42
    The problem with appealing to history in defining neural representations.Ori Hacohen - 2022 - European Journal for Philosophy of Science 12 (3):1-17.
    Representations seem to play a major role in many neuroscientific explanations. Philosophers have long attempted to properly define what it means for a neural state to be a representation of a specific content. Teleosemantic theories of content which characterize representations, in part, by appealing to a historical notion of function, are often regarded as our best path towards an account of neural representations. This paper points to the anti-representationalist consequences of these accounts. I argue that assuming such (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  44.  34
    Some dilemmas for an account of neural representation: A reply to Poldrack.Michael L. Anderson & Heather Champion - 2022 - Synthese 200 (2).
    “The physics of representation” aims to define the word “representation” as used in the neurosciences, argue that such representations as described in neuroscience are related to and usefully illuminated by the representations generated by modern neural networks, and establish that these entities are “representations in good standing”. We suggest that Poldrack succeeds in, exposes some tensions between the broad use of the term in neuroscience and the narrower class of entities that he identifies in the end, and (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  45.  49
    Weighing in on decisions in the brain: neural representations of pre-awareness practical intention.Robyn Repko Waller - 2021 - Synthese 199 (1-2):5175-5203.
    Neuroscientists have located brain activity that prepares or encodes action plans before agents are aware of intending to act. On the basis of these findings and broader agency research, activity in these regions has been proposed as the neural realizers of practical intention. My aim in this paper is to evaluate the case for taking these neural states to be neural representations of intention. I draw on work in philosophy of action on the role and nature of (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  10
    Multivariate cross-classification: applying machine learning techniques to characterize abstraction in neural representations.Jonas T. Kaplan, Kingson Man & Steven G. Greening - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  47.  40
    Putting the subjective back into intersubjective: The importance of person-specific, distributed, neural representations in perception-action mechanisms.Stephanie D. Preston - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (1):36-37.
    The shared circuits model (SCM) relies on well-regarded theories of perception-action, mirror neurons, and forward models, but the functional/informational level of the model limits its ability to explain complex behavior such as true imitation. Data from our lab and others confirm the more general details of the model, accepted by most, but specify the neural mechanisms involved in perception-action processes.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  48.  27
    How to decide whether a neural representation is a cognitive concept?Maartje E. J. Raijmakers & Peter C. M. Molenaar - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):641-642.
    A distinction should be made between the formation of stimulus-driven associations and cognitive concepts. To test the learning mode of a neural network, we propose a simple and classic input-output test: the discrimination shift task. Feed-forward PDP models appear to form stimulus-driven associations. A Hopfield network should be extended to apply the test.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  39
    A Knowledge-Based Arrangement of Prototypical Neural Representation Prior to Experience Contributes to Selectivity in Upcoming Knowledge Acquisition.Hiroki Kurashige, Yuichi Yamashita, Takashi Hanakawa & Manabu Honda - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12.
  50.  20
    Finding order in our world: The primacy of the concrete in neural representations and the role of invariance in substance reidentification.Bruce J. MacLennan - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (1):78-79.
    I discuss neuroscientific and phenomenological arguments in support of Millikan's thesis. I then consider invariance as a unifying theme in perceptual and conceptual tracking, and how invariants may be extracted from the environment. Finally, some wider implications of Millikan's nondescriptionist approach to language are presented, with specific application to color terms.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
1 — 50 / 989