Results for 'nonsense syllables'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  16
    Compound nonsense-syllable stimuli presented without an intervening space.Barbara S. Musgrave, Albert E. Goss & Elizabeth Shrader - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 (6):609.
  2.  48
    Retention of nonsense syllables in intentional and incidental learning.W. C. Biel & R. C. Force - 1943 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 32 (1):52.
  3.  18
    The relative variability of nonsense syllables and words.F. M. Sauer - 1930 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 13 (3):235.
  4.  11
    "Association" of nonsense syllables following varied learning conditions.Bonnie Webb Camp - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 59 (1):35.
  5.  17
    The effect of nonsense-syllable compound stimuli on latency in a verbal paired associate task.Barbara S. Musgrave - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 63 (5):499.
  6.  18
    Retention of serial nonsense syllables as a function of rest-interval responding rate and meaningfulness.E. James Archer - 1953 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 45 (4):245.
  7.  24
    The relative difficulty of nonsense syllables.W. C. F. Krueger - 1934 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 17 (1):145.
  8.  25
    The reliability of nonsense-syllable scores.J. B. Stroud, A. F. Lehman & C. McCue - 1934 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 17 (2):294.
  9.  19
    The reliability of nonsense syllable scores derived by group method of experimentation.J. B. Stroud - 1936 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 19 (5):621.
  10.  19
    The comparative retention values of maze habits and of nonsense syllables.J. A. McGeoch & A. W. Melton - 1929 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 12 (5):392.
  11.  16
    The strength and direction of associations formed in the learning of nonsense syllables.E. Raskin & S. W. Cook - 1937 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 20 (4):381.
  12.  5
    The comparative retention values of a maze habit, of nonsense syllables, and of rational learning.J. A. McGeoch - 1932 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 15 (6):662.
  13.  16
    Can the superior learnability of meaningful and pleasant words be transferred to nonsense syllables?Albert Silverstein & Richard A. Dienstbier - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 78 (2p1):292.
  14.  28
    Interaction of arousal and recall interval in nonsense syllable paired-associate learning.Lewis J. Kleinsmith & Stephen Kaplan - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (2):124.
  15.  18
    The relative reliability of words and nonsense syllables as learning material.F. C. Davis - 1930 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 13 (3):221.
  16.  27
    Generalization in the initial stages of learning nonsense syllables: I. Integral responses.B. R. Philip & H. E. Peixotto - 1943 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 33 (1):50.
  17.  17
    Relationship between MAS scores and association values of nonsense syllables.E. Philip Trapp & Donald H. Kausler - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 59 (4):233.
  18.  12
    A remote association explanation of the relative difficulty of learning nonsense syllables in a serial list.B. R. Bugelski - 1950 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 40 (3):336.
  19.  14
    Proactive inhibition in the recognition of nonsense syllables.Helen E. Peixotto - 1947 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 37 (1):81.
  20.  25
    Generalization in the initial stages of learning nonsense syllables: II. Partial and inadequate responses.B. R. Philip & H. E. Peixotto - 1943 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 33 (2):136.
  21.  25
    Stimulus generalization of the conditioned eyelid response to structurally similar nonsense syllables.David W. Abbott & Louis E. Price - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (4):368.
  22.  15
    Studies of distributed practice: VIII. Learning and retention of paired nonsense syllables as a function of intralist similarity.Benton J. Underwood - 1953 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 45 (3):133.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  13
    Consonant and Vowel Confusions in Well-Performing Children and Adolescents With Cochlear Implants, Measured by a Nonsense Syllable Repetition Test.Arne Kirkhorn Rødvik, Ole Tvete, Janne von Koss Torkildsen, Ona Bø Wie, Ingebjørg Skaug & Juha Tapio Silvola - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24. Hemispheric biases in processing cvc nonsense syllables.Jb Hellige, At Kujawski & Tl Eng - 1987 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 25 (5):332-332.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  10
    The effects of syllable familiarization on rote learning, association value, and reminiscence.Donald A. Riley & Laura W. Phillips - 1959 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 57 (6):372.
  26.  15
    Learning Words While Listening to Syllables: Electrophysiological Correlates of Statistical Learning in Children and Adults.Ana Paula Soares, Francisco-Javier Gutiérrez-Domínguez, Alexandrina Lages, Helena M. Oliveira, Margarida Vasconcelos & Luis Jiménez - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    From an early age, exposure to a spoken language has allowed us to implicitly capture the structure underlying the succession of speech sounds in that language and to segment it into meaningful units. Statistical learning, the ability to pick up patterns in the sensory environment without intention or reinforcement, is thus assumed to play a central role in the acquisition of the rule-governed aspects of language, including the discovery of word boundaries in the continuous acoustic stream. Although extensive evidence has (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  27.  19
    Rote learning as a function of distribution of practice and the complexity of the situation.Donald A. Riley - 1952 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 43 (2):88.
  28.  26
    Acquired pleasantness as a stimulus and a response variable in paired-associate learning.Albert Silverstein - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 102 (3):534.
  29.  16
    Is the acquired-pleasantness effect in paired-associate learning free from confounding by meaningfulness and similarity?Albert Silverstein - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 97 (1):116.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  12
    Effects of association value on perceptual search.Edward E. Smith & Howard Egeth - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (5):687.
  31.  26
    Denotative meaning established by classical conditioning.Arthur W. Staats, Carolyn K. Staats & William G. Heard - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (4):300.
  32.  61
    Meaning established by classical conditioning.Carolyn K. Staats & Arthur W. Staats - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 54 (1):74.
  33.  16
    An experimental analogue of repression: III. The effect of induced failure and success on memory measured by recall.Anchard Frederic Zeller - 1951 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 42 (1):32.
  34.  9
    The alleged retroactive effect of visual stimuli subsequent to a given response.F. A. Courts - 1937 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 20 (2):144.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  8
    Association by contiguity: Role of response availability.Geoffrey Keppel - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (4):624.
  36.  20
    Effects of situational anxiety at different stages of practice.Joan L. Bardach - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 59 (6):420.
  37.  10
    The correlations between different memories.Faye Bennett - 1916 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 (5):404.
  38.  8
    Functional stimulus learning as related to degree of practice and meaningfulness.Kenneth L. Leicht & Donald H. Kausler - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (1):100.
  39.  8
    Experimental studies in rote-learning theory. VI. Comparison of retention following learning to same criterion by massed and distributed practice. [REVIEW]C. I. Hovland - 1940 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 26 (6):568.
  40.  8
    An attempt to reconcile unlearning and reproductive inhibition explanations of proactive inhibition.B. R. Bugelski - 1948 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 38 (6):670.
  41.  4
    A culture-free learning task.B. R. Bugelski & Sandra Lattanzio - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 93 (2):354.
  42.  16
    Explanation of serial learning errors within Deese-Kresse categories.E. Rae Harcum & Edwin W. Coppage - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (3):489.
  43.  15
    Supplementary report: Familiarity or low validity?E. James Archer - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 62 (2):207.
  44.  28
    Studies in incidental learning: IX. A comparison of the methods of successive and single recalls.Leo Postman & Laura W. Phillips - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (3):236.
  45.  8
    Retroactive effect and degree of similarity.N. Y. Cheng - 1929 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 12 (5):444.
  46.  22
    Effect of constant versus varied pairing of simultaneous intentional- and incidental-learning materials with different rates and numbers of exposures.Marilyn E. Miller & Virginia Lakso - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (3):256.
  47.  11
    Factors influencing the occurrence of reminiscence: attempted formal rehearsal during the interpolated period.John H. Rohrer - 1949 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 39 (4):484.
  48.  19
    A study of the relative amounts of forward and backward associations of verbal material.T. G. Hermans - 1936 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 19 (6):769.
  49.  13
    Muscle responses and their relation to rote learning.R. N. Berry & R. C. Davis - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (2):188.
  50.  10
    Stimulus conditions and retroactive inhibition.Joel Greenspoon & Redge Ranyard - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (1):55.
1 — 50 / 1000