Linked bibliography for the SEP article "Associationist Theories of Thought" by Eric Mandelbaum

This is an automatically generated and experimental page

If everything goes well, this page should display the bibliography of the aforementioned article as it appears in the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, but with links added to PhilPapers records and Google Scholar for your convenience. Some bibliographies are not going to be represented correctly or fully up to date. In general, bibliographies of recent works are going to be much better linked than bibliographies of primary literature and older works. Entries with PhilPapers records have links on their titles. A green link indicates that the item is available online at least partially.

This experiment has been authorized by the editors of the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. The original article and bibliography can be found here.

  • Anderson, J., K. Spoehr, and D. Bennett, 1994, “A Study in Numerical Perversity: Teaching Arithmetic to a Neural Network”, in Neural Networks for Knowledge Representation and Inference, D. Levine and M. Aparicio IV (eds.), East Sussex: Psychology Press, pp. 311–335. (Scholar)
  • Armstrong, K., S. Kose, L. Williams, A. Woolard, and S. Heckers, 2012, “Impaired Associative Inference in Patients with Schizophrenia”, Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(3): 622–629. (Scholar)
  • Asch, S., 1962, “A Problem in the Theory of Associations”, Psychologische Beitrage, (6): 553–563. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1969, “A Reformulation of the Problem of Association”, American Psychologist, 24(2): 92–102. (Scholar)
  • Aydede, M., 1997, “Language of Thought: The Connectionist Contribution”, Minds and Machines, 7(1): 57–101. (Scholar)
  • Baeyens, F., P. Eelen, O. Van den Bergh, and G. Crombez, 1990, “Flavor-Flavor and Color-Flavor Conditioning in Humans”, Learning and Motivation, 21(4): 434–455. (Scholar)
  • Baeyens,F., P. Eelen, and G. Crombez, 1995, “Pavlovian Associations are Forever: On Classical Conditioning and Extinction”, Journal of Psychophysiology, 9(2): 127–141. (Scholar)
  • Bar-Anan Y., B. Nosek, and M. Vianello, 2009, “The Sorting Paired Features Task: A Measure of Association Strengths”, Experimental Psychology, 56(5): 329–343. (Scholar)
  • Bates, E. and B. MacWhinney, 1987, “Competition, Variation, and Language Learning”, in B. MacWhinney (ed.), Mechanisms of Language Acquisition, Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 157–193. (Scholar)
  • Bendana, J. and E. Mandelbaum, forthcoming, “The Fragmentation of Belief”, in D. Kindermann, C. Borgoni, and A. Onofri (eds.), The Fragmented Mind, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Berger, J., 2020, “Implicit attitudes and awareness”, Synthese, 197(3): 1291–1312. (Scholar)
  • Bernstein, I. and M. Webster, 1980, “Learned Taste Aversions in Humans”, Physiology and Behavior, 25(3): 363–366. (Scholar)
  • Bernstein, I., 1985, “Learned Food Aversions in the Progression of Cancer and its Treatment”, in N. Braveman and P. Bronstein, (eds.), Experimental Assessments and Clinical Applications of Conditioned Food Aversions, New York: New York Academy of Sciences, pp. 365–80. (Scholar)
  • Black, W. and W. Prokasy (eds.), 1972, Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. (Scholar)
  • Bloom, P., 2000, How Children Learn the Meanings of Words, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Bouton, M., 2002, “Context, Ambiguity, and Unlearning: Sources of Relapse after Behavioral Extinction”, Biological Psychiatry, 52(10): 976–986. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2004, “Context and Behavioral Processes in Extinction”, Learning and Memory, 11(5): 485–494. (Scholar)
  • Brett, L., W. Hankins, and J. Garcia, 1976, “Prey-Lithium Aversions. III: Buteo hawks”, Behavioral Biology, 17(1): 87–98. (Scholar)
  • Camp, L., 2007, “Thinking with Maps”, Philosophical Perspectives, 21(1): 145–182. (Scholar)
  • Carey, S., 1978a, “Less May Never Mean More”, in R. Campbell and P. Smith, (eds.), Recent Advances in the Psychology of Language, New York: Plenum Press, p. 109–132. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1978b, “The Child as Word Learner”, in J. Bresnan, G. Miller, and M. Halle, (eds.), Linguistic Theory and Psychological Reality, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 264–293. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2010, “Beyond Fast Mapping”, Language Learning and Development, 6(3): 184–205. (Scholar)
  • Carey, S. and E. Bartlett, 1978, “Acquiring a Single New Word”, Proceedings of the Stanford Child Language Conference, 15: 17–29. (Scholar)
  • Caselli, M.C., E. Bates, P. Casadio, J. Fenson, L. Fenson, L. Sanderl, and J. Weir, 1995, “A Cross-linguistic Study of Early Lexical Development”, Cognitive Development, 10(2): 159–199. (Scholar)
  • Chaiken, S. and Y. Trope (eds.), 1999, Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology, New York: Guilford Press. (Scholar)
  • Chalmers, D., 1993, “Connectionism and Compositionality: Why Fodor and Pylyshyn Were Wrong”, Philosophical Psychology, 6(3): 305–319. (Scholar)
  • Chater, N., 2009, “Rational Models of Conditioning”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(2): 204–205. (Scholar)
  • –––, J. Tenenbaum, and A. Yuille, 2006, “Probabilistic Models of Cognition: Conceptual Foundations”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(7): 287–291. (Scholar)
  • Chomsky, N., 1959, “A Review of B.F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior”, Language, 35(1): 26–58. (Scholar)
  • Churchland, P., 1986, “Some Reductive Strategies in Cognitive Neurobiology”, Mind, 95(379): 279–309. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1989, A Neurocomputational Perspective: The Nature of Mind and the Structure of Science, Cambridge, MA: MIT. (Scholar)
  • Churchland, P. and T. Sejnowski, 1990, “Neural Representation and Neural Computation”, Philosophical Perspectives, 4: 343–382. (Scholar)
  • Collins, A. and E. Loftus, 1975, “A Spreading-Activation Theory of Semantic Processing”, Psychological Review, 82(6): 407–428. (Scholar)
  • Danks D., 2013, “Moving from Levels and Reduction to Dimensions and Constraints”, Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 35: 2124–2129. (Scholar)
  • De Houwer, J., 2009, “The Propositional Approach to Associative Learning as an Alternative for Association Formation Models”, Learning & Behavior, 37(1): 1–20. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2011, “Evaluative Conditioning: A Review of Procedure Knowledge and Mental Process Theories”, in T. Schachtman and S. Reilly (eds.), Associative Learning and Conditioning Theory: Human and Non-Human Applications, New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 399–416. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2014, “A Propositional of Implicit Evaluation”, Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8(7): 342–353. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2018, “Propositional Models of Evaluative Conditioning”, Social Psychological Bulletin, 13(2): 1–21. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2019, “Moving Beyond System 1 and System 2: Conditioning, Implicit Evaluation, and Habitual Responding Might Be Mediated by Relational Knowledge”, Experiental Psychology, 66(4): 257–265. (Scholar)
  • De Houwer, J., S. Thomas, and F. Baeyens, 2001, “Association Learning of Likes and Dislikes: A Review of 25 years of Research on Human Evaluative Conditioning”, Psychological Bulletin, 127(6): 853–869. (Scholar)
  • Dehaene, S., 2011, The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Diaz, E., G. Ruis, and F. Baeyens, 2005, “Resistance to Extinction of Human Evaluative Conditioning Using a Between-Subjects Design”, Cognition and Emotion, 19(2): 245–268. (Scholar)
  • Dickinson, A., D. Shanks, and J. Evenden, 1984, “Judgment of Act-Outcome Contingency: The role of Selective Attribution”, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36(1): 29–50. (Scholar)
  • Dirikx, T., D. Hermans, D. Vansteenwegen, F. Baeyens, and P. Eelen, 2004, “Reinstatement of Extinguished Conditioned Responses and Negative Stimulus Valence as a Pathway to Return of Fear in Humans”, Learning and Memory, 11: 549–54. (Scholar)
  • Elman, J., 1991, “Distributed Representations, Simple Recurrent Networks, and Grammatical Structure”, Machine learning, 7(2–3): 195–225. (Scholar)
  • Elman, J., E. Bates, M. Johnson, A. Karmiloff-Smith, D. Parisi, and K. Plunkett, 1996, Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Evans, G., 1982, The Varieties of Reference, J. McDowell (ed.), Oxford: Clarendon Press. (Scholar)
  • Evans, J., and K. Frankish (eds.), 2009, In Two Minds: Dual Processes and Beyond, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, and K. Stanovich, 2013, “Dual-Process Theories of Higher Cognition: Advancing the Debate,” Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3): 223–241. (Scholar)
  • Fazio, R., 2007, “Attitudes as Object-Evaluation Associations of Varying Strength”, Social Cognition, 25(5): 603–637. (Scholar)
  • Festinger, L. and J. Carlsmith, 1959, “Cognitive Consequences of Forced Compliance”, The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58(2): 203–210. (Scholar)
  • Field, A. and G. Davey, 1999, “Reevaluating Evaluative Conditioning: A Nonassociative Explanation of Conditioning Effects in the Visual Evaluative Conditioning Paradigm”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 25(2): 211–224. (Scholar)
  • Fodor, J., 1983, The Modularity of Mind, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2001, The Mind Doesn’t Work that Way, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2003, Hume Variations, Oxford: Clarendon Press. (Scholar)
  • Fodor, J., and B. McLaughlin, 1990, “Connectionism and the Problem of Systematicity: Why Smolensky’s Solution Doesn’t Work”, Cognition, 35(2): 183–204. (Scholar)
  • Fodor, J., and Z. Pylyshyn, 1988, “Connectionism and Cognitive Architecture: A Critical Analysis”, Cognition, 28(1–2): 3–71. (Scholar)
  • Frankish, K., 2009, “Systems and Levels: Dual-System Theories and the Personal-Subpersonal Distinction”, in Evans and Frankish 2009: pp.89–107. (Scholar)
  • Gagliano, M., V. Vyazovsky, A. Borbely, M. Grimonprez, and M. Depczynski, 2016, “Learning by Association in Plants”, Scientific Reports, 6(38427): 1–8. (Scholar)
  • Gallistel, C., S. Fairhurst, and P. Balsam, 2004, “The Learning Curve: Implications of a Quantitative Analysis”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(36): 13124–13131. (Scholar)
  • Gallistel, C., and A. King, 2009, Memory and the Computational Brain: Why Cognitive Science Will Transform Neuroscience, West Sussex: Wiley Blackwell. (Scholar)
  • Garcia, J., 1981, “Tilting at the Paper Mills of Academe”, American Psychologist, 36(2): 149–158. (Scholar)
  • Garcia, J., R. Kovner, and K. Green, 1970, “Cue Properties vs Palatability of Flavors in Avoidance Learning”, Psychonomic Science, 20(5): 313–314. (Scholar)
  • Garcia, J., B. McGowan, and K. Green, 1972, “Biological Constraints on Conditioning II”, in Black and Prokasy 1972: pp.3–27. (Scholar)
  • Garcia, J., W. Hankins, and K. Rusiniak, 1974, “Behavioral Regulation of the Milieu Interne in Man and Rat”, Science, 185(4154): 824–831. (Scholar)
  • Garcia, J., R.A. Koelling, 1966, “Relationship of cue to consequence in avoidance learning”, Psychonomic Science, 4: 123–124. (Scholar)
  • Gendler, T., 2008, “Alief and Belief”, Journal of Philosophy, 105(10): 634–63. (Scholar)
  • Gleitman, L., K. Cassidy, R. Nappa, A. Papafragou, and J. Trueswell, 2005, “Hard Words”, Language Learning and Development, 1(1): 23–64. (Scholar)
  • Glosser, G. and R. Freidman, 1991, “Lexical but not Semantic Priming in Alzheimer’s Disease”, Psychology and Aging, 6(4): 522–27. (Scholar)
  • Goldin-Meadow, S., M. Seligman, and S. Gelman, 1976, “Language in the Two-Year Old”, Cognition, 4(2): 189–202. (Scholar)
  • Greenwald, A., D. McGhee, and J. Schwartz, 1998, “Measuring Individual Differences in Implicit Cognition: The Implicit Association Test”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6): 1464–1480. (Scholar)
  • Hahn, A., C. Judd, H. Hirsch, and I. Blair, 2014, “Awareness of Implicit Attitudes”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3): 1369–1392. (Scholar)
  • Heyes, C., 2012, “Simple Minds: A Qualified Defence of Associative Learning”, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1603): 2695–2703. (Scholar)
  • Hughes, S., Y. Ye, P. Van Dessel, and J. De Houwer, 2019, “When people co occur with good or bad events: Graded effects of relational qualifiers on evaluative conditioning.”, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(2): 196–208. (Scholar)
  • Hull, C., 1943, Principles of Behavior, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. (Scholar)
  • Hume, D., 1738, A Treatise of Human Nature, L.A. Selby-Bigge (ed.), 2nd ed., revised by P.H. Nidditch, Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1975. (Scholar)
  • James, W., 1890, The Principles of Psychology (Vol. 1), New York: Holt. (Scholar)
  • Johnson, K., 2004, “On the Systematicity of Language and Thought”, Journal of Philosophy, 101(3): 111–139. (Scholar)
  • Kahneman, D., 2011, Thinking, Fast and Slow, New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. (Scholar)
  • Kamin, L., 1969, “Predictability, Surprise, Attention, and Conditioning”, in B. Campbell and R. Church (eds.), Punishment and Aversive Behavior, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, pp. 279–296. (Scholar)
  • Kant, I., 1781/1787, Critique of Pure Reason, in P. Guyer and A. Wood (eds.), Critique of Pure Reason, New York: Cambridge University Press. (Scholar)
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A., 1995, Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books. (Scholar)
  • Kruglanski, A., 2013, “Only One? The Default Interventionist Perspective as a Unimodel—Commentary on Evans & Stanovich”, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3): 242–247. (Scholar)
  • Kurdi, B., and M. Banaji, 2017, “Repeated evaluative pairings and evaluative statements: How effectively do they shift implicit attitudes?”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(2): 194–213. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2019, “Attitude change via repeated evaluative pairings versus evaluative statements: Shared and unique features”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 116(5): 681–703. (Scholar)
  • Locke, J., 1690, An Essay Concerning Human Understanding, in Peter H. Nidditch (ed.), An Essay Concerning Human Understanding, Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1975, (Scholar)
  • Logue, A., I. Ophir, and K. Strauss, 1981, “The Acquisition of Taste Aversion in Humans”, Behavioral Research and Therapy, 19(4): 319–33. (Scholar)
  • Luka, B., and L. Barsalou, 2005, “Structural facilitation: Mere exposure effects for grammatical acceptability as evidence for syntactic priming in comprehension”, Journal of Memory and Language, 52: 444–467. (Scholar)
  • Lycan, W, 1990, “The Continuity of the Levels of Nature”, in W. Lycan (ed.), Mind and Cognition: A Reader, Cambridge: Basil Blackwell, pp. 77–96. (Scholar)
  • Madva, A., and M. Brownstein, 2018, “Stereotypes, Prejudice, and the Taxonomy of the Implicit Social Mind”, Nous, 52(3): 611–644. (Scholar)
  • Mandelbaum, E., 2013a, “Against Alief”, Philosophical Studies, 165(1): 197–211. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2013b, “Numerical Architecture”, Topics in Cognitive Science, 5(2): 367–386. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2016, “Attitude, Inference, Association: On the Propositional Structure of Implicit Attitudes”, Nous, 50(3): 629–658. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2017, “Seeing and Conceptualizing: Modularity and the Shallow Contents of Vision”, Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 97(2): 267–283. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2019, “Troubles with Bayesianism: An Introduction to the Psychological Immune System”, Mind & Language, 34(2): 141–157. (Scholar)
  • Mann, T., and M. Ferguson, 2015, “Can we undo our first impressions? The role of reinterpretation in reversing implicit evaluations”, Journal of Social and Personality Psychology, 108(6): 823–849. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2017, “Reversing implicit first impressions through reinterpretation after a two-day delay.”, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 68: 122–127. (Scholar)
  • Mann, T., B. Kurdi, and M. Banaji, 2019, “ How effectively can implicit evaluations be updated? Using evaluative statements after aversive repeated evaluative pairings”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, doi: 10.1037/xge0000701. (Scholar)
  • Markman, E., 1989, Categorization and Naming in Children: Problems of Induction, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Markson, L. and P. Bloom, 1997, “Evidence Against a Dedicated System for Word Learning in Children”, Nature, 385(6619): 813–815. (Scholar)
  • Marr, D., 1982, Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information, NY: W.H. Freeman and Co. (Scholar)
  • Mason, M. and M. Bar, 2012, “The Effect of Mental Progression on Mood”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(2): 217–221. [Mason and Bar 2012 available on line] (Scholar)
  • McClelland, J., M. Botvinick, D. Noelle, D. Plaut, T. Rogers, M. Seidenberg, and L. Smith, 2010, “Letting Structure Emerge: Connectionist and Dynamic Systems Approaches to Cognition”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(8): 348–356. (Scholar)
  • Minsky, M., 1963, “Steps toward Artificial Intelligence”, in E. Feigenbaum and J. Feldman (eds.), Computers And Thought, New York, NY: McGraw-Hill, pp. 406–450. (Scholar)
  • Mitchell, C., J. De Houwer, and P. Lovibond, 2009, “The Propositional Nature of Human Associative Learning”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(2): 183–246. (Scholar)
  • Nosek, B. and M. Banaji, 2001, “The Go/No-Go Association Task”, Social Cognition, 19(6): 625–66. (Scholar)
  • Osman, M., 2013, “A Case Study Dual-Process Theories of Higher Cognition—Commentary on Evans & Stanovich”, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3): 248–252. (Scholar)
  • Pavlov, I., 1906, “The Scientific Investigation of the Psychical Faculties or Processes in the Higher Animals”, Science, 24(620): 613–619. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1927, Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Payne, B., Cheng, C., Govorun, O., and Stewart, B., 2005, “An Inkblot for Attitudes: Affect Misattribution as Implicit Measurement”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3): 277–293. (Scholar)
  • Perea, M. and E. Rosa, 2002, “The Effects of Associative and Semantic Priming in the Lexical Decision Task”, Psychological Research, 66(3): 180–194. (Scholar)
  • Prinz, J., 2002, Furnishing the Mind: Concepts and their Perceptual Basis, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • ––– and A. Clark, 2004, “Putting Concepts to Work: Some Thoughts for the 21st Century”, Mind & Language, 19(1): 57–69. (Scholar)
  • Quilty-Dunn, J. forthcoming, “Perceptual Pluralism”, Nous, 1–41. (Scholar)
  • Quilty-Dunn, J. and E. Mandelbaum, 2018, “Inferential Transitions”, Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 96(3): 532–547. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2019, “Non-Inferential Transitions: Imagery and Association”, in T. Chan and A. Nes (eds.),Inference and Consciousness, New York: Routledge, pp. 151–171. (Scholar)
  • Rescorla, R., 1968, “Probability of Shock in the Presence and Absence of CS in Fear Conditioning”, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66(1): 1–5. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1988, “Pavlovian Conditioning: It’s Not What You Think It Is”, American Psychologist, 43(3): 151–160. (Scholar)
  • Rescorla, E., and A. Wagner, 1972, “A Theory of Pavlovian Conditioning: Variations in the Effectiveness of Reinforcement and Nonreinforcement”, in Black and Prokasy 1972, pp. 64–99. (Scholar)
  • Roll, D. and J. Smith, 1972, “Conditioned Taste Aversion in Anesthetized Rats”, in M. Hager and J. Seligman (eds.), Biological Boundaries of Learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, pp. 98–102. (Scholar)
  • Rozin, P., 1986, “One-Trial Acquired Likes and Dislikes in Humans: Disgust as a US, Food Predominance, and Negative Learning Predominance”, Learning and Motivation, 17(2): 180–189. (Scholar)
  • Rumelhart, D., P. Smolensky, J. McClelland, and G. Hinton, 1986, “Sequential Thought Processes in PDP Models”, in J.McClelland and D. Rumelhart (eds.), Parallel Distributed Processing Vol. 2: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition: Psychological and Biological Models, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 7–57. (Scholar)
  • Rusiniak, K., W. Hankins, J. Garcia, and L. Brett, 1979, “Flavor-illness Aversions: Potentiation of Odor by Taste in Rats”, Behavioral and Neural Biology, 25(1): 1–17. (Scholar)
  • Rydell, R. and A. McConnell, 2006, “Understanding Implicit and Explicit Attitude Change: A Systems of Reasoning Analysis”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6): 995–1008. (Scholar)
  • Sandhoffer, C., L. Smith, and J. Luo, 2000, “Counting Nouns and Verbs in the Input: Differential Frequencies, Different Kinds of Learning?”, Journal of Child Language, 27(3): 561–585. (Scholar)
  • Seligman, M., 1970, “On the Generality of the Laws of Learning”, Psychological Review, 77(5): 406–418. (Scholar)
  • Shanks, D., 2010, “Learning: From Association to Cognition”, Annual Review of Psychology, 1, 273–301. (Scholar)
  • Skinner, B., 1938, The Behavior of Organisms: An Experimental Analysis, Oxford: Appleton-Century. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1953, Science and Human Behavior, New York: Simon and Schuster. (Scholar)
  • Sloman, S., 1996, “The Empirical Case for Two Systems of Reasoning”, Psychological Bulletin, 119(1): 3–22. (Scholar)
  • Smith, E. R. and J. DeCoster, 2000, “Dual-Process Models in Social and Cognitive Psychology: Conceptual Integration and Links to Underlying Memory Systems”, Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(2): 108–131. (Scholar)
  • Smith, J. and D. Roll, 1967, “Trace Conditioning with X-rays as an Aversive Stimulus”, Psychonomic Science, 9(1): 11–12. (Scholar)
  • Smolensky, P., 1988, “On the Proper Treatment of Connectionism”, Behavioral and Bruin Sciences, 11(1): l–23. (Scholar)
  • Snedeker, J. and L. Gleitman, 2004, “Why it is Hard to Label Our Concepts”, in D. Hall and S. Waxman (eds.), Weaving a Lexicon, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 257–294. (Scholar)
  • Stanovich, K., 2011, Rationality and the Reflective Mind, New York: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Tenenbaum, J., C. Kemp, T. Griffiths, and N. Goodman, 2011, “How to Grow a Mind: Statistics, Structure, and Abstraction”, Science, 331(6022): 1279–1285. (Scholar)
  • Thorndike, E., 1911, Animal intelligence: Experimental studies, New York: Macmillan. (Scholar)
  • Todrank, J., D. Byrnes, A. Wrzesniewski, and P. Rozin, 1995, “Odors can Change Preferences for People in Photographs: A Cross-Modal Evaluative Conditioning Study with Olfactory USs and Visual CSs”, Learning and Motivation, 26(2): 116–140. (Scholar)
  • Tolman, E., 1948, “Cognitive Maps in Rats and Men”, Psychological Review, 55(4): 189–208. (Scholar)
  • Van Dessel, P., Y. Ye, and J. De Houwer 2019, “Chaning deep-rooted implicit evaluation in the blink of an eye: engative verbal information shifts automatic liking of Gandhi”, Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10(2): 266–273. (Scholar)
  • Van Gelder, T., 1995, “What Might Cognition Be, If not Computation?”, The Journal of Philosophy, 91(7): 345–381. (Scholar)
  • Vansteenwegen, D., G. Francken, B. Vervliet, A. De Clercq, and P. Eelen, 2006, “Resistance to Extinction in Evaluative Conditioning”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32(1): 71–79. (Scholar)
  • Wilson, T., S. Lindsey, and T. Schooler, 2000, “A Model of Dual Attitudes”, Psychological Review, 107(1): 101–26. [Wilson, Lindsey, and Schooler 2000 available online] (Scholar)

Generated Sun May 22 21:36:55 2022