Linked bibliography for the SEP article "The Psychology of Normative Cognition" by Daniel Kelly and Stephen Setman
This is an automatically generated and experimental page
If everything goes well, this page should display the bibliography of the aforementioned article as it appears in the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, but with links added to PhilPapers records and Google Scholar for your convenience. Some bibliographies are not going to be represented correctly or fully up to date. In general, bibliographies of recent works are going to be much better linked than bibliographies of primary literature and older works. Entries with PhilPapers records have links on their titles. A green link indicates that the item is available online at least partially.
This experiment has been authorized by the editors of the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. The original article and bibliography can be found here.
- Andrews, Kristin, 2017, “Pluralistic Folk Psychology in Humans and Other Ape”, The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of the Social Mind , Julian Kiverstein (ed.), New York: Routledge Press,
117–138. (Scholar)
- –––, 2020, “Naïve Normativity: The Social Foundation of Moral Cognition”, Journal of the American Philosophical Association, 6(1): 36–56. doi:10.1017/apa.2019.30 (Scholar)
- Andrews, Kristin and Susana Monsó, forthcoming, “Animal Moral Psychologies”, in Vargas and Doris forthcoming. (Scholar)
- Ariew, André, 2003, “Ernst Mayr’s ‘Ultimate/Proximate’ Distinction Reconsidered and Reconstructed”, Biology & Philosophy, 18(4): 553–565. doi:10.1023/a:1025565119032 (Scholar)
- Ayala‐López, Saray, 2018, “A Structural Explanation of Injustice in Conversations: It’s about Norms”, Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 99(4): 726–748. doi:10.1111/papq.12244 (Scholar)
- Ayars, Alisabeth and Shaun Nichols, 2017, “Moral Empiricism and the Bias for Act-Based Rules”, Cognition, 167: 11–24. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2017.01.007 (Scholar)
- –––, 2020, “Rational Learners and Metaethics: Universalism, Relativism, and Evidence from Consensus”, Mind & Language, 35(1): 67–89. doi:10.1111/mila.12232 (Scholar)
- Barkow, Jerome H., Leda Cosmides, and John Tooby, 1992, The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, New York: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
- Barrett, H. Clark, Alexander Bolyanatz, Alyssa N. Crittenden, Daniel M. T. Fessler, Simon Fitzpatrick, Michael Gurven, Joseph Henrich, Martin Kanovsky, Geoff Kushnick, Anne Pisor, Brooke A. Scelza, Stephen Stich, Chris von Rueden, Wanying Zhao, and Stephen Laurence, 2016, “Small-Scale Societies Exhibit Fundamental Variation in the Role of Intentions in Moral Judgment”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(17): 4688–4693. doi:10.1073/pnas.1522070113 (Scholar)
- Baumgartner, Thomas, Urs Fischbacher, Anja Feierabend, Kai Lutz,
and Ernst Fehr, 2009, “The Neural Circuitry of a Broken
Promise”, Neuron, 64(5): 756–770.
doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.017 (Scholar)
- Bednar, Jenna, Aaron Bramson, Andrea Jones-Rooy, and Scott Page,
2010, “Emergent Cultural Signatures and Persistent Diversity: A
Model of Conformity and Consistency”, Rationality and
Society, 22(4): 407–444. doi:10.1177/1043463110374501 (Scholar)
- Beeghly, Erin and Jules Holroyd, 2020, “Bias in Context: An Introduction to the Symposium”, Journal of Applied Philosophy, 37(2): 163–168. doi:10.1111/japp.12424 (Scholar)
- Beller, Sieghard, 2010, “Deontic Reasoning Reviewed:
Psychological Questions, Empirical Findings, and Current
Theories”, Cognitive Processing, 11(2): 123–132.
doi:10.1007/s10339-009-0265-z (Scholar)
- Berniūnas, Renatas, Vilius Dranseika, and Paulo Sousa, 2016,
“Are There Different Moral Domains? Evidence from Mongolia:
Moral Domains”, Asian Journal of Social Psychology,
19(3): 275–282. doi:10.1111/ajsp.12133 (Scholar)
- Berniūnas, Renatas, Vytis Silius, and Vilius Dranseika, 2020,
“Beyond the Moral Domain: The Normative Sense Among the
Chinese”, Psichologija, 60: 86–105.
doi:10.15388/psichol.2019.11 (Scholar)
- Bicchieri, Cristina, 2006, The Grammar of Society: The Nature and Dynamics of Social Norms, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/cbo9780511616037 (Scholar)
- –––, 2016, Norms in the Wild: How to Diagnose, Measure, and Change Social Norms, Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190622046.001.0001 (Scholar)
- Bicchieri, Cristina, Ryan Muldoon and Alessandro Sontuoso, 2018,
“Social Norms”, in The Stanford Encyclopedia of
Philosophy (Winter 2018 edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.), URL =
< target="other">https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2018/entries/social-norms/>. (Scholar)
- Bone, Jonathan, Antonio S. Silva, and Nichola J. Raihani, 2014,
“Defectors, Not Norm Violators, Are Punished by
Third-Parties”, Biology Letters, 10(7): 20140388.
doi:10.1098/rsbl.2014.0388 (Scholar)
- Bone, Jonathan E., Katherine McAuliffe, and Nichola J. Raihani,
2016, “Exploring the Motivations for Punishment: Framing and
Country-Level Effects”, PLOS ONE, 11(8): e0159769.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0159769 (Scholar)
- Boyd, Robert, 2017, A Different Kind of Animal: How Culture
Transformed Our Species, (The University Center for Human Values
Series), Princeton: Princeton University Press. (Scholar)
- Boyd, Robert and Peter J. Richerson, 1992, “Punishment
Allows the Evolution of Cooperation (or Anything Else) in Sizable
Groups”, Ethology and Sociobiology, 13(3):
171–195. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(92)90032-y (Scholar)
- –––, 2001, “Norms and Bounded Rationality”, in Gigerenzer and Selten 2001: 181–296. (Scholar)
- –––, 2005a, The Origin and Evolution of
Cultures, New York: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
- –––, 2005b, “Solving the Puzzle of Human Cooperation”, in Evolution and Culture, Stephen C. Levinson and Pierre Jaisson, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 105–132. (Scholar)
- –––, 2008, “Gene-Culture Coevolution and the Evolution of Social Institutions”, In: Better than Conscious? Decision Making, the Human Mind, and Implications for Institutions, Christoph Engel and Wolf Singer (eds), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 305–324. (Scholar)
- Boyd, Richard, Peter J. Richerson, and Joseph Henrich, 2011,
“The Cultural Niche: Why Social Learning Is Essential for Human
Adaptation”, Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 108(Supplement 2): 10918–10925.
doi:10.1073/pnas.1100290108 (Scholar)
- Brennan, Geoffrey, Lina Eriksson, Robert E. Goodin, and Nicholas Southwood, 2013, Explaining Norms, Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199654680.001.0001 (Scholar)
- Brown, Donald, 1991, Human Universals, New York:
McGraw-Hill. (Scholar)
- Brownstein, Michael, 2015 [2019], “Implicit Bias”, in
The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2019 edition),
Edward N. Zalta (ed.), URL =
<https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2019/entries/implicit-bias/>. (Scholar)
- Brownstein, Michael and Daniel Kelly, 2019, “Review of The Evolution of Moral Progress: A Biocultural Theory by Allen Powell and James Buchanan”, The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science: Review of Books. [Brownstein and Kelly 2019 available online] (Scholar)
- Brownstein, Michael and Jennifer Saul (eds.), 2016, Implicit Bias and Philosophy, 2 volumes, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Buchanan, Allen and Russell Powell, 2018, The Evolution of Moral Progress: A Biocultural Account, Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oso/9780190868413.001.0001 (Scholar)
- Burnham, Terence C. and Dominic D. P. Johnson, 2005, “The Biological and Evolutionary Logic of Human Cooperation”, Analyse & Kritik, 27(1): 113–135. doi:10.1515/auk-2005-0107 (Scholar)
- Campbell, Richmond and Victor Kumar, 2012, “Moral Reasoning on the Ground”, Ethics, 122(2): 273–312. doi:10.1086/663980 (Scholar)
- Carruthers, Peter, 2006, The Architecture of the Mind, Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199207077.001.0001 (Scholar)
- –––, 2011, The Opacity of Mind: An Integrative Theory of Self-Knowledge, Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199596195.001.0001 (Scholar)
- Cheng, Joey T., Jessica L. Tracy, Tom Foulsham, Alan Kingstone,
and Joseph Henrich, 2013, “Two Ways to the Top: Evidence That
Dominance and Prestige Are Distinct yet Viable Avenues to Social Rank
and Influence”, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 104(1): 103–125. doi:10.1037/a0030398 (Scholar)
- Chernyak, Nadia and David M. Sobel, 2016, “‘But He
Didn’t Mean to Do It’: Preschoolers Correct Punishments
Imposed on Accidental Transgressors”, Cognitive
Development, 39: 13–20.
doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.03.002 (Scholar)
- Chudek, Maciej and Joseph Henrich, 2011, “Culture–Gene
Coevolution, Norm-Psychology and the Emergence of Human
Prosociality”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(5):
218–226. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.03.003 (Scholar)
- Chudek, Maciek, Wanying Zhao, and Joseph Henrich, 2013, “Culture-Gene Coevolution, Large-Scale Cooperation, and the Shaping of Human Social Psychology”, in Sterelny et al. 2013: 425–458. (Scholar)
- Clay, Zanna and Claudio Tennie, 2018, “Is Overimitation a
Uniquely Human Phenomenon? Insights From Human Children as Compared to
Bonobos”, Child Development, 89(5): 1535–1544.
doi:10.1111/cdev.12857 (Scholar)
- Conte, Rosaria, Giulia Andrighetto, and Marco Campennì
(eds.), 2013, Minding Norms: Mechanisms and Dynamics of Social
Order in Agent Societies, (Oxford Series on Cognitive Models and
Architectures), Oxford: Oxford University Press.
doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199812677.001.0001 (Scholar)
- Cosmides, Leda and John Tooby, 1992, “Cognitive Adaptations
for Social Exchange”, in Barkow, Cosmides, and Tooby 1992:
151–192. (Scholar)
- Cummins, Denise Dellarosa, 1996, “Evidence for the Innateness of Deontic Reasoning”, Mind & Language, 11(2): 160–190. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.1996.tb00039.x (Scholar)
- Cummins, Robert, 2000, “‘How does it work?’ vs. ‘What are the laws?’: Two conceptions of psychological explanation”, in Explanation and Cognition, Frank C. Keil and Robert A. Wilson (Eds), Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 117–145. (Scholar)
- Curtin, Cameron M., H. Clark Barrett, Alexander Bolyanatz, Alyssa
Crittenden, Daniel M.T. Fessler, Simon Fitzpatrick, Michael Gurven,
Martin Kanovsky, Geoff Kushnick, Stephen Laurence, Anne Pisor, Brooke
Scelza, Stephen Stich, Chris von Rueden, and Joseph Henrich,
forthcoming, “Kinship Intensity and the Use of Mental States in
Moral Judgment across Societies”, Evolution and Human
Behavior, first online: 12 July 2020.
doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.07.002 (Scholar)
- Cushman, Fiery, Liane Young, and Joshua D. Greene, 2010, “Multi-System Moral Psychology”, in Doris et al. 2010: 47–71. (Scholar)
- Davis, Taylor, Erin P. Hennes, and Leigh Raymond, 2018,
“Cultural Evolution of Normative Motivations for Sustainable
Behaviour”, Nature Sustainability, 1(5): 218–224.
doi:10.1038/s41893-018-0061-9 (Scholar)
- Davis, Taylor and Daniel Kelly, 2018, “Norms, Not Moral Norms: The Boundaries of Morality Do Not Matter”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41: e101. Commentary on Stanford 2018. doi:10.1017/s0140525x18000067 (Scholar)
- Davidson, Lacey J. and Daniel Kelly, 2020, “Minding the Gap: Bias, Soft Structures, and the Double Life of Social Norms”, Journal of Applied Philosophy, 37(2): 190–210. doi:10.1111/japp.12351 (Scholar)
- Dean, Lewis G., Gill L. Vale, Kevin N. Laland, Emma Flynn, and
Rachel L. Kendal, 2014, “Human Cumulative Culture: A Comparative
Perspective: Human Cumulative Culture”, Biological
Reviews, 89(2): 284–301. doi:10.1111/brv.12053 (Scholar)
- Dennett, Daniel, 1978, Brainstorms, Montgomery, VT: Bradford Books. (Scholar)
- Devine, Patricia G., Patrick S. Forscher, Anthony J. Austin, and
William T.L. Cox, 2012, “Long-Term Reduction in Implicit Race
Bias: A Prejudice Habit-Breaking Intervention”, Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6): 1267–1278.
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.06.003 (Scholar)
- Doris, John M. and The Moral Psychology Research Group, 2010, The Moral Psychology Handbook, New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199582143.001.0001 (Scholar)
- Doris, John M. and Alexandra Plakias, 2008, “How to Argue
About Disagreement: Evaluative Diversity and Moral Realism”, in
Sinnott-Armstrong 2008: 303–332. (Scholar)
- Dwyer, Susan, Bryce Huebner, and Marc D. Hauser, 2010, “The Linguistic Analogy: Motivations, Results, and Speculations”, Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(3): 486–510. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01064.x (Scholar)
- Elster, Jon, 1991, “Rationality and Social Norms”,
European Journal of Sociology, 32(1): 109–129.
doi:10.1017/s0003975600006172 (Scholar)
- Fehr, Ernst and Colin F. Camerer, 2007, “Social
Neuroeconomics: The Neural Circuitry of Social Preferences”,
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(10): 419–427.
doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.002 (Scholar)
- Fehr, Ernst and Armin Falk, 2002, “Psychological Foundations
of Incentives”, European Economic Review,
46(4–5): 687–724. doi:10.1016/s0014-2921(01)00208-2 (Scholar)
- Fehr, Ernst and Urs Fischbacher, 2004, “Third-Party
Punishment and Social Norms”, Evolution and Human
Behavior, 25(2): 63–87.
doi:10.1016/s1090-5138(04)00005-4 (Scholar)
- Fehr, Ernst and Simon Gächter, 2002, “Altruistic Punishment in Humans”, Nature, 415(6868): 137–140. doi:10.1038/415137a (Scholar)
- Fessler, Daniel M. T. and Edouard Machery, 2012, “Culture and Cognition”, In The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Cognitive Science, Eric Margolis, Richard Samuels, and Stephen P. Stich (eds), Oxford: Oxford University Press, pages 503–527. (Scholar)
- Fitzpatrick, Simon, forthcoming, “Chimpanzee Normativity: Evidence and Objections”, Biology & Philosophy, 35(45), first online: 5 August 2020. doi:10.1007/s10539-020-09763-1 (Scholar)
- Fodor, Jerry A., 1968, Psychological Explanation: An Introduction to the Philosophy of Psychology, New York: Random House. (Scholar)
- –––, 1983, Modularity of Mind, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
- Fogarty, Laurel and Nicole Creanza, 2017, “The Niche
Construction of Cultural Complexity: Interactions between Innovations,
Population Size and the Environment”, Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
372(1735): 20160428. doi:10.1098/rstb.2016.0428 (Scholar)
- Frankish, Keith, 2010, “Dual-Process and Dual-System Theories of Reasoning: Dual-Process and Dual-System Theories of Reasoning”, Philosophy Compass, 5(10): 914–926. doi:10.1111/j.1747-9991.2010.00330.x (Scholar)
- Gaus, Gerald, 2016, “The Open Society as a Rule-Based Order”, Erasmus Journal for Philosophy and Economics, 9(2): 1–13. doi:10.23941/ejpe.v9i2.225 (Scholar)
- Gaus, Gerald and Shaun Nichols, 2017, “Moral Learning in the Open Society: The Theory and Practice of Natural Liberty”, Social Philosophy and Policy, 34(1): 79–101. doi:10.1017/s0265052517000048 (Scholar)
- Gavrilets, Sergey and Peter J. Richerson, 2017, “Collective Action and the Evolution of Social Norm Internalization”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(23): 6068–6073. doi:10.1073/pnas.1703857114 (Scholar)
- Gelfand, Michele J., 2018, Rule Makers, Rule Breakers: How
Tight and Loose Cultures Wire Our World, New York: Scribner. (Scholar)
- Gelfand, Michele J., Jesse R. Harrington, and Joshua Conrad
Jackson, 2017, “The Strength of Social Norms Across Human
Groups”, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(5):
800–809. doi:10.1177/1745691617708631 (Scholar)
- Gelfand, Michele J., Lisa H. Nishii, and Jana L. Raver, 2006,
“On the Nature and Importance of Cultural
Tightness-Looseness”, Journal of Applied Psychology,
91(6): 1225–1244. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.6.1225 (Scholar)
- Gelfand, Michele, Jana Raver, Lisa Hisae Nishii, Lisa Leslie,
Janetta Lun, Beng Chong Lim, Lili Duan, Assaf Almaliach, Soon Ang,
Jakobina Arnadottir, et al., 2011, “Differences between Tight
and Loose Cultures: A 33-Nation Study”, Science,
332(6033): 1100–1104. doi:10.1126/science.1197754 (Scholar)
- Gigerenzer, Gerd and Reinhard Selten (eds.), 2001, Bounded
Rationality: The Adaptive Toolbox, Cambridge, MA: The MIT
Press. (Scholar)
- Gigerenzer, Gerd, Peter M. Todd, and the ABC Research Group, 2000,
Simple Heuristics that Make Us Smart, New York: Oxford
University Press. (Scholar)
- Gintis, Herbert, Samuel Bowles, Robert Boyd, and Ernst Fehr
(eds.), 2005, Moral Sentiments and Material Interests: The
Foundations of Cooperation in Economic Life, (Economic Learning
and Social Evolution 6), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
- Göckeritz, Susanne, Marco F.H. Schmidt, and Michael
Tomasello, 2014, “Young Children’s Creation and
Transmission of Social Norms”, Cognitive Development,
30: 81–95. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.01.003 (Scholar)
- Gowlett, John A.J., 2006, “The Early Settlement of Northern
Europe: Fire History in the Context of Climate Change and the Social
Brain”, Comptes Rendus Palevol, 5(1–2):
299–310. doi:10.1016/j.crpv.2005.10.008 (Scholar)
- Graham, Jesse, Jonathan Haidt, Sena Koleva, Matt Motyl, Ravi Iyer,
Sean P. Wojcik, and Peter H. Ditto, 2013, “Moral Foundations
Theory”, in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,
San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 47:55–130.
doi:10.1016/b978-0-12-407236-7.00002-4 (Scholar)
- Griffiths, Paul E., 2007, “Ethology, Sociobiology, and Evolutionary Psychology”, in A Companion to the Philosophy of Biology, Sarkar Sahotra and Anya Plutynski (eds.), Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 393–414. doi:10.1002/9780470696590.ch21 (Scholar)
- Hagen, Edward H. and Peter Hammerstein, 2006, “Game Theory
and Human Evolution: A Critique of Some Recent Interpretations of
Experimental Games”, Theoretical Population Biology,
69(3): 339–348. doi:10.1016/j.tpb.2005.09.005 (Scholar)
- Hagger, Martin S., Panagiotis Rentzelas, and Nikos L. D.
Chatzisarantis, 2014, “Effects of Individualist and Collectivist
Group Norms and Choice on Intrinsic Motivation”, Motivation
and Emotion, 38(2): 215–223.
doi:10.1007/s11031-013-9373-2 (Scholar)
- Haidt, Jonathan, 2001, “The Emotional Dog and Its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment”, Psychological Review, 108(4): 814–834. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.108.4.814 (Scholar)
- –––, 2012, The Righteous Mind: Why Good People Are Divided by Politics and Religion, New York: Pantheon Books. (Scholar)
- Haslanger, Sally, 2015, “Distinguished Lecture: Social Structure, Narrative and Explanation”, Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 45(1): 1–15. doi:10.1080/00455091.2015.1019176 (Scholar)
- Haun, Daniel B. M., Yvonne Rekers, and Michael Tomasello, 2014,
“Children Conform to the Behavior of Peers; Other Great Apes
Stick With What They Know”, Psychological Science,
25(12): 2160–2167. doi:10.1177/0956797614553235 (Scholar)
- Hauser, Marc D., Liane Young, and Fiery Cushman, 2008,
“Reviving Rawls’ linguistic analogy”, in
Sinnott-Armstrong 2008: 107–144. (Scholar)
- Hawkins, Robert X.D., Noah D. Goodman, and Robert L. Goldstone,
2019, “The Emergence of Social Norms and Conventions”,
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(2): 158–169.
doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.11.003 (Scholar)
- Heath, Joseph, 2017, “Morality, Convention and Conventional Morality”, Philosophical Explorations, 20(3): 276–293. doi:10.1080/13869795.2017.1362030 (Scholar)
- Henrich, Joseph, 2015, The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Scholar)
- –––, forthcoming, The WEIRDest People in the
World: How the West Became Psychologically Peculiar and Particularly
Prosperous, New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. (Scholar)
- Henrich, Joseph, Robert Boyd, Samuel Bowles, Colin Camerer, Ernst
Fehr, Herbert Gintis, and Richard McElreath, 2001, “In Search of
Homo Economicus: Behavioral Experiments in 15 Small-Scale
Societies”, American Economic Review, 91(2):
73–78. doi:10.1257/aer.91.2.73 (Scholar)
- Henrich, Joseph, Robert Boyd, Samuel Bowles, Colin Camerer, Ernst Fehr, and Herbert Gintis (eds), 2004, Foundations of Human Sociality: Economic Experiments and Ethnographic Evidence from Fifteen Small-Scale Societies, Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/0199262055.001.0001 (Scholar)
- Henrich, Joseph, Robert Boyd, Samuel Bowles, Colin Camerer, Ernst Fehr, Herbert Gintis, Richard McElreath, Michael Alvard, Abigail Barr, Jean Ensminger, Natalie Smith Henrich, Kim Hill, Francisco Gil-White, Michael Gurven, Frank W. Marlowe, John Q. Patton, and David Tracer, 2005, “‘Economic Man’ in Cross-Cultural Perspective: Behavioral Experiments in 15 Small-Scale Societies”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(6): 795–815. doi:10.1017/s0140525x05000142 (Scholar)
- Henrich, Joseph and Francisco J. Gil-White, 2001, “The
Evolution of Prestige: Freely Conferred Deference as a Mechanism for
Enhancing the Benefits of Cultural Transmission”, Evolution
and Human Behavior, 22(3): 165–196.
doi:10.1016/s1090-5138(00)00071-4 (Scholar)
- Henrich, Joseph, Steven J. Heine, and Ara Norenzayan, 2010, “The Weirdest People in the World?”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3): 61–83. doi:10.1017/s0140525x0999152x (Scholar)
- Henrich, Joseph and Richard McElreath, 2003, “The Evolution
of Cultural Evolution”, Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues,
News, and Reviews, 12(3): 123–135.
doi:10.1002/evan.10110 (Scholar)
- Henrich, Joseph, Richard McElreath, Abigail Barr, Jean Ensminger,
Clark Barrett, Alexander Bolyanatz, Juan Camilo Cardenas, Michael
Gurven, Edwins Gwako, Natalie Henrich, Carolyn Lesorogol, Frank
Marlowe, David Tracer, and John Ziker, 2006, “Costly Punishment
Across Human Societies”, Science, 312(5781):
1767–1770. doi:10.1126/science.1127333 (Scholar)
- Henrich, Natalie and Joseph Henrich, 2007, Why Humans
Cooperate: A Cultural and Evolutionary Explanation, Oxford:
Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
- Herrmann, Benedikt, Christian Thöni, and Simon Gächter,
2008, “Antisocial Punishment Across Societies”,
Science, 319(5868): 1362–1367.
doi:10.1126/science.1153808 (Scholar)
- Heyes, Cecilia M., 2018, Cognitive Gadgets: The Cultural
Evolution of Thinking, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press. (Scholar)
- Hoehl, Stefanie, Stefanie Keupp, Hanna Schleihauf, Nicola
McGuigan, David Buttelmann, and Andrew Whiten, 2019,
“‘Over-Imitation’: A Review and Appraisal of a
Decade of Research”, Developmental Review, 51:
90–108. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2018.12.002 (Scholar)
- Hofstede, Geert, 1980, Culture’s Consequences:
International Differences in Work Related Values, Beverly Hills,
CA: SAGE. (Scholar)
- –––, 2001, Culture’s Consequences:
Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions, and Organizations Across
Nations, Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. (Scholar)
- House, Bailey R., Patricia Kanngiesser, H. Clark Barrett, Tanya
Broesch, Senay Cebioglu, Alyssa N. Crittenden, Alejandro Erut, Sheina
Lew-Levy, Carla Sebastian-Enesco, Andrew Marcus Smith, Süheyla
Yilmaz, and Joan B. Silk, 2020, “Universal Norm Psychology Leads
to Societal Diversity in Prosocial Behaviour and Development”,
Nature Human Behaviour, 4(1): 36–44.
doi:10.1038/s41562-019-0734-z (Scholar)
- House, Bailey R., Joan B. Silk, Joseph Henrich, H. Clark Barrett, Brooke A. Scelza, Adam H. Boyette, Barry S. Hewlett, Richard McElreath, and Stephen Laurence, 2013, “Ontogeny of Prosocial Behavior across Diverse Societies”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(36): 14586–14591. doi:10.1073/pnas.1221217110 (Scholar)
- Inglehart, Ronald, 1997, Modernization and Postmodernization:
Cultural, Economic, and Political Change in 43 Societies,
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Scholar)
- Jensen, Keith, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello, 2007,
“Chimpanzees Are Rational Maximizers in an Ultimatum
Game”, Science, 318(5847): 107–109.
doi:10.1126/science.1145850 (Scholar)
- Josephs, Marina, Tamar Kushnir, Maria Gräfenhain, and Hannes
Rakoczy, 2016, “Children Protest Moral and Conventional
Violations More When They Believe Actions Are Freely Chosen”,
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141: 247–255.
doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.002 (Scholar)
- Joyce, Richard, 2007, The Evolution of Morality, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
- Kahneman, Daniel, 2011, Thinking Fast and Slow, New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. (Scholar)
- Kelly, Daniel R., 2011, Yuck! The Nature and Moral Significance of Disgust, Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. (Scholar)
- –––, 2020, “Internalized Norms and Intrinsic Motivation: Are Normative Motivations Psychologically Primitive?”, Emotion Review, June: 36-45. (Scholar)
- –––, forthcoming, “Two Ways to Adopt a
Norm: The (Moral?) Psychology of Avowal and Internalization”, in
Vargas and Doris forthcoming. (Scholar)
- Kelly, Daniel and Taylor Davis, 2018, “Social Norms and Human Normative Psychology”, Social Philosophy and Policy, 35(1): 54–76. doi:10.1017/s0265052518000122 (Scholar)
- Kelly, Daniel R. and Stephen Stich, 2007, “Two Theories of the Cognitive Architecture Underlying Morality”, in The Innate Mind, Volume 3: Foundations and Future Horizons, Peter Carruthers, Stephen Laurence, and Stephen Stich (eds), New York: Oxford University Press, 348–366. (Scholar)
- Kelly, Daniel, Stephen Stich, Kevin J. Haley, Serena J. Eng, and Daniel M. T. Fessler, 2007, “Harm, Affect, and the Moral/Conventional Distinction”, Mind & Language, 22(2): 117–131. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.2007.00302.x (Scholar)
- Kenward, Ben, 2012, “Over-Imitating Preschoolers Believe
Unnecessary Actions Are Normative and Enforce Their Performance by a
Third Party”, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
112(2): 195–207. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.02.006 (Scholar)
- Kenward, Ben, Markus Karlsson, and Joanna Persson, 2011,
“Over-Imitation Is Better Explained by Norm Learning than by
Distorted Causal Learning”, Proceedings of the Royal Society
B: Biological Sciences, 278(1709): 1239–1246.
doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1399 (Scholar)
- Keupp, Stefanie, Tanya Behne, and Hannes Rakoczy, 2013, “Why
Do Children Overimitate? Normativity Is Crucial”, Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2): 392–406.
doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.002 (Scholar)
- Kitcher, Philip, 2011, The Ethical Project, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Scholar)
- Kline, Michelle A. and Robert Boyd, 2010, “Population Size Predicts Technological Complexity in Oceania”, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1693): 2559–2564. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.0452 (Scholar)
- Kling, Arnold, 2016, “Cultural Intelligence”,
National Affairs, 27: 150–163. (Scholar)
- Krasnow, Max M., Andrew W. Delton, Leda Cosmides, and John Tooby,
2015, “Group Cooperation without Group Selection: Modest
Punishment Can Recruit Much Cooperation”, PLOS ONE,
10(4): e0124561. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0124561 (Scholar)
- Kumar, Victor, 2015, “Moral Judgment as a Natural Kind”, Philosophical Studies, 172(11): 2887–2910. doi:10.1007/s11098-015-0448-7 (Scholar)
- Laland, Kevin N., 2017, Darwin’s Unfinished Symphony:
How Culture Made the Human Mind, Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press. (Scholar)
- Laland, Kevin N., John Odling-Smee, and M. W. Feldman, 2001,
“Cultural Niche Construction and Human Evolution: Niche
Construction and Human Evolution”, Journal of Evolutionary
Biology, 14(1): 22–33.
doi:10.1046/j.1420-9101.2001.00262.x (Scholar)
- Laland, Kevin N., John Odling-Smee, and Sean Myles, 2010,
“How Culture Shaped the Human Genome: Bringing Genetics and the
Human Sciences Together”, Nature Reviews Genetics,
11(2): 137–148. doi:10.1038/nrg2734 (Scholar)
- Lai, Calvin K., Maddalena Marini, Steven A. Lehr, Carlo Cerruti,
Jiyun-Elizabeth L. Shin, Jennifer A. Joy-Gaba, Arnold K. Ho, Bethany
A. Teachman, Sean P. Wojcik, Spassena P. Koleva, et al., 2014,
“Reducing Implicit Racial Preferences: I. A Comparative
Investigation of 17 Interventions”, Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 143(4): 1765–1785.
doi:10.1037/a0036260 (Scholar)
- Lamm, Ehud, 2014, “Forever United: the Coevolution of Language and Normativity”, in The Social Origins of Language: Early Society, Communication and Polymodality, Daniel Dor, Chris Knight and Jerome Lewis (eds), Oxford: Oxford University Press, 267–283. (Scholar)
- Lesorogol, Carolyn K., 2007, “Bringing Norms In: The Role of Context in Experimental Dictator Games”, Current Anthropology, 48(6): 920–926. (Scholar)
- Laurence, Stephen and Eric Margolis, 2001, “The Poverty of the Stimulus Argument”, The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 52(2): 217–276. doi:10.1093/bjps/52.2.217 (Scholar)
- Levin, Janet, 2004 [2018], “Functionalism”, in The
Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2018 edition), Edward
N. Zalta (ed.), URL =
<https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2018/entries/functionalism/>. (Scholar)
- Lindström, Björn, Simon Jangard, Ida Selbing, and
Andreas Olsson, 2018, “The Role of a ‘Common Is
Moral’ Heuristic in the Stability and Change of Moral
Norms”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
147(2): 228–242. doi:10.1037/xge0000365 (Scholar)
- Loeb, D., 1998, “Moral Realism and the Argument from Disagreement”, Philosophical Studies, 90(3): 281–303. doi:10.1023/a:1004267726440 (Scholar)
- Lycan, William G., 1990, “The Continuity of Levels of Nature”, in Lycan (ed). Mind and Cognition: A Reader, Oxford: Blackwell Publishers, 77–96. (Scholar)
- Lyons, Derek E., Andrew G. Young, and Frank C. Keil, 2007,
“The Hidden Structure of Overimitation”, Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(50): 19751–19756.
doi:10.1073/pnas.0704452104 (Scholar)
- Machery, Edouard, 2012, “Delineating the Moral
Domain”, Baltic International Yearbook of Cognition, Logic
and Communication, 7(1). doi:10.4148/biyclc.v7i0.1777 (Scholar)
- –––, 2018, “Morality: A Historical
Invention”, in Atlas of Moral Psychology, Kurt Gray and
Jesse Graham (eds), New York, NY: Guilford Press, 259–265. (Scholar)
- Machery, Edouard, Daniel Kelly, and Stephen P. Stich, 2005, “Moral Realism and Cross-Cultural Normative Diversity”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(6): 830–830. doi:10.1017/s0140525x05370142 (Scholar)
- Machery, Edouard and Ron Mallon, 2010, “Evolution of Morality”, in Doris et al. 2010: 3–46. (Scholar)
- Mackie, J. L., 1977, Ethics: Inventing Right and Wrong, New York: Pelican Books. (Scholar)
- Madva, Alex, 2016, “A Plea for Anti-Anti-Individualism: How Oversimple Psychology Misleads Social Policy”, Ergo, 3(27): 701–728. doi:10.3998/ergo.12405314.0003.027 (Scholar)
- Mameli, Matteo, 2013, “Meat Made Us Moral: A Hypothesis on the Nature and Evolution of Moral Judgment”, Biology & Philosophy, 28(6): 903–931. doi:10.1007/s10539-013-9401-3 (Scholar)
- Maner, Jon K., 2017, “Dominance and Prestige: A Tale of Two
Hierarchies”, Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 26(6): 526–531. doi:10.1177/0963721417714323 (Scholar)
- Marr, David, 1982, Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information, Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. (Scholar)
- Marwell, Gerald and Ruth E. Ames, 1981, “Economists Free
Ride, Does Anyone Else?”, Journal of Public Economics,
15(3): 295–310. doi:10.1016/0047-2727(81)90013-x (Scholar)
- Mathew, Sarah and Robert Boyd, 2011, “Punishment Sustains Large-Scale Cooperation in Prestate Warfare”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(28): 11375–11380. doi:10.1073/pnas.1105604108 (Scholar)
- Mathew, Sarah, Robert Boyd, and Matthijs van Veelen, 2013,
“Human Cooperation Among Kin and Close Associates May Require
Enforcement of Norms by Third Parties”, in Cultural
Evolution: Society, Technology, Language, and Religion, Peter J.
Richerson and Morten H. Christiansen (eds), (Strüngmann Forum
Report 12), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 45–60. (Scholar)
- Mathew, Sarah and Charles Perreault, 2015, “Behavioural
Variation in 172 Small-Scale Societies Indicates That Social Learning
Is the Main Mode of Human Adaptation”, Proceedings of the
Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1810): 20150061.
doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.0061 (Scholar)
- Mayr, Ernst, 1961, “Cause and Effect in
Biology”, American Association for the Advancement of
Science, 134(3489): 1501–1506. (Scholar)
- McAuliffe, Brendan J., Jolanda Jetten, Matthew J. Hornsey, and
Michael A. Hogg, 2003, “Individualist and Collectivist Norms:
When It’s Ok to Go Your Own Way”, European Journal of
Social Psychology, 33(1): 57–70. doi:10.1002/ejsp.129 (Scholar)
- McAuliffe, Katherine, Jillian J. Jordan, and Felix Warneken, 2015, “Costly Third-Party Punishment in Young Children”, Cognition, 134: 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.013 (Scholar)
- McElreath, Richard, Robert Boyd, and Peter J. Richerson, 2003,
“Shared Norms and the Evolution of Ethnic Markers”,
Current Anthropology, 44(1): 122–130.
doi:10.1086/345689 (Scholar)
- Mercier, Hugo and Dan Sperber, 2017, The Enigma of Reason, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Scholar)
- Mikhail, John, 2007, “Universal Moral Grammar: Theory, Evidence and the Future”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(4): 143–152. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.007 (Scholar)
- –––, 2011, Elements of Moral Cognition: Rawls'’ Linguistic Analogy and the Cognitive Science of Moral and Legal Judgment, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/cbo9780511780578 (Scholar)
- Morris, Michael W., Ying-yi Hong, Chi-yue Chiu, and Zhi Liu, 2015,
“Normology: Integrating Insights about Social Norms to
Understand Cultural Dynamics”, Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 129: 1–13.
doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2015.03.001 (Scholar)
- Moya, Cristina and Robert Boyd, 2015, “Different Selection Pressures Give Rise to Distinct Ethnic Phenomena: A Functionalist Framework with Illustrations from the Peruvian Altiplano”, Human Nature, 26(1): 1–27. doi:10.1007/s12110-015-9224-9 (Scholar)
- Muthukrishna, Michael, Thomas J.H. Morgan, and Joseph Henrich,
2016, “The When and Who of Social Learning and Conformist
Transmission”, Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(1):
10–20. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.05.004 (Scholar)
- Nichols, Shaun, 2004, Sentimental Rules: On the Natural
Foundations of Moral Judgement, New York: Oxford University
Press. doi:10.1093/0195169344.001.0001 (Scholar)
- –––, forthcoming, Rational Rules: Towards a Theory of Moral Learning, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
- Nichols, Shaun, Shikhar Kumar, Theresa Lopez, Alisabeth Ayars, and Hoi-Yee Chan, 2016, “Rational Learners and Moral Rules: Rational Learners and Moral Rules”, Mind & Language, 31(5): 530–554. doi:10.1111/mila.12119 (Scholar)
- Nielsen, Mark, Rohan Kapitány, and Rosemary Elkins, 2015,
“The Perpetuation of Ritualistic Actions as Revealed by Young
Children’s Transmission of Normative Behavior”,
Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(3): 191–198.
doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.11.002 (Scholar)
- Nisbett, Richard E., 2004, The Geography of Thought: How Asians and Westerners Think Differently…and Why, New York: Free Press. (Scholar)
- Nisbett, Richard E. and Dov Cohen, 1996, Culture Of Honor: The
Psychology Of Violence In The South, Boulder, CO: Westview
Press. (Scholar)
- Nucci, Larry P., 2001, Education in the Moral Domain,
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
doi:10.1017/cbo9780511605987 (Scholar)
- O’Gorman, Rick, David Sloan Wilson, and Ralph R. Miller,
2008, “An Evolved Cognitive Bias for Social Norms”,
Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(2): 71–78.
doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.07.002 (Scholar)
- O’Neill, Elizabeth, 2017, “Kinds of Norms”, Philosophy Compass, 12(5): e12416. doi:10.1111/phc3.12416 (Scholar)
- O’Neill, Elizabeth and Edouard Machery, 2018, “The Normative Sense: What is Universal? What Varies?”, in Zimmerman, Jones, and Timmons 2018: ch. 2. (Scholar)
- Oosterbeek, Hessel, Randolph Sloof, and Gijs van de Kuilen, 2004,
“Cultural Differences in Ultimatum Game Experiments: Evidence
from a Meta-Analysis”, Experimental Economics, 7(2):
171–188. doi:10.1023/b:exec.0000026978.14316.74 (Scholar)
- Peach, Jennifer, Emiko Yoshida, and Mark P. Zanna, 2011,
“Learning What Most People like: How Implicit Attitudes and
Normative Evaluations Are Shaped by Motivation and Culture and
Influence Meaningful Behavior”, in The Psychology of
Attitudes and Attitude Change, Joseph P. Forgas, Joel Cooper,
William D. Crano, and Steven J. Spencer (eds), Philadelphia:
Psychology Press, 95–108. (Scholar)
- Pinker, Steven, 2018, Enlightenment Now: The Case for Reason,
Science, Humanism, and Progress, New York: Viking Press. (Scholar)
- Price, Michael E, Leda Cosmides, and John Tooby, 2002,
“Punitive Sentiment as an Anti-Free Rider Psychological
Device”, Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(3):
203–231. doi:10.1016/s1090-5138(01)00093-9 (Scholar)
- Prinz, Jesse J., 2008, “Resisting the Linguistic Analogy: A
Commentary on Hauser, Young, and Cushman,” in Sinnott-Armstrong
2008: 157–170. (Scholar)
- Prooijen, Jan-Willem van, 2018, The Moral Punishment Instinct, Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oso/9780190609979.001.0001 (Scholar)
- Putnam, Hilary, 1963 [1975], “Brains and Behavior”,
Analytical Philosophy, Second Series, R. J. Butler (ed.),
(Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 211–235. Reprinted in Putnam 1975:
325–341. (Scholar)
- –––, 1967 [1975], “Psychological Predicates”, in Art, Mind and Religion, W. H. Capitan and D. D. Merrill (eds), Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press, 37–48. Reprinted as “The Nature of Mental States”, in Putnam 1975: 429–440. (Scholar)
- –––, Philosophical Papers, Volume 2: Mind,
Language, and Reality, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
doi:10.1017/CBO9780511625251.018
- de Quervain, Dominique J.-F., Urs Fischbacher, Valerie Treyer,
Melanie Schellhammer, Ulrich Schnyder, Alfred Buck, and Ernst Fehr,
2004, “The Neural Basis of Altruistic Punishment”,
Science, 305(5688): 1254–1258.
doi:10.1126/science.1100735 (Scholar)
- Rakoczy, Hannes, Katharina Hamann, Felix Warneken, and Michael
Tomasello, 2010, “Bigger Knows Better: Young Children
Selectively Learn Rule Games from Adults Rather than from
Peers”, British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
28(4): 785–798. doi:10.1348/026151009x479178 (Scholar)
- Rakoczy, Hannes and Marco F. H. Schmidt, 2013, “The Early
Ontogeny of Social Norms”, Child Development
Perspectives, 7(1): 17–21. doi:10.1111/cdep.12010 (Scholar)
- Rakoczy, Hannes, Felix Warneken, and Michael Tomasello, 2008,
“The Sources of Normativity: Young Children’s Awareness of
the Normative Structure of Games”, Developmental
Psychology, 44(3): 875–881.
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.875 (Scholar)
- Ramsey, Grant, 2013, “Culture in Humans and Other Animals”, Biology & Philosophy, 28(3): 457–479. doi:10.1007/s10539-012-9347-x (Scholar)
- Richerson, Peter J., 2013, “Human Cooperation is a Complex
Problem with Many Possible Solutions: Perhaps All of Them Are
True!”, Cliodynamics: The Journal of Theoretical and
Mathematical History, 4(1): 139–152. (Scholar)
- Richerson, Peter, Ryan Baldini, Adrian V. Bell, Kathryn Demps, Karl Frost, Vicken Hillis, Sarah Mathew, Emily K. Newton, Nicole Naar, Lesley Newson, Cody Ross, Paul E. Smaldino, Timothy M. Waring, and Matthew Zefferman, 2016, “Cultural Group Selection Plays an Essential Role in Explaining Human Cooperation: A Sketch of the Evidence”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39: e30. doi:10.1017/s0140525x1400106x (Scholar)
- Richerson, Peter J. and Robert Boyd, 2001, “The Evolution of Subjective Commitment to Groups: A Tribal Instincts Hypothesis”, in The Evolution and the Capacity for Commitment, Randolph M. Nesse (ed.), New York, NY: Russell Sage, 186–220. (Scholar)
- –––, 2005, Not By Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution, Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Scholar)
- –––, 2013, “Rethinking Paleoanthropology: a World Queerer Than We Supposed”, in Evolution of Mind, Brain, and Culture, Gary Hatfield and Holly Pittman (eds), Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Museum of Archaeology and Anthropology, 263–302. (Scholar)
- Richerson, Peter J., Robert Boyd, and Joseph Henrich, 2010, “Gene-Culture Coevolution in the Age of Genomics”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(Supplement 2): 8985–8992. doi:10.1073/pnas.0914631107 (Scholar)
- Richerson, Peter J. and Joseph Henrich, 2012, “Tribal Social Instincts and the Cultural Evolution of Institutions to Solve Collective Action Problems”, Cliodynamics: The Journal of Theoretical and Mathematical History, 3(1): 38–80. (Scholar)
- Riedl, Katrin, Keith Jensen, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello,
2012, “No Third-Party Punishment in Chimpanzees”,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(37):
14824–14829. doi:10.1073/pnas.1203179109 (Scholar)
- Robbins, Philip, 2009 [2017], “Modularity of Mind”
(Winter 2017 edition), in The Stanford Encyclopedia of
Philosophy, Edward N. Zalta (ed.), URl =
<https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2017/entries/modularity-mind/>. (Scholar)
- Roberts, Steven O., Susan A. Gelman, and Arnold K. Ho, 2017, “So It Is, So It Shall Be: Group Regularities License Children’s Prescriptive Judgments”, Cognitive Science, 41(supplement 3): 576–600. doi:10.1111/cogs.12443 (Scholar)
- Roberts, Steven O., Cai Guo, Arnold K. Ho, and Susan A. Gelman,
2018, “Children’s Descriptive-to-Prescriptive Tendency
Replicates (and Varies) Cross-Culturally: Evidence from China”,
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 165: 148–160.
doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.018 (Scholar)
- Roberts, Steven O., Arnold K. Ho, and Susan A. Gelman, 2019,
“The Role of Group Norms in Evaluating Uncommon and Negative
Behaviors”, Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 148(2): 374–387. doi:10.1037/xge0000534 (Scholar)
- Roedder, Erica and Gilbert Harman, 2010, “Linguistics and Moral Theory”, in Doris et al. 2010: 273 – 296. (Scholar)
- von Rohr, Claudia Rudolf, Judith M. Burkart, and Carel P. van Schaik, 2011, “Evolutionary Precursors of Social Norms in Chimpanzees: A New Approach”, Biology & Philosophy, 26(1): 1–30. doi:10.1007/s10539-010-9240-4 (Scholar)
- Ross, Don, 2012, “The Evolution of Individualistic Norms”, Baltic International Yearbook of Cognition, Logic and Communication, 7. doi:10.4148/biyclc.v7i0.1780 (Scholar)
- –––, 2019, “Consciousness, Language, and the Possibility of Non-Human Personhood: Reflections on Elephants”, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 26(3–4): 227–251. (Scholar)
- Roth, Alvin E., Vesna Prasnikar, Masahiro Okuno-Fujiwara, and
Shmuel Zamir, 1991, “Bargaining and Market Behavior in
Jerusalem, Ljubljana, Pittsburgh, and Tokyo: An Experimental
Study”, The American Economic Review, 81(5):
1068–1095. (Scholar)
- Rozin, Paul, Laura Lowery, Sumio Imada, and Jonathan Haidt, 1999,
“The CAD Triad Hypothesis: A Mapping between Three Moral
Emotions (Contempt, Anger, Disgust) and Three Moral Codes (Community,
Autonomy, Divinity).”, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 76(4): 574–586.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.4.574 (Scholar)
- Samland, Jana, Marina Josephs, Michael R. Waldmann, and Hannes
Rakoczy, 2016, “The Role of Prescriptive Norms and Knowledge in
Children’s and Adults’ Causal Selection”,
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(2):
125–130. doi:10.1037/xge0000138 (Scholar)
- Samuels, Richard, 1998, “Evolutionary Psychology and the Massive Modularity Hypothesis”, The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 49(4): 575–602. doi:10.1093/bjps/49.4.575 (Scholar)
- Sauer, Hanno, 2019, “Butchering Benevolence Moral Progress beyond the Expanding Circle”, Ethical Theory and Moral Practice, 22(1): 153–167. doi:10.1007/s10677-019-09983-9 (Scholar)
- Schmidt, Marco F.H., Hannes Rakoczy, and Michael Tomasello, 2011, “Young Children Attribute Normativity to Novel Actions without Pedagogy or Normative Language: Young Children Attribute Normativity”, Developmental Science, 14(3): 530–539. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01000.x (Scholar)
- Schmidt, Marco F. H., Lucas P. Butler, Julia Heinz, and Michael
Tomasello, 2016, “Young Children See a Single Action and Infer a
Social Norm: Promiscuous Normativity in 3-Year-Olds”,
Psychological Science, 27(10): 1360–1370.
doi:10.1177/0956797616661182 (Scholar)
- Schmidt, Marco F. H. and Michael Tomasello, 2012, “Young Children Enforce Social Norms”, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(4): 232–236. doi:10.1177/0963721412448659 (Scholar)
- Schultz, P. Wesley, Jessica M. Nolan, Robert B. Cialdini, Noah J.
Goldstein, and Vladas Griskevicius, 2007, “The Constructive,
Destructive, and Reconstructive Power of Social Norms”,
Psychological Science, 18(5): 429–434.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01917.x (Scholar)
- Schulz, Jonathan F., Duman Bahrami-Rad, Jonathan P. Beauchamp, and
Joseph Henrich, 2019, “The Church, Intensive Kinship, and Global
Psychological Variation”, Science, 366(6466): eaau5141.
doi:10.1126/science.aau5141 (Scholar)
- Shweder, Richard A., Nancy M. Much, Manamohan Mahapatra, and
Lawrence Park, 1997 “The ‘Big Three’ of Morality
(Autonomy, Community, and Divinity), and the ‘Big Three’
Explanations of Suffering”, in Morality and Health.,
Allan M. Brandt and Paul Rozin (eds), New York: Routledge. (Scholar)
- Singer, Peter, 1981 [2011], The Expanding Circle: Ethics and Sociobiology, New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux; revised edition, The Expanding Circle: Ethics, Evolution, and Moral Progress, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 2011. (Scholar)
- Sinnott-Armstrong, Walter (ed.), 2008, Moral Psychology,
Volume 2 The Cognitive Science of Morality: Intuition and
Diversity, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
- Smetana, Judith G., 1993, “Understanding of Social
Rules”, in The Development of Social Cognition: The Child As
Psychologist, Mark Bennett (ed.), New York: Guilford Press, .
111–141. (Scholar)
- Soon, Valerie, 2020, “Implicit Bias and Social Schema: A Transactive Memory Approach”, Philosophical Studies, 177(7): 1857–1877. doi:10.1007/s11098-019-01288-y (Scholar)
- Sperber, Dan, 1996, Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach, New York: Blackwell Publishers. (Scholar)
- Sripada, Chandra Sekhar and Stephen Stich, 2007, “A Framework for the Psychology of Norms”, The Innate Mind, Volume 2: Culture and Cognition, Peter Carruthers, Stephen Laurence, and Stephen Stich (eds.), New York: Oxford University Press, 280–301. (Scholar)
- Stanford, P. Kyle, 2018, “The Difference between Ice Cream and Nazis: Moral Externalization and the Evolution of Human Cooperation”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41: e95. doi:10.1017/s0140525x17001911 (Scholar)
- Sterelny, Kim, 2003, Thought in a Hostile World, New York, Blackwell. (Scholar)
- –––, 2012, The Evolved Apprentice: How
Evolution Made Humans Unique, Cambridge, MA. The MIT Press. (Scholar)
- Sterelny, Kim, Richard Joyce, Brett Calcott, and Ben Fraser (eds), 2013, Cooperation and Its Evolution, Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. (Scholar)
- Stich, Stephen, 2018, “The Quest for the Boundaries of Morality”, in Zimmerman, Jones, and Timmons 2018: ch. 1. (Scholar)
- –––, forthcoming, “Did Religion Play a
Role in the Evolution of Morality?”, Religion, Brain &
Behavior, first online: 27 December 2019, 1–11.
doi:10.1080/2153599x.2019.1678511 (Scholar)
- Suchak, Malini, Timothy M. Eppley, Matthew W. Campbell, Rebecca A.
Feldman, Luke F. Quarles, and Frans B. M. de Waal, 2016, “How
Chimpanzees Cooperate in a Competitive World”, Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(36): 10215–10220.
doi:10.1073/pnas.1611826113 (Scholar)
- Summers, Jesse S., 2017, “Rationalizing Our Way into Moral Progress”, Ethical Theory and Moral Practice, 20(1): 93–104. doi:10.1007/s10677-016-9750-5 (Scholar)
- Tabibnia, Golnaz, Ajay B. Satpute, and Matthew D. Lieberman, 2008,
“The Sunny Side of Fairness: Preference for Fairness Activates
Reward Circuitry (and Disregarding Unfairness Activates Self-Control
Circuitry)”, Psychological Science, 19(4):
339–347. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02091.x (Scholar)
- Tennie, Claudio, Josep Call, and Michael Tomasello, 2009,
“Ratcheting up the Ratchet: On the Evolution of Cumulative
Culture”, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:
Biological Sciences, 364(1528): 2405–2415.
doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0052 (Scholar)
- Thaler, Richard H., 1992, The Winner's Curse: Paradoxes and
Anomalies of Economic Life, New York: Free Press. (Scholar)
- Thieme, Hartmut, 1997, “Lower Palaeolithic Hunting Spears
from Germany”, Nature, 385(6619): 807–810.
doi:10.1038/385807a0 (Scholar)
- Tomasello, Michael, 1999, The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Scholar)
- –––, 2009, Why We Cooperate, Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press. (Scholar)
- –––, 2016, A Natural History of Human Morality, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Scholar)
- –––, 2019, Becoming Human: A Theory of
Ontogeny, Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press. (Scholar)
- Tooby, John and Leda Cosmides, 1992, “The Psychological
Foundations of Culture” in Barkow, Cosmides, and Tooby 1992:
19–136. (Scholar)
- –––, 2005, “Conceptual Foundations of
Evolutionary Psychology”, in The Handbook of Evolutionary
Psychology, David M. Buss (ed.), Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley &
Sons, 5–67. doi:10.1002/9780470939376.ch1 (Scholar)
- Turchin, Peter, 2018, Historical Dynamics: Why States Rise and
Fall, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Scholar)
- Turiel, Elliot, 1983, The Development of Social Knowledge, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (Scholar)
- Tworek, Christina M. and Andrei Cimpian, 2016, “Why Do
People Tend to Infer ‘Ought’ From ‘Is’? The
Role of Biases in Explanation”, Psychological Science,
27(8): 1109–1122. doi:10.1177/0956797616650875 (Scholar)
- Uskul, Ayse K., Susan E. Cross, Ceren Gunsoy, and Pelin Gul, 2019,
“Cultures of Honor”, in Handbook of Cultural
Psychology, second edition, New York: The Guilford Press,
793–821. (Scholar)
- Vaesen, Krist, Mark Collard, Richard Cosgrove, and Wil Roebroeks,
2016, “Population Size Does Not Explain Past Changes in Cultural
Complexity”, Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 113(16): E2241–E2247.
doi:10.1073/pnas.1520288113 (Scholar)
- Vaish, Amrisha, Esther Herrmann, Christiane Markmann, and Michael Tomasello, 2016, “Preschoolers Value Those Who Sanction Non-Cooperators”, Cognition, 153: 43–51. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2016.04.011 (Scholar)
- Vargas, Manuel and John M. Doris (eds), forthcoming, The Oxford Handbook of Moral Psychology, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
- Vincent, Sarah, Rebecca Ring, and Kristin Andrews, 2018, “Normative Practices of Other Animals”, in Zimmerman, Jones, and Timmons 2018: 57–83 (ch. 3). (Scholar)
- de Waal, Frans, 2006, Primates and Philosophers: How Morality Evolved, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Scholar)
- Wendel, W. Bradley, 2001, “Mixed Signals: Rational-Choice
Theories of Social Norms and the Pragmatics of Explanation”,
Washington & Lee Public Law Research Paper, 01-8.
doi:10.2139/ssrn.278277 (Scholar)
- Whitehead, Hal and Luke Rendell, 2015, The Cultural Lives of
Whales and Dolphins, Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
Press. (Scholar)
- Whiten, Andrew, 2019, “Cultural Evolution in Animals”,
Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 50(1):
27–48. doi:10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110218-025040 (Scholar)
- Whiten, Andrew, Nicola McGuigan, Sarah Marshall-Pescini, and Lydia
M. Hopper, 2009, “Emulation, Imitation, over-Imitation and the
Scope of Culture for Child and Chimpanzee”, Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
364(1528): 2417–2428. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0069 (Scholar)
- Yoshida, Emiko, Jennifer M. Peach, Mark P. Zanna, and Steven J.
Spencer, 2012, “Not All Automatic Associations Are Created
Equal: How Implicit Normative Evaluations Are Distinct from Implicit
Attitudes and Uniquely Predict Meaningful Behavior”, Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(3): 694–706.
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.09.013 (Scholar)
- Zimmerman, Aaron, Karen Jones, and Mark Timmons (eds), 2018, The Routledge Handbook of Moral Epistemology, Abingdon, UK: Routledge Press. doi:10.4324/9781315719696 (Scholar)