Linked bibliography for the SEP article "Reproducibility of Scientific Results" by Fiona Fidler and John Wilcox
This is an automatically generated and experimental page
If everything goes well, this page should display the bibliography of the aforementioned article as it appears in the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, but with links added to PhilPapers records and Google Scholar for your convenience. Some bibliographies are not going to be represented correctly or fully up to date. In general, bibliographies of recent works are going to be much better linked than bibliographies of primary literature and older works. Entries with PhilPapers records have links on their titles. A green link indicates that the item is available online at least partially.
This experiment has been authorized by the editors of the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. The original article and bibliography can be found here.
- Agnoli, Franca, Jelte M. Wicherts, Coosje L. S. Veldkamp, Paolo
Albiero, and Roberto Cubelli, 2017, “Questionable Research
Practices among Italian Research Psychologists”, Jakob
Pietschnig (ed.), PLoS ONE, 12(3): e0172792.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0172792 (Scholar)
- Allen, Peter J., Kate P. Dorozenko, and Lynne D. Roberts, 2016, “Difficult Decisions: A Qualitative Exploration of the Statistical Decision Making Process from the Perspectives of Psychology Students and Academics”, Frontiers in Psychology, 7(February): 188. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00188 (Scholar)
- Anderson, Christopher J., Štěpán Bahnik,
Michael Barnett-Cowan, Frank A. Bosco, Jesse Chandler, C. R. Chartier,
F. Cheung, et al., 2016, “Response to Comment on ‘Estimating
the Reproducibility of Psychological Science’”,
Science, 351(6277): 1037. doi:10.1126/science.aad9163 (Scholar)
- Anderson, Melissa S., Emily A. Ronning, Raymond De Vries, and
Brian C. Martinson, 2010, “Extending the Mertonian Norms:
Scientists’ Subscription to Norms of Research”, The
Journal of Higher Education, 81(3): 366–393.
doi:10.1353/jhe.0.0095 (Scholar)
- Atmanspacher, Harald and Sabine Maasen, 2016a,
“Introduction”, in Atmanspacher and Maasen 2016b:
1–8. doi:10.1002/9781118865064.ch0">10.1002/9781118865064.ch0 (Scholar)
- ––– (eds.), 2016b, Reproducibility: Principles, Problems, Practices, and Prospects, Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. doi:10.1002/9781118865064 (Scholar)
- Baker, Monya, 2016, “1,500 Scientists Lift the Lid on Reproducibility”, Nature, 533(7604): 452–454. doi:10.1038/533452a (Scholar)
- Bakker, Marjan, Chris H. J. Hartgerink, Jelte M. Wicherts, and Han
L. J. van der Maas, 2016, “Researchers’ Intuitions About Power
in Psychological Research”, Psychological Science,
27(8): 1069–1077. doi:10.1177/0956797616647519 (Scholar)
- Bakker, Marjan and Jelte M. Wicherts, 2011, “The
(Mis)Reporting of Statistical Results in Psychology Journals”,
Behavior Research Methods, 43(3): 666–678.
doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0089-5 (Scholar)
- Begley, C. Glenn and Lee M. Ellis, 2012, “Raise Standards
for Preclinical Cancer Research: Drug Development”,
Nature, 483(7391): 531–533. doi:10.1038/483531a (Scholar)
- Bem, Daryl J., 2011, “Feeling the Future: Experimental
Evidence for Anomalous Retroactive Influences on Cognition and
Affect”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
100(3): 407–425. (Scholar)
- Benjamin, Daniel J., James O. Berger, Magnus Johannesson, Brian A.
Nosek, Eric-Jan Wagenmakers, Richard Berk, Kenneth A. Bollen, et al.,
2018, “Redefine Statistical Significance”, Nature
Human Behaviour, 2(1): 6–10.
doi:10.1038/s41562-017-0189-z (Scholar)
- Braude, Stephen E., 1979, ESP and Psychokinesis. A Philosophical Examination, Philadelphia: Temple University Press. (Scholar)
- Button, Katherine S., John P. A. Ioannidis, Claire Mokrysz, Brian
A. Nosek, Jonathan Flint, Emma S. J. Robinson, and Marcus R.
Munafò, 2013, “Power Failure: Why Small Sample Size
Undermines the Reliability of Neuroscience”, Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 14(5): 365–376. doi:10.1038/nrn3475 (Scholar)
- Camerer C.F., et al., 2018, “Evaluating the replicability of
social science experiments in Nature and Science
between 2010 and 2015”, Nature Human Behaviour, 2:
637–644. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0399-z (Scholar)
- Cartwright, Nancy, 1991, “Replicability, Reproducibility and
Robustness: Comments on Harry Collins”, History of Political
Economy, 23(1): 143–155. (Scholar)
- Chambers, Christopher D., 2013, “Registered Reports: A New
Publishing Initiative at Cortex”, Cortex, 49(3):
609–610. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.12.016 (Scholar)
- –––, 2017, The Seven Deadly Sins of
Psychology A Manifesto for Reforming the Culture of Scientific
Practice, Princeton: Princeton University Press. (Scholar)
- Chang, Andrew C. and Phillip Li, 2015, “Is Economics
Research Replicable? Sixty Published Papers from Thirteen Journals Say
‘Usually Not’”, Finance and Economics Discussion
Series, 2015(83): 1–26. doi:10.17016/feds.2015.083 (Scholar)
- Churchman, C. West, 1948, “Statistics, Pragmatics, Induction”, Philosophy of Science, 15(3): 249–268. doi:10.1086/286991 (Scholar)
- Collins, Harry M., 1985, Changing Order: Replication and Induction in Scientific Practice, London; Beverly Hills: Sage Publications. (Scholar)
- –––, 2016, “Reproducibility of
experiments: experiments’ regress, statistical uncertainty
principle, and the replication imperative” in Atmanspacher and
Maasen 2016b: 65–82. doi:10.1002/9781118865064.ch4 (Scholar)
- Cohen, Jacob, 1962, “The Statistical Power of
Abnormal-Social Psychological Research: A Review”,, The
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65(3): 145–153.
doi:10.1037/h0045186 (Scholar)
- –––, 1994, “The Earth Is Round (\(p <
.05\))”, American Psychologist, 49(12): 997–1003,
doi:10.1037/0003-066x.49.12.997 (Scholar)
- Cova, Florian, Brent Strickland, Angela Abatista, Aurélien Allard, James Andow, Mario Attie, James Beebe, et al., forthcoming, “Estimating the Reproducibility of Experimental Philosophy”, Review of Philosophy and Psychology, early online: 14 June 2018. doi:10.1007/s13164-018-0400-9 (Scholar)
- Cristea, Ioana Alina and John P. A. Ioannidis, 2018, “P
Values in Display Items Are Ubiquitous and Almost Invariably Significant: A Survey of Top Science Journals”, Christos A. Ouzounis (ed.), PLoS ONE, 13(5): e0197440. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0197440 (Scholar)
- Cumming, Geoff, 2012, Understanding the New Statistics: Effect
Sizes, Confidence Intervals, and Meta-Analysis. New York:
Routledge. (Scholar)
- Cumming, Geoff and Robert Calin-Jageman, 2017, Introduction to
the New Statistics: Estimation, Open Science and Beyond, New
York: Routledge. (Scholar)
- Cumming, Geoff, Fiona Fidler, Martine Leonard, Pavel Kalinowski,
Ashton Christiansen, Anita Kleinig, Jessica Lo, Natalie McMenamin, and
Sarah Wilson, 2007, “Statistical Reform in Psychology: Is
Anything Changing?”, Psychological Science, 18(3):
230–232. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01881.x (Scholar)
- Di Bucchianico, Marilena, 2014, “A Matter of Phronesis:
Experiment and Virtue in Physics, A Case Study”, in Virtue
Epistemology Naturalized, Abrol Fairweather (ed.), Cham: Springer
International Publishing, 291–312.
doi:10.1007/978-3-319-04672-3_17 (Scholar)
- Dominus, Susan, 2017, “When the Revolution Came for Amy
Cuddy”, The New York Times, October 21, Sunday
Magazine, page 29. (Scholar)
- Douglas, Heather, 2016, “Values in Science”, in Paul Humphreys, The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Science, New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 609–630. (Scholar)
- Earp, Brian D. and David Trafimow, 2015, “Replication, Falsification, and the Crisis of Confidence in Social Psychology”, Frontiers in Psychology, 6(May): 621. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00621 (Scholar)
- Errington, Timothy M., Elizabeth Iorns, William Gunn, Fraser
Elisabeth Tan, Joelle Lomax, and Brian A Nosek, 2014, “An Open
Investigation of the Reproducibility of Cancer Biology
Research”, ELife, 3(December): e043333.
doi:10.7554/elife.04333 (Scholar)
- Etz, Alexander and Joachim Vandekerckhove, 2016, “A Bayesian
Perspective on the Reproducibility Project: Psychology”, Daniele
Marinazzo (ed.), PLoS ONE, 11(2): e0149794.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0149794 (Scholar)
- Fanelli, Daniele, 2010a, “Do Pressures to Publish Increase
Scientists’ Bias? An Empirical Support from US States Data”,
Enrico Scalas (ed.), PLoS ONE, 5(4): e10271.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010271 (Scholar)
- –––, 2010b, “‘Positive’
Results Increase Down the Hierarchy of the Sciences”, Enrico
Scalas (ed.), PLoS ONE, 5(4): e10068.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010068 (Scholar)
- –––, 2012, “Negative Results Are
Disappearing from Most Disciplines and Countries”,
Scientometrics, 90(3): 891–904.
doi:10.1007/s11192-011-0494-7 (Scholar)
- Fang, Ferric C., R. Grant Steen, and Arturo Casadevall, 2012,
“Misconduct Accounts for the Majority of Retracted Scientific
Publications”, Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 109(42): 17028–17033.
doi:10.1073/pnas.1212247109 (Scholar)
- Feest, Uljana, 2016, “The Experimenters’ Regress Reconsidered: Replication, Tacit Knowledge, and the Dynamics of Knowledge Generation”, Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A, 58(August): 34–45. doi:10.1016/j.shpsa.2016.04.003 (Scholar)
- Fidler, Fiona, Mark A. Burgman, Geoff Cumming, Robert Buttrose,
and Neil Thomason, 2006, “Impact of Criticism of Null-Hypothesis
Significance Testing on Statistical Reporting Practices in
Conservation Biology”, Conservation Biology, 20(5):
1539–1544. doi:10.1111/j.1523-1739.2006.00525.x (Scholar)
- Fidler, Fiona, Yung En Chee, Bonnie C. Wintle, Mark A. Burgman,
Michael A. McCarthy, and Ascelin Gordon, 2017, “Metaresearch for
Evaluating Reproducibility in Ecology and Evolution”,
BioScience, 67(3): 282–289.
doi:10.1093/biosci/biw159 (Scholar)
- Fiedler, Klaus and Norbert Schwarz, 2016, “Questionable
Research Practices Revisited”, Social Psychological and
Personality Science, 7(1): 45–52.
doi:10.1177/1948550615612150 (Scholar)
- Fiske, Susan T., 2016, “A Call to Change Science’s Culture
of Shaming”, Association for Psychological Science
Observer, 29(9).
[Fiske 2016 available online] (Scholar)
- Franklin, Allan, 1989, “The Epistemology of
Experiment”, in David Gooding, Trevor Pinch, and Simon Schaffer
(eds.), The Uses of Experiment: Studies in the Natural
Sciences, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp.
437–460. (Scholar)
- –––, 1994, “How to Avoid the Experimenters’ Regress”, Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A, 25(3): 463–491. doi:10.1016/0039-3681(94)90062-0 (Scholar)
- Franklin, Allan and Harry Collins, 2016, “Two Kinds of Case
Study and a New Agreement”, in The Philosophy of Historical
Case Studies, Tilman Sauer and Raphael Scholl (eds.), Cham:
Springer International Publishing, 319: 95–121.
doi:10.1007/978-3-319-30229-4_6 (Scholar)
- Fraser, Hannah, Tim Parker, Shinichi Nakagawa, Ashley Barnett, and
Fiona Fidler, 2018, “Questionable Research Practices in Ecology
and Evolution”, Jelte M. Wicherts (ed.), PLoS ONE,
13(7): e0200303. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0200303 (Scholar)
- Freedman, Leonard P., Iain M. Cockburn, and Timothy S. Simcoe,
2015, “The Economics of Reproducibility in Preclinical
Research”, PLoS Biology, 13(6): e1002165.
doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002165 (Scholar)
- Giner-Sorolla, Roger, 2012, “Science or Art? How Aesthetic
Standards Grease the Way Through the Publication Bottleneck but
Undermine Science”, Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 7(6): 562–571. doi:10.1177/1745691612457576 (Scholar)
- Gigerenzer, Gerd, 2018, “Statistical Rituals: The
Replication Delusion and How We Got There”, Advances in
Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(2):
198–218. doi:10.1177/2515245918771329 (Scholar)
- Gilbert, Daniel T., Gary King, Stephen Pettigrew, and Timothy D.
Wilson, 2016, “Comment on ‘Estimating the Reproducibility
of Psychological Science’”, Science, 351(6277):
1037–1037. doi:10.1126/science.aad7243 (Scholar)
- Goldman, Alvin I., 1999, Knowledge in a Social World,
Oxford: Clarendon. doi:10.1093/0198238207.001.0001 (Scholar)
- Gómez, Omar S., Natalia Juristo, and Sira Vegas, 2010,
“Replications Types in Experimental Disciplines”, in
Proceedings of the 2010 ACM-IEEE International Symposium on
Empirical Software Engineering and Measurement - ESEM ’10,
Bolzano-Bozen, Italy: ACM Press. doi:10.1145/1852786.1852790 (Scholar)
- Hackett, B., 2005, “Essential tensions: Identity, control, and
risk in research”, Social Studies of Science, 35(5):
787–826. doi:10.1177/0306312705056045 (Scholar)
- Haller, Heiko, and Stefan Krauss, 2002, “Misinterpretations
of Significance: a Problem Students Share with Their Teachers?”
Methods of Psychological Research—Online, 7(1):
1–20.
[Haller & Kraus 2002 available online] (Scholar)
- Hartgerink, Chris H.J., Robbie C.M. van Aert, Michèle B.
Nuijten, Jelte M. Wicherts, and Marcel A.L.M. van Assen, 2016,
“Distributions of p-Values Smaller than .05 in
Psychology: What Is Going On?”, PeerJ, 4(April): e1935.
doi:10.7717/peerj.1935 (Scholar)
- Hendrick, Clyde, 1991. “Replication, Strict Replications,
and Conceptual Replications: Are They Important?”, in Neuliep
1991: 41–49. (Scholar)
- Ioannidis, John P. A., 2005, “Why Most Published Research Findings Are False”, PLoS Medicine, 2(8): e124. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124 (Scholar)
- Ioannidis, John P. A., Daniele Fanelli, Debbie Drake Dunne, and
Steven N. Goodman, 2015, “Meta-Research: Evaluation and
Improvement of Research Methods and Practices”, PLOS
Biology, 13(10): e1002264. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002264 (Scholar)
- Jennions, Michael D. and Anders Pape Møller, 2003, “A
Survey of the Statistical Power of Research in Behavioral Ecology and
Animal Behavior”, Behavioral Ecology, 14(3):
438–445. doi:10.1093/beheco/14.3.438 (Scholar)
- John, Leslie K., George Loewenstein, and Drazen Prelec, 2012,
“Measuring the Prevalence of Questionable Research Practices
With Incentives for Truth Telling”, Psychological
Science, 23(5): 524–532. doi:10.1177/0956797611430953 (Scholar)
- Kaiser, Jocelyn, 2018, “Plan to Replicate 50 High-Impact
Cancer Papers Shrinks to Just 18”, Science, 31 July
2018. doi:10.1126/science.aau9619 (Scholar)
- Keppel, Geoffrey, 1982, Design and Analysis. A
Researcher’s Handbook, second edition, Englewood Cliffs,
NJ: Prentice-Hall. (Scholar)
- Kerr, Norbert L., 1998, “HARKing: Hypothesizing After the
Results Are Known”, Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 2(3): 196–217. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0203_4 (Scholar)
- Kidwell, Mallory C., Ljiljana B. Lazarević, Erica Baranski,
Tom E. Hardwicke, Sarah Piechowski, Lina-Sophia Falkenberg, Curtis
Kennett, et al., 2016, “Badges to Acknowledge Open Practices: A
Simple, Low-Cost, Effective Method for Increasing Transparency”,
Malcolm R Macleod (ed.), PLOS Biology, 14(5): e1002456.
doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002456 (Scholar)
- Klein, Richard A., Kate A. Ratliff, Michelangelo Vianello,
Reginald B. Adams, Štěpán Bahník, Michael J.
Bernstein, Konrad Bocian, et al., 2014, “Investigating Variation
in Replicability: A ‘Many Labs’ Replication
Project”, Social Psychology, 45(3): 142–152.
doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000178 (Scholar)
- Lakens, Daniel, Federico G. Adolfi, Casper J. Albers, Farid
Anvari, Matthew A. J. Apps, Shlomo E. Argamon, Thom Baguley, et al.,
2018, “Justify Your Alpha”, Nature Human
Behaviour, 2(3): 168–171.
doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0311-x (Scholar)
- Longino, Helen E., 1990, Science as Social Knowledge: Values and Objectivity in Scientific Inquiry, Princeton: Princeton University Press. (Scholar)
- –––, 1996, “Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Values in Science: Rethinking the Dichotomy”, in Feminism, Science, and the Philosophy of Science, Lynn Hankinson Nelson and Jack Nelson (eds.), Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 39–58. doi:10.1007/978-94-009-1742-2_3 (Scholar)
- –––, 1997, “Feminist Epistemology as a Local Epistemology: Helen E. Longino”, Aristotelian Society Supplementary Volume, 71(1): 19–35. doi:10.1111/1467-8349.00017 (Scholar)
- Lykken, David T., 1968, “Statistical Significance in
Psychological Research”, Psychological Bulletin, 70(3,
Pt.1): 151–159. doi:10.1037/h0026141 (Scholar)
- Madden, Charles S., Richard W. Easley, and Mark G. Dunn, 1995,
“How Journal Editors View Replication Research”,
Journal of Advertising, 24(December): 77–87.
doi:10.1080/00913367.1995.10673490 (Scholar)
- Makel, Matthew C., Jonathan A. Plucker, and Boyd Hegarty, 2012,
“Replications in Psychology Research: How Often Do They Really
Occur?”, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6):
537–542. doi:10.1177/1745691612460688 (Scholar)
- MacCoun, Robert J. and Saul Perlmutter, 2017, “Blind
Analysis as a Correction for Confirmatory Bias in Physics and in
Psychology”, in Psychological Science Under Scrutiny,
Scott O. Lilienfeld and Irwin D. Waldman (eds.), Hoboken, NJ: John
Wiley & Sons, pp. 295–322.
doi:10.1002/9781119095910.ch15 (Scholar)
- Martin, B., 1992, “Scientific fraud and the power structure
of science”, Prometheus, 10(1):
83–98. doi:10.1080/08109029208629515 (Scholar)
- Masicampo, E.J. and Daniel R. Lalande, 2012, “A Peculiar
Prevalence of p Values Just below .05”, Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(11): 2271–2279.
doi:10.1080/17470218.2012.711335 (Scholar)
- Mahoney, Michael J., 1985, “Open Exchange and Epistemic
Progress”,, American Psychologist, 40(1): 29–39. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.40.1.29 (Scholar)
- Meehl, Paul E., 1967, “Theory-Testing in Psychology and Physics: A Methodological Paradox”, Philosophy of Science, 34(2): 103–115. doi:10.1086/288135 (Scholar)
- –––, 1978, “Theoretical Risks and Tabular
Asterisks: Sir Karl, Sir Ronald, and the Slow Progress of Soft
Psychology”, Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 46(4): 806–834.
doi:10.1037/0022-006x.46.4.806 (Scholar)
- Merton, Robert K., 1942 [1973], “A Note on Science and
Technology in a Democratic Order”, Journal of Legal and
Political Sociology, 1(1–2): 115–126; reprinted
as “The Normative Structure of Science”, in Robert K.
Merton (ed.) The Sociology of Science: Theoretical and Empirical
Investigations, Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. (Scholar)
- Munafò, Marcus R., Brian A. Nosek, Dorothy V. M. Bishop,
Katherine S. Button, Christopher D. Chambers, Nathalie Percie du Sert,
Uri Simonsohn, Eric-Jan Wagenmakers, Jennifer J. Ware, and John P. A.
Ioannidis, 2017, “A Manifesto for Reproducible Science”,
Nature Human Behaviour, 1(1): 0021.
doi:10.1038/s41562-016-0021 (Scholar)
- Neuliep, James William (ed.), 1991, Replication Research in
the Social Sciences, (Journal of social behavior and personality;
8: 6), Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications. (Scholar)
- Neuliep, James W. and Rick Crandall, 1990, “Editorial Bias
Against Replication Research”, Journal of Social Behavior
and Personality, 5(4): 85–90 (Scholar)
- Nosek, Brian A. and Daniël Lakens, 2014, “Registered
Reports: A Method to Increase the Credibility of Published
Results”, Social Psychology, 45(3): 137–141.
doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000192 (Scholar)
- Nosek, Brian A., Jeffrey R. Spies, and Matt Motyl, 2012,
“Scientific Utopia: II. Restructuring Incentives and Practices
to Promote Truth Over Publishability”, Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 7(6): 615–631.
doi:10.1177/1745691612459058 (Scholar)
- Nosek, B. A., G. Alter, G. C. Banks, D. Borsboom, S. D. Bowman, S.
J. Breckler, S. Buck, et al., 2015, “Promoting an Open Research
Culture”, Science, 348(6242): 1422–1425.
doi:10.1126/science.aab2374, (Scholar)
- Nosek, Brian A., Charles R. Ebersole, Alexander C. DeHaven, and
David T. Mellor, 2018, “The Preregistration Revolution”,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(11):
2600–2606. doi:10.1073/pnas.1708274114 (Scholar)
- Nuijten, Michèle B., Chris H. J. Hartgerink, Marcel A. L.
M. van Assen, Sacha Epskamp, and Jelte M. Wicherts, 2016, “The
Prevalence of Statistical Reporting Errors in Psychology
(1985–2013)”, Behavior Research Methods, 48(4):
1205–1226. doi:10.3758/s13428-015-0664-2 (Scholar)
- Oakes, Michael, 1986, Statistical Inference: A Commentary for
the Social and Behavioral Sciences, New York: Wiley. (Scholar)
- Open Science Collaboration (OSC), 2015, “Estimating the
Reproducibility of Psychological Science”, Science,
349(6251): 943–951. doi:10.1126/science.aac4716 (Scholar)
- Oransky, Ivan, 2016, “Half of Biomedical Studies Don’t
Stand up to Scrutiny and What We Need to Do about That”, The
Conversation, 11 November 2016.
[Oransky 2016 available online] (Scholar)
- Parker, T.H., E. Main, S. Nakagawa, J. Gurevitch, F. Jarrad, and
M. Burgman, 2016, “Promoting Transparency in Conservation
Science: Editorial”, Conservation Biology, 30(6):
1149–1150. doi:10.1111/cobi.12760 (Scholar)
- Pashler, Harold and Eric-Jan Wagenmakers, 2012,
“Editors’ Introduction to the Special Section on Replicability
in Psychological Science: A Crisis of Confidence?”,
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6): 528–530.
doi:10.1177/1745691612465253 (Scholar)
- Peng, Roger D., 2011, “Reproducible Research in
Computational Science”, Science, 334(6060):
1226–1227. doi:10.1126/science.1213847 (Scholar)
- –––, 2015, “The Reproducibility Crisis in
Science: A Statistical Counterattack”, Significance,
12(3): 30–32. doi:10.1111/j.1740-9713.2015.00827.x (Scholar)
- Radder, Hans, 1996, In And About The World: Philosophical Studies Of Science And Technology, Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. (Scholar)
- –––, 2003, “Technology and Theory in
Experimental Science”, in Hans Radder (ed.), The Philosophy
of Scientific Experimentation, Pittsburgh: University of
Pittsburgh Press, pp. 152–173. (Scholar)
- –––, 2006, The World Observed/The World Conceived, Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press. (Scholar)
- –––, 2009, “Science, Technology and the
Science-Technology Relationship”, in Anthonie Meijers (ed.),
Philosophy of Technology and Engineering Sciences, Amsterdam:
Elsevier, pp. 65–91. doi:10.1016/b978-0-444-51667-1.50007-0 (Scholar)
- –––, 2012, The Material Realization of Science: From Habermas to Experimentation and Referential Realism, Boston: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-94-007-4107-2 (Scholar)
- Rauscher, Frances H., Gordon L. Shaw, and Catherine N. Ky, 1993,
“Music and Spatial Task Performance”, Nature,
365(6447): 611–611. doi:10.1038/365611a0 (Scholar)
- Rauscher, Frances H., Gordon L. Shaw, and Katherine N. Ky, 1995,
“Listening to Mozart Enhances Spatial-Temporal Reasoning:
Towards a Neurophysiological Basis”, Neuroscience
Letters, 185(1): 44–47.
doi:10.1016/0304-3940(94)11221-4 (Scholar)
- Ritchie, Stuart J., Richard Wiseman, and Christopher C. French,
2012, “Failing the Future: Three Unsuccessful Attempts to
Replicate Bem’s ‘Retroactive Facilitation of Recall’
Effect”, Sam Gilbert (ed.), PLoS ONE, 7(3): e33423.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0033423 (Scholar)
- Rooney, Phyllis, 1992, “On Values in Science: Is the Epistemic/Non-Epistemic Distinction Useful?”, PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association, 1992(1): 13–22. doi:10.1086/psaprocbienmeetp.1992.1.192740 (Scholar)
- Rosenthal, Robert, 1979, “The File Drawer Problem and
Tolerance for Null Results”, Psychological Bulletin,
86(3): 638–641. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638 (Scholar)
- –––, 1991, “Replication in Behavioral
Research”, in Neuliep 1991: 1–39. (Scholar)
- Rosnow, Ralph L. and Robert Rosenthal, 1989, “Statistical
Procedures and the Justification of Knowledge in Psychological
Science”,, American Psychologist, 44(10):
1276–1284. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.44.10.1276 (Scholar)
- Rowhani-Farid, Anisa, Michelle Allen, and Adrian G. Barnett, 2017,
“What Incentives Increase Data Sharing in Health and Medical
Research? A Systematic Review”, Research Integrity and Peer
Review, 2: 4. doi:10.1186/s41073-017-0028-9 (Scholar)
- Rudner, Richard, 1953, “The Scientist Qua Scientist Makes Value Judgments”, Philosophy of Science, 20(1): 1–6. doi:10.1086/287231 (Scholar)
- Sargent, C.L., 1981, “The Repeatability Of Significance And
The Significance Of Repeatability”, European Journal of
Parapsychology, 3: 423–433. (Scholar)
- Schekman, Randy, 2013, “How Journals like Nature, Cell and
Science Are Damaging Science | Randy Schekman”, The
Guardian, December 9, sec. Opinion,
[Schekman 2013 available online] (Scholar)
- Schmidt, Stefan, 2009, “Shall We Really Do It Again? The
Powerful Concept of Replication Is Neglected in the Social
Sciences”, Review of General Psychology, 13(2):
90–100. doi:10.1037/a0015108 (Scholar)
- Silberzahn, Raphael,and Uhlmann, Eric L., 2015, “Many hands
make tight work: crowdsourcing research can balance discussions,
validate findings and better inform policy”, Nature, 526(7572):
189–192. (Scholar)
- Simmons, Joseph P., Leif D. Nelson, and Uri Simonsohn, 2011,
“False-Positive Psychology: Undisclosed Flexibility in Data
Collection and Analysis Allows Presenting Anything as
Significant”, Psychological Science, 22(11):
1359–1366. doi:10.1177/0956797611417632 (Scholar)
- Smith, Daniel R., Ian C.W. Hardy, and Martin P. Gammell, 2011,
“Power Rangers: No Improvement in the Statistical Power of
Analyses Published in Animal Behaviour”, Animal
Behaviour, 81(1): 347–352.
doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.09.026 (Scholar)
- Sovacool, B. K., 2008, “Exploring scientific misconduct: Isolated individuals, impure institutions, or an inevitable idiom of modern science?” Journal of Bioethical Inquiry, 5: 271–282. doi: 10.1007/s11673-008-9113-6 (Scholar)
- Steel, Daniel, 2010, “Epistemic Values and the Argument from Inductive Risk*”, Philosophy of Science, 77(1): 14–34. doi:10.1086/650206 (Scholar)
- Stegenga, Jacob, 2018, Medical Nihilism, Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Scholar)
- Steinle, Friedrich, 2016, “Stability and Replication of
Experimental Results: A Historical Perspective”, in Atmanspacher
and Maasen 2016b: 39–68. doi:10.1002/9781118865064.ch3 (Scholar)
- Sterling, Theodore D., 1959, “Publication Decisions and
Their Possible Effects on Inferences Drawn from Tests of Significance
– or Vice Versa”, Journal of the American Statistical
Association, 54(285): 30–34.
doi:10.1080/01621459.1959.10501497 (Scholar)
- Sutton, Jon, 2018, “Tone Deaf?”, The
Psychologist, 31: 12–13.
[Sutton 2018 available online] (Scholar)
- Szucs, Denes and John P. A. Ioannidis, 2017, “Empirical
Assessment of Published Effect Sizes and Power in the Recent Cognitive
Neuroscience and Psychology Literature”, Eric-Jan Wagenmakers
(ed.), PLoS Biology, 15(3): e2000797.
doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.2000797 (Scholar)
- Teira, David, 2013, “A Contractarian Solution to the Experimenter’s Regress”, Philosophy of Science, 80(5): 709–720. doi:10.1086/673717 (Scholar)
- Vazire, Simine, 2018, “Implications of the Credibility
Revolution for Productivity, Creativity, and Progress”,
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(4): 411–417.
doi:10.1177/1745691617751884 (Scholar)
- Wagenmakers, Eric-Jan, Ruud Wetzels, Denny Borsboom, Han L. J. van
der Maas, and Rogier A. Kievit, 2012, “An Agenda for Purely
Confirmatory Research”, Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 7(6): 632–638. doi:10.1177/1745691612463078 (Scholar)
- Washburn, Anthony N., Brittany E. Hanson, Matt Motyl, Linda J.
Skitka, Caitlyn Yantis, Kendal M. Wong, Jiaqing Sun, et al., 2018,
“Why Do Some Psychology Researchers Resist Adopting Proposed
Reforms to Research Practices? A Description of Researchers’
Rationales”, Advances in Methods and Practices in
Psychological Science, 1(2): 166–173.
doi:10.1177/2515245918757427 (Scholar)
- Wasserstein, Ronald L. and Nicole A. Lazar, 2016, “The
ASA’s Statement on p-Values: Context, Process, and Purpose”,
The American Statistician, 70(2): 129–133.
doi:10.1080/00031305.2016.1154108 (Scholar)