Linked bibliography for the SEP article "Bodily Awareness" by Frédérique de Vignemont

This is an automatically generated and experimental page

If everything goes well, this page should display the bibliography of the aforementioned article as it appears in the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, but with links added to PhilPapers records and Google Scholar for your convenience. Some bibliographies are not going to be represented correctly or fully up to date. In general, bibliographies of recent works are going to be much better linked than bibliographies of primary literature and older works. Entries with PhilPapers records have links on their titles. A green link indicates that the item is available online at least partially.

This experiment has been authorized by the editors of the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. The original article and bibliography can be found here.

  • Alsmith, A.J.T., 2012, “The concept of a structural affordance”, Avant: The Journal of the Philosophical-Interdisciplinary Vanguard, 3(2): 94–107. (Scholar)
  • –––, forthcoming, “Mental Activity & the Sense of Ownership”, Review of Philosophy and Psychology. (Scholar)
  • Andersen, E. S., 1978, “Lexical universals of body-part terminology”, in J. H. Greenberg (ed.), Universals of human language, Stanford: Stanford University Press, 335–368. (Scholar)
  • Anema, H. A., van Zandvoort, M. J., de Haan, E. H., Kappelle, L. J., de Kort, P. L., Jansen, B. P., et al., 2009, “A double dissociation between somatosensory processing for perception and action”, Neuropsychologia, 47: 1615–1620.
  • Anscombe, E., 1962, “On Sensations of Position”, Analysis, 22(3): 55–58. (Scholar)
  • Armstrong, D., 1962, Bodily sensations, London: Routledge and Paul. (Scholar)
  • Aydede, M. (ed.), 2006, Pain: New Essays on Its Nature and the Methodology of Its Study, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2009, “Is Feeling Pain the Perception of Something?Journal of Philosophy, 106(10): 531–567. (Scholar)
  • Baier, K., 1964, “The place of pain”, The Philosophical Quarterly, 14(55): 138–150. (Scholar)
  • Bain, D., 2003, “Intentionalism and pain”, The Philosophical Quarterly, 53(213): 502–523. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2011, “The imperative view of pain”, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 18(9–10): 164–185. (Scholar)
  • Bayne, T., and Pacherie, E., 2007, “Narrators and comparators: The architecture of agentive self-awareness”, Synthese, 159: 475–491. (Scholar)
  • Berlucchi, G. and Aglioti, S., 1997, “The body in the brain: neural bases of corporeal awareness”, Trends in neuroscience, 20(12): 560–564. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2010, “The body in the brain revisited”, Experimental Brain Research, 200(1): 25–35. (Scholar)
  • Bermudez, J.L., 1998, The paradox of self-consciousness, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2005, “The phenomenology of bodily awareness”, in D. Woodruff Smith and A. Thomasson (eds.), Phenomenology and philosophy of mind, Oxford: Clarendon Press, 295–316. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2011, “Bodily awareness and self-consciousness”, in S. Gallagher (ed.), Oxford handbook of the self, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2015, “Bodily ownership, bodily awareness and knowledge without observation”, Analysis, 75(1): 37–45. (Scholar)
  • Biederman, I., 1987, “Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding”, Psychological Review, 94: 115–147. (Scholar)
  • Bisiach, E., Rusconi, M.L., and Vallar, G., 1991, “Remission of somatoparaphrenic delusion through vestibular stimulation”, Neuropsychologia, 29(10): 1029–31. (Scholar)
  • Blakemore, S-J, Bristow, D., Bird, G., Frith, C., Ward, J., 2005, “Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision-touch synaesthesia”, Brain, 128: 1571–1583. (Scholar)
  • Blanke, O., Mohr, C., 2005, “Out-of-body experience, heautoscopy, and autoscopic hallucination of neurological origin. Implications for neurocognitive mechanisms of corporeal awareness and self-consciousness”, Brain Research Review, 50(1): 184–99. (Scholar)
  • Blanke, O., Ionta, S., Fornari, E., Mohr, C., Maeder, P., 2010, “Mental imagery for full and upper human bodies: common right hemisphere activations and distinct extrastriate activations”, Brain Topography , 23(3): 321–32. (Scholar)
  • Blankenburg, F., Ruben, J., Meyer, R., Schwiemann, J., Villringer, A., 2003, “Evidence for a rostral-to-caudal somatotopic organization in human primary somatosensory cortex with mirror-reversal in areas 3b and 1”, Cerebral Cortex, 13(9): 987–93 (Scholar)
  • Block, N., 1983, “Mental pictures and cognitive science”, Philosophical Review, 92: 499–541. (Scholar)
  • Bonnier, P., 1905, “L’Aschématie”, Revue Neurologique (Paris), 13: 605–609.
  • Botvinick, M., and Cohen, J., 1998, “Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see”, Nature, 391: 756. (Scholar)
  • Brewer, B., 1995, “Bodily awareness and the self”, in J.L. Bermudez, T. Marcel, N. Eilan (eds.), The body and the self, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Brown, C. H., 2005, “Hand and arm”, in M. Haspelmath, M. Dryer, D. Gil, and B. Comrie (eds.), The world atlas of language structures, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 522–525. (Scholar)
  • Brozzoli, C., Makin, T.R., Cardinali, L., Holmes, N.P., Farnè, A., 2012, “Peripersonal Space: A Multisensory Interface for Body–Object Interactions”, in MM Murray, MT Wallace (eds.), The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes, Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press. (Scholar)
  • Campbell, J., 1994, Past, space and the self, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Cardinali, L., Frassinetti, F., Brozzoli, C., Urquizar, C., Roy, A. C., and Farnè, A., 2009, “Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema”, Current Biology, 19(12): R478–R479. (Scholar)
  • Carman, T., 1999, “The Body in Husserl and Merleau-Ponty”, Philosophical topics, 27(2): 205–226. (Scholar)
  • Carruthers, G., 2008, “Types of body representation and the sense of embodiment”, Consciousness and Cognition, 17(4): 1302–1316. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2009, “Is the body schema sufficient for the sense of embodiment? An alternative to de Vignemont's model”, Philosophical Psychology, 22(2): 123–142. (Scholar)
  • Cassam, Q., 1995, “Introspection and Bodily Self-ascription”, in J.L. Bermudez, T. Marcel, N. Eilan, (eds.), The body and the self, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1997, Self and World, New-York: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Chen, C. K., 2011, “Bodily awareness and Immunity to error through misidentification”, European Journal of Philosophy, 19(1): 21–38. (Scholar)
  • Cholewiak, R. W., and Collins, A. A., 2003, “Vibrotactile localization on the arm: Effects of place, space, and age”, Perception and Psychophysics, 65: 1058–1077. (Scholar)
  • Clark, A., 1997, Being There: Putting Brain, Body and World Together Again, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (Scholar)
  • Coburn, R.C., 1966, “Pain and Space”, The Journal of philosophy, 63(13): 381–396. (Scholar)
  • Cocchini, G., Beschin, N., Sala, S.D., 2002, “Chronic anosognosia: a case report and theoretical account”, Neuropsychologia, 40(12): 2030–8. (Scholar)
  • Cole, J. and Paillard, J., 1995, “Living without Touch and Peripheral Information about Body Position and Movement: Studies with Deafferented Subjects”, in J.L. Bermudez, T. Marcel, N. Eilan, (eds.), The body and the self, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Coliva, A., 2012, “Which ‘key to all mythologies' about the self?” in F. Recanati and S. Prosser (eds.), Immunity to error through misidentification, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 22–45. (Scholar)
  • Coslett, H.B., 1998, “Evidence for a disturbance of the body schema in neglect”, Brain and cognition, 37: 527–544. (Scholar)
  • Craig, A.D., 2003, “Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body”, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13: 500–5. (Scholar)
  • Csibra, G., 2007, “Action mirroring and action interpretation: An alternative account”, in P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti, M. Kawato (eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition. Attention and performance XXII, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Damasio, A., 1999, The Feeling of What Happens, London: William Heinemann. (Scholar)
  • Davies, M., Coltheart, M., Langdon, R., & Breen, N., 2001, “Monothematic delusions: Towards a two-factor account”, Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 8(2): 133–158. (Scholar)
  • Decety, J.,Grèzes, J.,Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M. 1997, “Brain activity during observation of action content and subject’s strategy”, Brain, 120: 1763–1777. (Scholar)
  • De Preester, H., 2007, “The deep bodily origins of the subjective perspective: models and their problems”, Consciousness and Cognition , 16(3): 604–18. (Scholar)
  • Dewall, N., MacDonald, G., Webster, G., Masten, C., Baumeister, R., Powell, C., Combs, D., Schurtz, D., Stillman, T., Tice, D., Eisenberger, N., 2010, “Tylenol reduces social pain: behavioral and neural evidence”, Psychological Science, 21(7): 931–7. (Scholar)
  • Di Pellegrino, G., Làdavas, E., Farnè, A., 1997, “Seeing where your hands are”, Nature, 388: 730. (Scholar)
  • Dijkerman, H. C., and de Haan, E. H., 2007, “Somatosensory processes subserving perception and action”, The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30: 189–201. (Scholar)
  • Dokic, J., 2003, “The sense of ownership: an analogy between sensation and action”, in J. Roessler and N. Eilan (eds.), Agency and Self-Awareness: Issues in Philosophy and Psychology, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Durgin, F.H., Evans, L., Dunphy, N., Klostermann, S., Simmons, K. 2007, “Rubber hands feel the touch of light”, Psychological Science, 18(2): 152–7. (Scholar)
  • Durgin, F.H., Baird, J.A., Greenburg, M., Russell, R., Shaughnessy, K., Waymouth, S., 2009, “Who is being deceived? The experimental demands of wearing a backpack”, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(5), 964–9. (Scholar)
  • Ehrsson, H. H., 2007, “The experimental induction of out-of-body experiences”, Science, 317(5841): 1048. (Scholar)
  • Ehrsson, H. H., Wiech, K., Weiskopf, N., Dolan, R. J., and Passingham, R. E., 2007, “Threatening a rubber hand that you feel is yours elicits a cortical anxiety response”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (U.S.A.), 104(23): 9828–9833. (Scholar)
  • Evans, G., 1982, The varieties of reference, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1985, “Molyneux's question”, in The collected papers of Gareth Evans, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Feinberg, T. E., 2009, From axons to identity: Neurological explorations of the nature of the self, New York: WW Norton. (Scholar)
  • Feinberg, T. E., & Roane, D., 2003, “Misidentification syndromes”, in T. E. Feinberg & M. Farah (eds.), Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology, New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 373–381. (Scholar)
  • Fourneret, P. and Jeannerod, M., 1998, “Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects”, Neuropsychologia, 36(11): 1133–1140. (Scholar)
  • Fulkerson, M., 2014, The First Sense: A Philosophical Study of Human Touch, Cambridge, MA: the MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Gallagher, S., 1986, “Body image and body schema: Aconceptual clarification”, Journal of Mind and Behavior, 7: 541–554. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1995, “Body schema and intentionality”, in J. L. Bermudez, A. Marcel, and N. Eilan (eds.), The body and the self, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2003, “Bodily self-awareness and object-perception”, Theoria et Historia Scientiarum: International Journal for Interdisciplinary Studies (Poland), 7(1): 53–68. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2005, How the body shapes the mind, New York: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2008, “Are minimal representations still representations?International Journal of Philosophical Studies, 16(3): 351–69. (Scholar)
  • Gallese, V., 2001, “The ‘Shared Manifold’ Hypothesis: From Mirror Neurons to Empathy”, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(5–7): 33–50. (Scholar)
  • Gallese, V., Sinigaglia, C., 2010, “The bodily self as power for action”, Neuropsychologia, 48(3): 746–55. (Scholar)
  • Gandevia, S.C., Phegan, C.M., 1999, “Perceptual distortions of the human body image produced by local anaesthesia, pain and cutaneous stimulation”, Journal of Physiology, 514: 609–16. (Scholar)
  • Goldenberg, G., 2003, “Goldsten and Gel’s case Schn.: A classic case in neuropsychology?” in C. Code, C.W. Wellesch, Y. Loenette, A.R. Lecours (eds.), Classic cases in neuropsychology, vol. 2, Hove, New York: Psychology Press, 281–299. (Scholar)
  • Goldman, A.I., 2006, Simulating minds, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2012, “A Moderate Approach to Embodied Cognitive Science”, Review of Philosophy and Psychology, Special issue, 3(1): 71-88. (Scholar)
  • Goldman, A.I. and de Vignemont, F., 2009, “Is social cognition embodied?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4): 154–9. (Scholar)
  • Goldreich, D. and Kanics, I.M., 2003, “Tactile acuity is enhanced in blindness”, Journal of Neuroscience, 23(8): 3439–45. (Scholar)
  • Gurwitsch, A., 1985, Marginal consciousness, Athens: Ohio University Press. (Scholar)
  • Hall, R. J., 2008, “If it itches, scratch!Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 86(4): 525–535. (Scholar)
  • Harcourt, E., 2008, “Wittgenstein and Bodily Self-Knowledge”, Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 77: 299–333. (Scholar)
  • Head, H., and Holmes, G., 1911, “Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions”, Brain, 34: 102–254. (Scholar)
  • Henri, M., 1965, Philosophie et Phénoménologie du corps, Paris: Presses Universitaire de France. (Scholar)
  • Hill, C. and Bennett, D. (eds.), 2014, Sensory integration and the unity of consciousness, Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Hilti, L.M. and Brugger, P., 2010, “Incarnation and animation: physical versus representational deficits of body integrity”, Experimental Brain Research, 204(3): 315–26. (Scholar)
  • Holly, W.J., 1986, “The Spatial Coordinates of Pain”, The Philosophical Quarterly, 36(144): 343–356. (Scholar)
  • Holmes, N. P., and Spence, C., 2005, “Beyond the body schema: Visual, prosthetic, and technological contributions to bodily perception and awareness”, in G. Knoblich, I. M. Thornton, M. Grosjean, and M. Shiffrar (eds.), Human body perception from the inside out, New York: Oxford University Press, 15–64. (Scholar)
  • Hopkins, R., 2011, “Re-imagining, Re-Viewing and Re-Touching”, in F. Macpherson (ed.), The senses : Classic and Contemporary Philosophical Perspectives, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Hurley, S., 1998, Consciousness in action, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Scholar)
  • Husserl, E., 1913, Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and to a Phenomenological Philosophy — Second Book: Studies in the Phenomenology of Constitution, trans. R. Rojcewicz and A. Schuwer, Dordrecht: Kluwer, 1989. (Scholar)
  • Jacob, P., 2008, “What do mirror neurons contribute to human social cognition?Mind and Language, 23(2): 190–223. (Scholar)
  • Jacob, P. and de Vignemont, F. forthcoming, “Vicarious experiences: perception, mirroring or imagination?” in J. Kiverstein (ed.), Routledge Handbook of Social Mind, London: Routledge. (Scholar)
  • Jacobson, H., 2013, “Killing the messenger: Representationalism and the painfulness of pain”, Philosophical Quarterly, 63(252): 509-519. (Scholar)
  • James, W. 1884, “What is an Emotion?Mind, 9: 188–205. (Scholar)
  • –––. 1890, The Principles of Psychology (2 vols.). New York: Henry Holt. (Scholar)
  • Kammers, M.P., de Vignemont, F., Verhagen, L., and Dijkerman, H.C., 2009, “The rubber hand illusion in action”, Neuropsychologia, 47: 204–211. (Scholar)
  • Katz, D., 1925, The world of touch, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (Scholar)
  • Kennett, S., Taylor-Clarke, M., Haggard, P., 2001, “Noninformative vision improves the spatial resolution of touch in humans”, Current Biology, 11(15): 1188–91. (Scholar)
  • Keysers, C., Wicker, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J.L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., 2004, “A Touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch”, Neuron, 42: 335–346. (Scholar)
  • Kinsbourne, M., 1995, “Awareness of One's Own Body: An Attentionnal Theory of Its Nature, Development, and Brain Basis”, in J.L. Bermudez, T. Marcel, N. Eilan, (eds.), The body and the self, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Kinsbourne, M. and Lempert, H., 1980, “Human figure representation by blind children”, Journal of General Psychology, 102: 33–7. (Scholar)
  • Klein, C., 2007, “An imperative theory of pain”, The Journal of Philosophy, 104(10): 517-532. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2015, What the body commands, Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Lackner, J.R., 1988, “Some proprioceptive influences on the perceptual representation of body shape and orientation”, Brain, 111: 281–297. (Scholar)
  • Leder, D., 1990, The absent body, Chicago: Chicago University Press. (Scholar)
  • Lenggenhager, B., Tadi, T., Metzinger, T., and Blanke, O. 2007, “Video ergo sum: Manipulating bodily self-consciousness”, Science, 317(5841): 109.6–109.9. (Scholar)
  • Legrand, D., 2006, “The bodily self. The sensori-motor roots of pre-reflexive self-consciousness”, Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 5: 89–118. (Scholar)
  • Lhermitte J., 1942, “De l’image corporelle”, Revue Neurologique, 74: 20–38. (Scholar)
  • Lipps, T., 1900, Asthetische Einfuhlung, Zeitschrift fur Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane, 22: 415–50. (Scholar)
  • Longo, M.R., Schüür, F., Kammers, M.P., Tsakiris, M., Haggard, P., 2008, “What is embodiment? A psychometric approach.” Cognition, 107: 978–998. (Scholar)
  • Longo, M.R. and Haggard, P., 2010, “An implicit body representation underlying human position sense”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (U.S.A.). 107(26): 11727–32. (Scholar)
  • Lunn, V., 1970, “Autoscopic phenomena”, Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia, 46 (Suppl 219): 118–25. (Scholar)
  • Macpherson, F., 2011, “Cross-modal experiences”, Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 111 (3): 429-468. (Scholar)
  • Majid, A., Enfield, N. J., and van Staden, M. (eds.), 2006, “Parts of the body: Cross-linguistic categorization” [Special Issue]. Language Sciences, 28: 137–360. (Scholar)
  • Makin, T.R., Holmes, N.P., Ehrsson, H.H., 2008, “On the other hand: dummy hands and peripersonal space”, Behavorial Brain Research, 191(1): 1–10. (Scholar)
  • Mancini, F., Longo, M.R., Iannetti, G.D., Haggard, P., 2011, “A supramodal representation of the body surface”, Neuropsychologia, 49(5): 1194-201.
  • Marcel, A., 2003, “The sense of agency: awareness and ownership of action”, in J. Roessler and N. Eilan (eds.), Agency and Self-Awareness: Issues in Philosophy and Psychology, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Margolis, J., 1966, “Awareness of Sensations and of the Location of Sensations”, Analysis, 27: 29–32. (Scholar)
  • Martin, M.G.F., 1992, “Sight and touch”, in T. Crane (ed.), The content of experience, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 199–201. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1993, “Sense modalities and spatial properties”, in N. Eilan, R. McCarty and B. Brewer (eds.), Spatial representations, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1995, “Bodily awareness: a sense of ownership”, in J.L. Bermudez, T. Marcel, N. Eilan, (eds.), The body and the self, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Martinez, M., 2011, “Imperative content and the painfulness of pain”, Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 10(1): 67-90. (Scholar)
  • McDowell, J., 2011, “Anscombe on bodily self-knowledge”, in Essays on Anscombe's Intention, edited by A. Ford, J. Hornsby and F. Stoutland. Cambridge MA, MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Melzack, R. and Wall, P.D., 1983, The Challenge of Pain, New York: Basic Books (Scholar)
  • Merleau-Ponty, M., 1945, Phénoménologie de la perception, Paris: Gallimard. (Scholar)
  • Miyazawa, N., Hayashi, M., Komiya, K., Akiyama, I., 2004, “Supernumerary phantom limbs associated with left hemispheric stroke: case report and review of the literature”, Neurosurgery, 54(1): 228–31. (Scholar)
  • Milner, D. and Goodale, M.A., 1995, The Visual Brain in Action, New York: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2008, “Two visual systems reviewed” Neuropsychologia, 46: 774–785. (Scholar)
  • Mishara, A.L., 2004, “The disconnection of external and internal in the conscious experience of schizophrenia: phenomenological, literary and neuroanatomical archaeologies of self”, Philosophica, 73: 87–126. (Scholar)
  • Mitchell, S. W. 1871, “Phantom limbs”, Lippincott's Magazine of Popular Literature and Science, 8: 563–569. (Scholar)
  • Morrison, J. B., and Tversky, B., 2005, “Bodies and their parts”, Memory and Cognition, 33: 696–709. (Scholar)
  • Neisser, U., 1992, “The development of consciousness and the acquisition of the self”, in F. Kessel, P.M. Cole, et D.L. Johnson (eds.), Self and consciousness: multiple perspectives, Hillsdale (NJ): Lawrence Erblaum, 1–18. (Scholar)
  • Niedenthal, P.M., 2007, “Embodying emotion”, Science, 316(5827): 1002–5. (Scholar)
  • Noë, A., 2004, Action in perception, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2010, “Vision without representation”, in N. Gangopadhyay, M. Madary, and F. Spicer (eds.), Perception, Action and Consciousness, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Noordhof, P., 2001, “In Pain”, Analysis, 61(2): 95–97. (Scholar)
  • O'Callaghan, C., 2011, “Perception and Multimodality”, in E. Margolis, R. Samuels, and S. Stich, Oxford Handbook of Philosophy and Cognitive Science, Oxford: Oxford University Press. 92-117 (Scholar)
  • O’Regan, K., 2011, Why Red Doesn't Sound Like a Bell, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • O'Regan, K., and Noë, A., 2001, “A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5): 939–101.
  • O'Shaughnessy, B., 1980, The Will, Vol. 1. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1995, “Proprioception and the body image”, in J.L. Bermudez, T. Marcel, N. Eilan, (eds.), The body and the self, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2000, Consciousness and the world, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Paillard, J., 1980, “Le corps situé et le corps identifié. Une approche psychophysiologique de la notion de schéma corporel”, Revue Medicale de la Suisse Romande, 100: 129–141. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1999, “Body schema and body image: A double dissociation in deafferented patients”, in G. N. Gantchev, S. Mori, and J.Massion (eds.), Motor control, today and tomorrow, Sofia: Professor Marius Drinov Academic Publishing House, 197–214. (Scholar)
  • Paillard, J., Michel, F., and Stelmach, G., 1983, “Localization without content. A tactile analogue of ‘blind sight’”, Archives of Neurology, 40: 548–551. (Scholar)
  • Peacocke, C., 2014, The Mirror of the World: Subjects, Consciousness, and Self-Consciousness, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Petkova, V.I., Zetterberg, H., Ehrsson, H.H., 2012, “Rubber Hands Feel Touch, but Not in Blind Individuals”, PLoS ONE, 7(4): e35912. (Scholar)
  • Prinz, J., 2004, Gut Reactions, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Proffitt, D.R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., Epstein, W. 2003, “The role of effort in perceiving distance”, Psychological Science, 14(2): 106–12. (Scholar)
  • Penfield, W., Boldrey, E., 1937, “Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation”, Brain, 60: 339–448. (Scholar)
  • Penfield, W., Rasmussen, T., 1950, The Cerebral Cortex of Man, New York: MacMillan. (Scholar)
  • Rawlence, Chris (Director), and Crichton-Miller, Emma (Producer), 1998, The man who lost his body [Film], London: Rosetta Pictures for BBC Television. (Scholar)
  • Richardson, L., 2013, Bodily Sensation and Tactile Perception. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 86(1): 134-154. (Scholar)
  • Rizzolatti, G., Scandolara, C., Matelli, M., & Gentilucci, M., 1981, “Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responses”, Behavioural Brain Research, 2(2): 147-163. (Scholar)
  • Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., 1995, “Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions”, Cognitive Brain Research, 3: 131–141. (Scholar)
  • Rizzolatti, G. and Sinigaglia, C., 2008, Mirrors in the brain: how our minds share actions, emotions, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Rochat, P., 1998, “Self perception and action in infancy”, Experimental Brain Research, 123: 102–109. (Scholar)
  • Röder, B., Rösler, F., Spence, C., 2004, “Early vision impairs tactile perception in the blind”, Current Biology, 14(2): 121–4. (Scholar)
  • Sacks, O., 1984, A Leg to Stand On, London: Picador. (Scholar)
  • Schilder, P., 1935, The image and appearance of the human body, New York: International Universities Press. (Scholar)
  • Schwenkler, J., 2013, “The objects of bodily awareness”, Philosophical studies, 162: 465-472. (Scholar)
  • Schwoebel, J., and Coslett, H. B., 2005, “Evidence for multiple, distinct representations of the human body”, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17: 543–553. (Scholar)
  • Seth, A., 2013, “Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self”, Trends in cognitive science, 17(11): 565-573. (Scholar)
  • Shoemaker, S., 1968, “Self-Reference and Self-Awareness”, The Journal of Philosophy, 65: 555–67. (Scholar)
  • –––, 1994, “The Royce lectures: Self-knowledge and ”inner sense“”, Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 54: 249–314. (Scholar)
  • Siewert, C., 2005, “Attention and sensorimotor intentionality”, in D. Woodruff Smith, and A. Thomasson (eds.), Phenomenology and philosophy of mind, Oxford: Clarendon Press (Scholar)
  • Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R., Frith, C.D., 2004, “Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain”, Science, 303: 1157–1162. (Scholar)
  • Sirigu, A., Grafman, J., Bressler, K., Sunderland, T., 1991, “Multiple representations contribute to body knowledge processing. Evidence from a case of autotopagnosia”, Brain, 114: 629–42. (Scholar)
  • Slater, M., Perez-Marcos, D., Ehrsson, H.H., Sanchez-Vives, M.V., 2008, “Towards a digital body: the virtual arm illusion”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2: 6. (Scholar)
  • Smith, A.D., 2002, The problem of perception, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Scholar)
  • Smith, A.J.T., 2009, “Acting on (Bodily) Experience”, Psyche, 15(1): 82-99. (Scholar)
  • Smith, J., 2006, “Bodily awareness, imagination, and the self”, European Journal of Philosophy, 14(1): 49-68. (Scholar)
  • Solomon, R., 1993, The Passions (2nd edition), Indianapolis: Hackett (Scholar)
  • Spence, C., Pavani, F., Driver, J., 2004, “Spatial constraints on visual-tactile cross-modal distractor congruency effects”, Cognive Affective & Behavioural Neuroscience, 4(2): 148–69. (Scholar)
  • Spence, C. and Bayne, T., 2015 “Is consciousness multisensory?” in M. Matthen (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of philosophy of perception, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • Staub, F., Bogousslavsky, J., Maeder, P., Maeder-Ingvar, M., Fornari, E., Ghika, J., Vingerhoets, F., Assal, G., 2006, “Intentional motor phantom limb syndrome”, Neurology, 67(12): 2140–6. (Scholar)
  • Strack, F., Martin, L.,Stepper, S., 1988, “Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54: 768–777. (Scholar)
  • Thelen, E. and Smith, L., 1994, A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (Scholar)
  • Thompson, E., 2005, “Sensorimotor subjectivity and the enactive approach to experience”, Phenomenology and the cognitive sciences, 4(4): 407–427. (Scholar)
  • Tipper, S.P., Phillips, N., Dancer, C., Lloyd, D., Howard, L.A., McGlone, F., 2001, “Vision influences tactile perception at body sites that cannot be viewed directly”, Experimental Brain Research, 139(2): 160–7. (Scholar)
  • Tsakiris, M., 2010, My body in the brain: a neurocognitive model of body-ownership. Neuropsychologia, 48(3): 703-12. (Scholar)
  • Turvey, M. and Carello, C., 1995, “Some dynamical themes in perception and action”, in R. Port and T. Van Gelder (eds.), Mind as Motion, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
  • Tye, M., 1995, Ten problems of consciousness: A representational theory of the phenomenal mind, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (Scholar)
  • –––, 2002, “On the Location of a Pain”, Analysis, 62(2): 150–153. (Scholar)
  • Vallar, G. and Ronchi, R., 2006, “Anosognosia for motor and sensory deficits after unilateral brain damage: a review”, Restorative Neurology and Neurosciences, 24(4–6): 247–57. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2009, “Somatoparaphrenia: A body delusion. A review of the neuropsychological literature”, Experimental Brain Research, 192(3): 533–551. (Scholar)
  • van Gelder, T., 1995, “What Might Cognition Be, If Not Computation?The Journal of Philosophy, XCII: 345–381 (Scholar)
  • Vesey, G. N. A., 1964, “Bodily Sensations”, Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 42: 232–247. (Scholar)
  • de Vignemont, F., 2007, “Habeas Corpus: the sense of ownership of one’s own body”, Mind and Language, 22(4): 447–449. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2010, “Body schema and body image: pros and cons”, Neuropsychologia, 48(3): 669–80. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2011, “A mosquito bite against the enactive view to bodily experiences”, The Journal of Philosophy, CVIII: 188-204.
  • –––, 2012, “Bodily immunity to error”, in F. Recanati and S. Prosser (eds.), Immunity to error through misidentification, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 224-246. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2014a, “A multimodal conception of bodily awareness”, Mind, 123(492): 989-1020.
  • –––, 2014b, “Acting for bodily awareness”, in Routledge handbook of embodied cognition, L. Shapiro (ed.), London: Routledge, 287-295. (Scholar)
  • –––, 2014c, “Shared body representations and the ”Whose“ system”, Neuropsychologia, 55: 128-136. (Scholar)
  • –––, forthcoming, Mind the body, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • de Vignemont, F., Tsakiris, M., & Haggard, P., 2005, “Body mereology”, in G. Knoblich, I.M. Thornton, M. Grosjean and M. Shiffrar (eds.), Human body perception from the inside out, New York: Oxford University Press, pp 147–1170. (Scholar)
  • de Vignemont, F. and Singer, T., 2006, “The empathic brain –how, when and why”, Trends in Cognive Science, 10(10): 435–441. (Scholar)
  • de Vignemont, F., Majid, A., Jolla, C., and Haggard, P. 2008, “Segmenting the body into parts, evidence from biases in tactile perception”, Quaterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3: 1–13. (Scholar)
  • de Vignemont, F. &/ Massin, O., 2015, “Touch”, in Oxford Handbook of philosophy of perception, M. Matthen (ed.), Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
  • de Vignemont, F. and Iannetti, G., 2015, “How many peripersonal spaces?” Neuropsychologia, 70: 327-34 (Scholar)
  • Vuilleumier, P., 2004, “Anosognosia: the neurology of beliefs and uncertainties”, Cortex, 40(1): 9–17. (Scholar)
  • Welch, R. B. and Warren, D. H., 1980, “Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy”, Psychological Bulletin, 88: 638–667. (Scholar)
  • Wicker B, Keysers C, Plailly J, Royet JP, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G. 2003, “Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust”, Neuron, 40(3): 655–64. (Scholar)
  • Wong, H.Y., 2009, “On the Necessity of Bodily Awareness for Bodily Action”, Psyche, 15(1): 31-48. (Scholar)
  • –––. forthcoming, “On the Significance of Bodily Awareness for Action”, Philosophical Quarterly. (Scholar)
  • Yamamoto, S. and Kitazawa, S., 2001, “Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing”, Nature Neuroscience, 4(7): 759–65. (Scholar)
  • Yoshimura, A., Matsugi, A., Esaki, Y., Nakagaki, K., Hiraoka, K. 2010, “Blind humans rely on muscle sense more than normally sighted humans for guiding goal-directed movement”, Neuroscience Letter, 471(3): 171–4. (Scholar)
  • Zahavi, D. and Parnas, J., 1998, “Phenomenal Consciousness and Self-awareness. A Phenomenological Critique of Representational Theory”, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5(5–6): 687–705. (Scholar)

Generated Sun Sep 16 06:08:50 2018