Skip to content
Publicly Available Published by De Gruyter December 19, 2022

Into the Black Box: Sex and Gender in the Study on Decision-Making – An Evidence from a Slovak Sample

  • Magdalena Adamus ORCID logo and Eva Ballová Mikušková ORCID logo EMAIL logo
From the journal Human Affairs

Abstract

The main goal of the paper was to obtain insights into how gender measures can be incorporated into quantitative research on risk-related behaviour. We explored relations between the measures (short versions of Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI), Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ), and Traditional Masculinity-Femininity (TMF) scale) and their explanatory power in relation to risky behaviours (Decision Outcome Inventory, DOI). The sample consisted of 470 adults (238 men). The corresponding BSRI and PAQ subscales correlated significantly, while TMF correlated positively with the femininity subscales. All the instruments demonstrated good internal consistency and the measures explained a significant portion of risky behaviour. The results suggest that, although sex is a proxy of behaviour, using a measure of the gender-related aspects of identity could enhance understanding of risk-related behaviour. Finally, men and women viewed themselves as equally masculine, indicating that gender stereotypes about desirability of agentic characteristics change.

1 Introduction

People expect men to be masculine and women to be feminine, but because our definitions of these two concepts are vague gender is often treated as a black box: we all have the impression that we know what gender is but encounter serious problems when try putting it into words (Hoffman et al., 2005). Therefore, building on a long-standing appeal to “create a better, more accurate conceptualisation of gender/sex for use in education, research, and practice” (Hyde et al., 2019, p. 15), we sought to improve understanding of sex/gender differences in a domain-specific quantitative research. We assumed that, even if the concepts of sex and gender overlap, application of sex as a proxy of gender may be substantially and methodologically flawed, as we can expect gender to be a better predictor of behaviour than biological sex (Muehlenhard & Peterson, 2011; Unger, 1979).

The main aim of the present study was to ascertain whether the two most popular, parsimonious measures of the gender-related self-concept Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) and Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ) can be reliably used in research on gendered behaviour—in this case, attitudes towards risk. To boost theoretical contribution of the paper, we also included the Traditional Masculinity-Femininity (TMF) scale and the dichotomous measure of self-stated biological sex (Westbrook & Saperstein, 2015). To meet the main goal, we investigated whether the selected questionnaires have sufficient (i) construct validity and (ii) explanatory power; that is, whether they meet the requirements of a good measure. Consequently, we (a) explored the relation between the questionnaires (BSRIshort/PAQshort and the TMF) to see whether instrumental and expressive traits are part of the individual’s subjective concept of masculinity and femininity, (b) examined the explanatory power of BSRIshort/PAQshort and the TMF in relation to risky behaviour measured with Decision Outcome Inventory and (c) explored whether gender and sex overlap.

1.1 Research on Differences Between Men and Women

The psychological research on differences between men and women has a long history, with the oldest studies being more than a hundred years old and dating back to Freud’s time at least (Spence & Buckner, 2000). The simple questions these studies asked provided simple answers. Women turned out to be less intelligent, have a different brain structure and, in the male-dominated economic world, were considered farther from the rational ideal than men (see Hyde, 2005; Hyde et al., 2019 for an overview). However, despite this long history, there is still great debate over what we understood by gender differences and how the issue should be approached.

As gender became a buzzword, many scholars claimed to be studying gender differences, when in fact they were mistaking gender for sex, coding it as a dummy variable, and—as Maney (2016) and Westbrook and Saperstein (2015) observed—tacitly implying that the differences are biological in origin and immutable. Connell (2006) concluded that treating gender as a fixed and dichotomous category leads to understanding statistical differences between the sexes as an explanation of thereof. Furthermore, Westbrook and Saperstein (2015, p. 534) noticed that “essentialist practices that treat sex and gender as synonymous (…) will continue to reproduce statistical representations that erase important dimensions of variation and likely limit understanding of the processes that perpetuate social inequality”. Even today, in the literature, there are few studies in which the relation between gender and performance have been investigated seriously for instance in association with entrepreneurial intentions (Adamus, et al., 2021a; Gupta et al., 2009), risk aversion (Adamus, et al., 2021b; Meier-Pesti & Penz, 2008) and political behaviour (Bittner & Goodyear-Grant, 2017). Nevertheless, despite being scarce, all these studies have suggested the observed differences between men and women are better explained by variables other than biological sex, including masculinity and femininity.

1.2 Measurement Issues

This gap in research may be caused by the lack of commonly accepted parsimonious and reliable measures. Although we expect that a significant portion of an individual’s identity is shaped by their gender-related self, measuring it is a problem because gender remains an abstract and ephemeral concept. Smiler and Epstein (Smiler & Epstein, 2010) observed that in the late 1980s, over 1400 gender-related measures were published—nearly six times as many as in the previous decade. However, only a small proportion of these newly published scales have been used or referred to more than five times (Smiler & Epstein, 2010). This shows two important things: there was (and still is) enormous interest in studying gender and even greater disagreement about the appropriate methods and approaches for capturing it.

So what are the characteristics of a good gender-related measure? Constantinople (1973) believed that it should serve as a reliable predictor of an individual’s sex, others have claimed that it should also predict sexual orientation (Kachel et al., 2016). Today, however, such an approach seems redundant. If the gender measure is to be an approximation of biological sex, then we have no need for it—it would be simpler to ask individuals about their self-ascribed biological sex. Moreover, Bem (1974) hinted that identifying traits as masculine or feminine based on the frequency with which they are considered typical of men or women would be biased. If we want an instrument that can be used to understand the gender phenomenon, we should focus on socially desirable traits rather than typical traits (Bem, 1974, p. 155). In our view, a good gender-related measure should meet two requirements: (i) reflect at least part of the gender-related self-concept (construct validity) and (ii) predict some forms of gender-related behaviour or social action (explanatory power) (Athenstaedt, 2003; Unger, 1979).

Although none of the existing measures seems to provide a comprehensive picture of the global gender-related self, quantitative studies do not shy away from asking questions about the scope and sources of gender differences. However, confounding sex and gender in quantitative studies limits our understanding of gendered phenomena and their potential underpinnings. To investigate differences between men and women properly, such research should be equipped with proper tools: both informative and concise. Consequently, we decided to explore whether any of the two personality traits-based questionnaires could become convenient candidates to be used by quantitative researchers. Importantly, Smiler and Epstein (Smiler & Epstein, 2010) observed the BSRI and PAQ are the most frequently used gender measures. Therefore, employing one of the two questionnaires could foster comparability of results delivered by the older literature with those yet to come. Furthermore, aware of the limitations of quantitative research, we decided to use the short versions of the BSRI and PAQ questionnaires because they have good reliability and because in more extensive studies it is pragmatic to use short forms rather than the more time-consuming 60-item questionnaires.

1.3 Sex, Gender and Risk-Taking

Measuring gender should not only involve describing at least part of the gender-related self but also the different forms of behaviour stereotypically associated with each of the sexes. One of such economically important and greatly gendered phenomena is risk-taking. Its importance stems primarily from the associations between risk-taking and favourable economic outcomes. For decades, the psychological and economic literature had indicated that women were more risk-averse and consequently, fared worse in terms of economic performance. Until a seminal paper by Nelson (2015) cast doubt on this. Since her results were published, an increasing number of studies have concluded that there are no significant differences between men and women (e.g., Csermely & Rabas, 2016; Filippin & Crosetto, 2016). In the Slovak context, Adamus (2021b) showed that regardless of the measure, men and women tend to express similar attitudes towards risk in economic contexts. The authors pointed that in the strictly financial domain risk attitudes may be better explained by factors other than biological sex including the BSRI femininity subscale. Therefore, to avoid setting too narrow a definition, in this paper we decided to define risky behaviour in line with the common-sense definition, namely as a situation involving potential negative consequences, losses or injury. Such forms of behaviour may be also well-described as reckless or careless. To do so, we employed Decision Outcome Inventory (Bruine de Bruin et al., 2007) that captures potentially harmful outcomes of every-day decisions. In other words, it measures how an individual behave in situations involving risk of a negative, maladaptive or ineffective outcome.

2 Methods

2.1 Objectives and Research Questions

In this study we address the issues encountered in domain-specific research when attempts are made to incorporate gender-related considerations into the design. The first essential step in gender-sensitive research is to obtain a proper understanding of the analytical distinction between sex and gender. The second is to ensure the appropriate instruments are used to obtain reliable measurements of the phenomena being investigated. Although the study is explorative and so there are no hypotheses, we put forward three research questions and stipulated explanations of possible results. First, we asked whether individuals scoring high on expressive (instrumental) traits have the notion of being feminine (masculine) as well. The consistency between those self-ascriptions would indicate that by measuring expressive and instrumental traits, BSRI and PAQ tap into the subjective notion of being feminine and/or masculine. Then, we sought answer to the question of the measures explanatory power. Are BSRI and PAQ capable of explaining gendered risky behaviour above and beyond biological sex? The measure that would show best construct validity and, at the same time, better explain gendered behaviour could be considered a reliable – among currently available – concise measure of gender-related self with a potential to provide meaningful information about antecedents of gendered behaviour.

2.2 Participants and Procedure

A total of 470 Slovak adults (238 men, 232 women) consented to and participated in Study 1 (Mage = 42.35, SDage = 13.12). Participants were 470 adults. Participation was voluntary and anonymous. Data collection was through an online survey hosted on Qualtrics and data were collected by an external agency (chosen in a tender and compliant with the international ESOMAR code). Participants were selected according to the following criteria: non-student, aged 18 and over, gender balance, education level—a balanced of secondary school education and university education, sample size 440 (the sample size was calculated using an alpha of 0.05, a confidence interval 95% and effect size 0.14; yielding a sample of 400 + 10%). Data collection was to be terminated after reaching the desired number (440); finally, the external agency collected data from 470 participants.

After reading and signing the informed consent form, the participants completed demographic questions on age and self-stated biological sex, the BSRI, PAQ, TMF and Decision Outcome Inventory, as well as a further six questionnaires (not reported here) that were intedended to form an independent study. The materials included two control questions to eliminate random responses (e.g., participants had to click on a specific option: ‘Now click response 4.’). Participants giving a wrong response to the control questions were not allowed to participate further in the research.

2.3 Measures

The Traditional Masculinity-Femininity questionnaire (TMF, Kachel et al., 2016) is a measure of self-ascribed femininity and masculinity. The instrument consists of six items measuring gender-role adoption (1 item), gender-role preference (1 item) and gender-role identity (4 items). Participants had to rate each item on a continuous, 7-point scale ranging from very masculine (1) to very feminine (7). Accordingly, participants choosing the middle value (i.e., 4) can be considered moderately feminine and masculine.

Bem’s Sex Role Inventory (BSRI, Bem, 1974) and the Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ, Spence et al., 1975) are two older instruments reflecting a trait approach that posits that masculinity and femininity are closely related to the individual’s instrumental and expressive traits. Studies have shown that the two subscales in the two instruments are weakly correlated, indicating that masculinity and femininity are two independent, unipolar dimensions. Based on results regarding instrument reliability reported by Fernández and Coello (2010), we decided to apply the short versions of the questionnaires (12 items BSRI and 16 items PAQ, the short version of the PAQ is now considered standard). Participants rated themselves on each item, on a scale from 1 (never or almost never true) to 7 (almost always true); a higher score indicated a higher masculinity/femininity.

Due to sufficient sample size we used random 66% of the sample to conduct Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and 33% of the sample to conduct Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to verify the factor structures of BSRI and PAQ.

Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted on all items of BSRI (n = 313) with oblique rotation (oblimin). Bartlett’s test of sphericity, χ2 (66) = 2188.46, p < 0.001 and KMO = 0.86, indicated that correlations between items were sufficiently large for EFA. An initial analysis was run to obtain eigenvalues for each component in the data. Two components had eigenvalues over Kaiser’s criterion of 1 and in combination explained 58% of the variance. The scree plot showed inflexions that would justify retaining these 2 factors. Component 1 represented masculinity and consisted of all original BSRI items of masculinity; Component 2 represented femininity and consisted of all original BSRI items of femininity. Internal consistency of components is in Table 1.

Table 1:

Description of the sample.

N α M SD t p d
Risky behaviour (DOI) 470 5.49 2.97
 Men 238 6.10 3.10 4.65 <0.001 0.43
 Women 232 4.85 2.70
Masculinity (BSRI) 470 0.84 4.56 1.01
 Men 238 4.56 1.00 0.04 0.968 0.01
 Women 232 4.56 1.02
Femininity (BSRI) 470 0.89 5.07 0.96
 Men 238 4.90 1.03 3.98 <0.001 0.37
 Women 232 5.25 0.86
Masculinity (PAQ) 470 0.79 3.49 0.58
 Men 238 3.51 0.60 0.78 0.437 0.07
 Women 232 3.46 0.55
Femininity (PAQ) 470 0.86 3.73 0.57
 Men 238 3.60 0.59 5.22 <0.001 0.48
 Women 232 3.87 0.52
Traditional masculinity-femininity (TMF) 470 0.98 3.66 2.27
 Men 238 1.67 0.91 42.30 <0.001 3.90
 Women 232 5.70 1.15
Gender-role adaptation (TMF) 470 3.72 2.45
 Men 238 1.57 1.08 42.33 <0.001 3.91
 Women 232 5.92 1.14
Gender-role preference (TMF) 470 3.74 2.55
 Men 238 1.47 0.99 44.80 <0.001 4.13
 Women 232 6.07 1.23
Gender-role identity (TMF) 470 0.97 3.66 2.27
 Men 238 1.67 0.91 42.30 <0.001 3.90
 Women 232 5.70 1.15
  1. Note: N, number; α, Cronbach’s alpha; M, mean; SD, standard deviation; min, minimum of the sample; max, maximum of the sample; t, the value of the t-test; df, degrees of freedom; p, the value of significance; d, Cohen’s d.

We applied the Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) to assess the fit of two factors model of BSRI on a 157 randomly selected participants. The model showed acceptable fit (GFI = 0.85; NFI = 0.85; CFI = 0.89; RMSEA = 0.11; SRMR = 0.18; RFI = 0.82; IFI = 0.90; PNFI = 0.70). Moreover, the factors had excellent composite reliability (CR) and convergent reliability (average variance extracted – AVE). For masculinity CR = 1.00 and AVE = 1.00, for femininity CR = 0.98 and AVE = 0.91.

Similarly, exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted on all items of PAQ (n = 313) with oblique rotation (oblimin). Bartlett’s test of sphericity, χ2(120) = 1633.29, p < 0.001 and KMO = 0.88, indicated that correlations between items were sufficiently large for EFA. Two components had eigenvalues over Kaiser’s criterion of 1 and in combination explained 39% of the variance. The scree plot showed inflexions that would justify retaining 2 factors: Component 1 represented masculinity and consisted of all original PAQ items of masculinity and Component 2 represented femininity and consisted of all original PAQ items of femininity. Internal consistency of components is in Table 1.

Next, we the Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was conducted to assess the fit of two factors model of PAQ on a 157 randomly selected participants. The model showed acceptable fit (GFI = 0.86; NFI = 0.77; CFI = 0.86; RMSEA = 0.09; SRMR = 0.14; RFI = 0.73; IFI = 0.86; PNFI = 0.67). The factors had good composite reliability (CR) and weaker convergent reliability (average variance extracted – AVE). For masculinity CR = 0.75 and AVE = 0.28, for femininity CR = 0.79 and AVE = 0.32.

The Decision Outcome Inventory (DOI, Bruine de Bruin et al., 2007) measures experiences of life events that are the result of decision-making. Participants had to choose from a list of life events where individuals may make the wrong decision (for example buying food, weddings) and those who had experienced these life situations responded to the items concerning the negative consequences of the decision (for example, having to throw out food they had bought). The last six items (being imprisoned, involved in a public fight or argument; being bankrupt; forgetting someone close’s birthday; being diagnosed with Type 2 diabetes; having a fracture due to a fall, slip, or a misstep) were answered by all the participants regardless of whether they had experienced the situation or not. The total score from the negative-consequence items was calculated as follows: the last six items were automatically awarded 1 point (for example, a participant who had been in prison earned 1 point); for the other items, we calculated the value using the following formula:

1(incidenceofanegativeconsequenceinthesampleincidenceofalifeeventinthesample)

For example, participants who threw out food they had bought earned 0.12 points:

1(82participantshadthrownoutfood93participantshadboughtfood)=0.12

The lower the score, the less negative the real-life outcomes the participant had experienced in life, the less risky behaviour they reported.

3 Data Analysis

All measured variables were described by means and standard deviations and internal consistency of instruments was computed as Cronbach’s alpha. To examine potential differences between men and women in all variables, the independent sample t-test was used and effect size was measured by Cohen’s d.

To understand the distinction between sex and gender, the typology of participants was done in two ways: types according self-ascribed femininity and masculinity (TMF) were determined by mean and standard deviation, types according traits masculinity and femininity (BSRI and PAQ) were computed through K-mean cluster analysis. Overlap between types as well as gender and sex overlap were examined using crosstabs; association between types and sexes was determined by a chi-squared test.

To examine whether instruments BSRI, PAQ and TMF have sufficient construct validity, the correlation analysis was conducted on all variables. Moreover, TMF types were compared using analysis of variance (one-way ANOVA) in their traits masculinity and femininity and the comparison of masculinity scores from BSRI and PAQ as well as comparison of femininity scores from BSRI and PAQ were done by paired t-test.

To examine explanatory power of BSRI and PAQ, the hierarchical regression analysis was used with risky behaviour as dependent variable and biological sex and femininity and masculinity as independent variables.

Data are available at https://osf.io/wy38b/?view_only=7bd2f107cdd3453d83fc05f0a6bd4fdd.

4 Results

4.1 Descriptive Statistics and Sex Differences

The descriptive statistics of the variables including internal consistency of instruments are given in Table 1. Men expressed stronger tendency to risky behaviour than women, and there were sex differences in all variables except masculinity (BSRI, PAQ): women had higher femininity and identified themselves more as feminine than men (sex differences in characteristics from BSRI and PAQ are displayed in the Figure 1).

Figure 1: 
            Masculine and feminine traits – sex differences. Note: BSRI, Bem’s sex role inventory; PAQ, personal attributes questionnaire.
Figure 1:

Masculine and feminine traits – sex differences. Note: BSRI, Bem’s sex role inventory; PAQ, personal attributes questionnaire.

4.2 Gender and Sex Overlap

To examine the self-ascribed femininity and masculinity and traits femininity and masculinity overlap and gender and biological sex overlap, we divided participants into three types according to mean and standard deviation in the TMF: as masculine were labelled those with a low score (M − 1SD; range 1–1.40), as feminine those with a high score (M + 1SD; range 5.92–7) and as unspecific or moderately feminine and masculine those between low and high score (range 1.41–5.91). Next, using K-mean cluster analysis (with 4 number of clusters, silhouette measure of cohesion and separation was 0.26), we divided participants into four groups according to their BSRI and PAQ scores: feminine (high in femininity and low in masculinity), masculine (high in masculinity and low in femininity), androgyny (high in femininity and masculinity) and unspecified (low in femininity and masculinity).

An association between the TMF typology and BSRI/PAQ typology could be observed (χ2 (6) = 72.04, p < 0.000). For more detail see Table 2; 36.4 percent of participants identified themselves as masculine had also traits masculinity (BSRI/PAQ) and 33.9 percent of those identified themselves as feminine had trait femininity (BSRI/PAQ). There were more feminine participants that had trait masculinity (14.8%) than masculine participants with trait femininity (9.0%). Those who identified themselves as feminine are more specified than masculine: only 9.5 percent of feminine, but up to 23.2 percent of masculine ones, were unspecific (low both in masculinity and femininity).

Table 2:

Comparison of TMF typology versus BSRI/PAQ typology.

BSRI/PAQ TMF typology
Typology Masculine Feminine Unspecific Total
Feminine N 10 41 60 111
% Within BSRI/PAQ type 9.0% 36.9% 54.1% 100.0%
% Within TMF type 8.3% 33.9% 26.3% 23.6%
Masculine N 44 20 71 135
% Within BSRI/PAQ type 32.6% 14.8% 52.6% 100.0%
% Within TMF type 36.4% 16.5% 31.1% 28.7%
Androgyny N 45 51 33 129
% Within BSRI/PAQ type 34.9% 39.5% 25.6% 100.0%
% Within TMF type 37.2% 42.1% 14.5% 27.4%
Unspecific N 22 9 64 95
% Within BSRI/PAQ type 23.2% 9.5% 67.4% 100.0%
% Within TMF type 18.2% 7.4% 28.1% 20.2%
Total N 121 121 228 470
% Within BSRI/PAQ type 25.7% 25.7% 48.5% 100.0%
% Within TMF type 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%

Next, comparing gender and biological sex (Table 3); an association between gender and biological sex could be observed (TMF: χ2 (2) = 238.07, p < 0.000; BSRI/PAQ: χ2 (3) = 32.35, p < 0.001). We found that 50.8 percent of men described themselves as masculine (TMF), while 33.2 percent of men had masculinity traits in the BSRI/PAQ. Similarly, 51.7 percent of women identified themselves as feminine, while 33.2 percent of women had femininity traits in the BSRI/PAQ.

Table 3:

Gender and sex overlap.

TMF typology BSRI/PAQ typology
Masculine Feminine Unspecific Masculine Feminine Androgyny Unspecific
Men N 121 1 116 79 34 61 64
% Within sex 50.8% 0.4% 48.7% 33.2% 14.3% 25.6% 26.9%
% Within TMF type 100.0% 0.8% 50.9% 58.5% 30.6% 47.3% 67.4%
Women N 0 120 112 56 77 68 31
% Within sex 0.0% 51.7% 48.3% 24.1% 33.2% 29.3% 13.4%
% Within TMF type 0.0% 99.2% 49.1% 41.5% 69.4% 52.7% 32.6%
Total N 121 121 228 135 111 129 95
% Within sex 25.7% 25.7% 48.5% 28.7% 23.6% 27.4% 20.2%
% Within TMF type 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%

4.3 Correlation Analysis

Correlations between all variables are in Table 4. The age related positively to femininity (BSRI, PAQ) and weakly with masculinity (PAQ).

Table 4:

Correlations among all variables.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1 Age
2 Decision outcomes (DOI) 0.02
3 Masculinity (BSRI) 0.08 0.05
4 Femininity (BSRI) 0.17∗∗ −0.11 0.34∗∗∗
5 Masculinity (PAQ) 0.09 −0.06 0.70∗∗∗ 0.31∗∗∗
6 Femininity (PAQ) 0.12 −0.12 0.25∗∗∗ 0.76∗∗∗ 0.39∗∗∗
7 Traditional masculinity-femininity (TMF) −0.02 −0.23∗∗∗ −0.02 0.22∗∗∗ −0.05 0.26∗∗∗
8 Gender-role adaptation (TMF) −0.02 −0.23∗∗∗ −0.04 0.21∗∗∗ −0.07 0.26∗∗∗ 0.95∗∗∗
9 Gender-role preference (TMF) −0.03 −0.22∗∗∗ −0.03 0.22∗∗∗ −0.06 0.27∗∗∗ 0.95∗∗∗ 0.93∗∗∗
10 Gender-role identity (TMF) −0.02 −0.23∗∗∗ −0.02 0.22∗∗∗ −0.05 0.26∗∗∗ 1.00∗∗∗ 0.95∗∗∗ 0.95∗∗∗

The BSRI correlated with the PAQ in the expected way: there was a strong positive correlation between the masculinity subscales of the BSRI and the PAQ as well as between the femininity subscales of the BSRI and the PAQ. Moreover, there was a weak positive correlation between the TMF and the femininity subscales of the BSRI and the PAQ (and no correlation between the TMF and the masculinity subscales of the BSRI and the PAQ).

4.4 Construct Validity of BSRI, PAQ and TMF

The construct validity of instruments BSRI, PAQ and TMF was examined through mutual correlations and paired comparison. A weak correlation between masculinity and femininity as traits (BSRI and PAQ) and the identification as masculine/feminine (TMF) were due to continuous scale of rating TMF items (ranging from very masculine to very feminine). To verify whether the subjective gender-related self-concept (TMF) reflects personality traits (BSRI, TMF) we divided participants into types using the TMF score, as mentioned above (Gender and sex overlap section). We compare these three types of participants in their traits masculinity and femininity. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed significant differences both in masculinity and femininity (BSRI, PAQ). Descriptive statistics and post-hoc tests are in Table 5. Participants who identified themselves as moderately feminine and masculine (unspecific) had lowest score of masculinity and femininity also in BSRI and PAQ. Interestingly, there were significant differences between masculine and feminine types of participants in femininity but not in masculinity (BSRI, PAQ): feminine participant had significantly higher trait femininity than masculine participants but masculine participant did not have significantly higher trait masculinity than feminine participants.

Table 5:

Masculinity and femininity (BSRI, PAQ) of masculine and feminine types (TMF).

Types (TMF) N M SD Post-hoc test (Tukey)
Masculinity (BSRI) Masculine 121 4.82 1.00 Masculine – unspecific: p < 0.001
Unspecific 228 4.37 0.97 Feminine – unspecific: p = 0.024
Feminine 121 4.66 1.02 Masculine – feminine: ns
F (467) = 9.03; p < 0.001
Femininity (BSRI) Masculine 121 5.07 1.09 Masculine – unspecific: p = 0.003
Unspecific 228 4.81 0.86 Feminine – unspecific: p < 0.001
Feminine 121 5.57 0.81 Masculine – feminine: p < 0.001
F (467) = 27.84; p < 0.001
Masculinity (PAQ) Masculine 121 3.70 0.63 Masculine – unspecific: p < 0.001
Unspecific 228 3.35 0.50 Feminine – unspecific: p = 0.012
Feminine 121 3.53 0.58 Masculine – feminine: ns
F (467) = 15.92; p < 0.001
Femininity (PAQ) Masculine 121 3.72 0.64 Masculine – unspecific: ns
Unspecific 228 3.58 0.52 Feminine – unspecific: p < 0.001
Feminine 121 4.03 0.48 Masculine – feminine: p < 0.001
F (467) = 27.29; p < 0.001
  1. Note: N, number; M, mean; SD, standard deviation; F, value of ANOVA; p, the value of significance.

The relationships between BSRI and PAQ trait masculinity and BSRI and PAQ trait femininity were examined by paired t-tests. There were significant differences (t = 35.414; p < 0.001; d = 1.401) between BSRI masculinity (M = 4.42; SD = 1.03) and PAQ masculinity (M = 3.39; SD = 0.59) and also between BSRI femininity (M = 5.10; SD = 0.96) and PAQ femininity (M = 3.78; SD = 0.57), t = 53.4475; p < 0.001; d = 2.115. To sum up, participants scored higher in BSRI than in PAQ.

4.5 Prediction of Gender-Related Behaviour

We found that risky behaviour correlated negatively with the femininity subscales of the BSRI and PAQ and TMF (Table 4). To examine the effect of biological sex and gender on specific risky behaviours (DOI), we calculated the hierarchical regression analysis.

A hierarchical regression was calculated to predict risky behaviour based on gender (BSRI, PAQ, TMF) and biological sex. The biological sex (dummy variable, man = 0, woman = 1) was entered in the first step, self-ascribed femininity and masculinity (TMF) in the second step, masculinity and femininity (BSRI, PAQ) entered in the third step of regression. Regression statistics are in Table 6.

Table 6:

Hierarchical regressions for variables predicting risky behaviour.

B SE β t p
1 (Constant) 6.103 0.189 32.353 <0.001
Biological sex (0 = men, 1 = women) −1.249 0.268 −0.210 −4.651 <0.001
2 (Constant) 6.577 0.286 22.959 <0.001
Biological sex (0 = men, 1 = women) −0.102 0.587 −0.017 −0.173 0.862
Self-ascribed femininity and masculinity (TMF) −0.285 0.130 −0.217 −2.194 0.029
3 (Constant) 8.028 1.021 7.865 <0.001
Biological sex (0 = men, 1 = women) −0.259 0.583 −0.044 −0.444 0.657
Self-ascribed femininity and masculinity (TMF) −0.249 0.130 −0.190 −1.911 0.057
Masculinity (BSRI) 0.671 0.193 0.228 3.471 0.001
Femininity (BSRI) −0.386 0.225 −0.125 −1.719 0.086
Masculinity (PAQ) −1.140 0.348 −0.221 −3.273 0.001
Femininity (PAQ) 0.365 0.388 0.071 0.940 0.348

The hierarchical multiple regression revealed that at step 1, the biological sex contribute significantly to the regression model, F (1468) = 21.635, p < 0.001 and explained 4.4% of the variation in risky behaviour. Introducing the self-ascribed femininity and masculinity (TMF) explained only an additional 1.0% of the variation in risky behaviour and this change in R2 was significant, F (2467) = 13.312, p < 0.001. Finally, introducing masculinity and femininity (BSRI, PAQ) explained an additional 3.1% of the variation in risky behaviour and this change in R2 was significant, F (6463) = 7.148, p < 0.001. Only masculinity was significant predictor of risky behaviour; surprisingly, BSRI masculinity was positive predictor and PAQ masculinity was negative predictor of risky behaviour.

5 Discussion

The main aim of the study was to investigate the applicability of gender-related measures to the research areas where gender/sex differences are likely to occur. The paper contribution is two-fold. First, it informs the theory by investigating whether the BSRIshort and PAQshort measures are reliable approximations of gender-related self. Second, by discussing applicability of selected gender measures in quantitative research, the study shows that the measures have a potential to explain gendered behaviour above and beyond biological sex.

First, all the measures used in the study exhibited good internal consistency, possibly because we used the short versions of the BSRI and PAQ, which generally have greater reliability (Campbell et al., 1997; Fernández & Coello, 2010). In addition, the BSRIshort and PAQshort masculinity and femininity scores correlate with the respective TMF scores. As a consequence, we can infer that instrumental and expressive traits covered by BSRIshort and PAQshort correspond with an individual’s self-ascribed masculinity and femininity, respectively. When individuals feel feminine (and/or masculine), they assign more expressive (and/or instrumental) traits to themselves, indicating that the PAQ and BSRI tap into the subjective notion of masculinity and femininity. However, the correlations are weak or at best moderate, clearly showing that the concepts of femininity and masculinity are more complex than the trait approach suggests. Moreover, a follow-up study (see online appendix: https://osf.io/wy38b/?view_only=7bd2f107cdd3453d83fc05f0a6bd4fdd) showed that instrumental and expressive traits are at best loosely associated with the current notion of the ideal man or woman. The divergence between the content of the instruments and contemporary notions of masculinity and femininity provides a good reason for further investigation on the multi-facetted and perhaps volatile concept of gender.

Interpreting the results, we should remember that Slovak society may be more conservative than other EU countries. For instance, according to the Eurobarometer, Slovakia is amongst the most conservative EU countries when it comes to the perception of gender roles appropriate for men and women (Cukrowska-Torzewska & Lovasz, 2020). About half of the Slovak society believe that men are less competent than women in household tasks and fathers should put their careers ahead of looking after their children. The self-ascribed instrumental and expressive traits may therefore be more stereotypical than in other countries. However, over time and with societal shifts, the content of the gender-related self-concept can change, and individuals assessing themselves as feminine or masculine will not necessarily exhibit the instrumental and expressive traits in the BSRI and PAQ. Our findings on the convergence on masculinity and femininity scores among men and women suggest this process has already begun, at least for instrumental traits.

Secondly, we investigated the explanatory power of the measures in relation to a stereotypically male domain. We found that the measures explained a significant portion of risky behaviour. Risky behaviour was negatively related to femininity and, surprisingly, not at all to masculinity. A subsequent analysis revealed that risky behaviour was predicted by masculinity (more than by sex), but the finding is ambiguous: risky behaviour becomes more frequent as the BSRI and TMF masculinity scores increase, yet becomes less frequent as PAQ masculinity increase. But, despite these puzzling findings, we can say that the measures meet the second requirement (explanatory power) and can therefore be considered sound operationalisations of gender—seen through the lenses of instrumental and expressive traits—for use in quantitative research. The conclusion is perhaps that masculinity and/or femininity should not be taken as a global measure of gender-related self, but rather as a measure of a specific element—instrumentality and expressivity—that could serve as a predictor of behaviour stereotypically associated with one sex.

Finally, an unexpected—but not unprecedented (see Spence & Helmreich, 1978)—result of the study was that despite considerable similarities between BSRI and PAQ, the study suggests the scales seem to have different ecological validity and slightly favour BSRI due to stronger correlations (and in the expected direction) with gendered behaviour. The study shows that the scales seem to differ in sensitivity and ecological validity. First, participants consistently rated themselves higher in subscales of BSRI than PAQ. Despite extensive similarities and duplicated items between the measures (e.g., warm and gentle items of the femininity subscales), our participants reported a stronger identification with items encompassed in the BSRI subscales. Additionally, BSRI subscales showed stronger expected relation with risky behaviour. Surprisingly, thus, the findings seem to provide preliminary evidence favouring the application of the BSRI questionnaire in quantitative research on gendered forms of behaviour. The relation between the measures as well as their relations with various forms of behaviour believed to differ across the sexes require further thorough investigation.

Nevertheless, the study shows how superficial—and harmful for understanding the sources of differences between men and women—the research confounding sex with gender may be. Even if we observe men and women to differ in their every-day risky or reckless behaviours, biological sex is far from being a sufficient explanation. Similarly, the few studies that endeavoured to capture gender in addition to sex, consistently show that gender explains the differences above and beyond biological sex. For instance, a study on entrepreneurial intentions among the Slovaks found that those who view themselves as more similar to successful entrepreneurs (in terms of gendered traits) are more likely to consider entrepreneurship a suitable career (Adamus, 2021a). Without taking into account the gendered dimension, we would observe women to express lower entrepreneurial intentions but remained unable to understand sources of the differences.

This indicates that conflating sex and gender fails to recognise the complexity of gendered phenomena. Methodologically, this approach may be relatively unchallenging: all researchers have to do is ask individuals about their biological sex and any subsequent comparisons of the two groups are likely to reveal differences. But research should not stick with easy solutions petrifying women’s disadvantaged position, inequality and discrimination. Instead, it should delve deep into the intricacies of gendered norms and behaviours. Responding to the calls of gender scholars, the present study provides an efficient tool that could serve this aim helping us understanding that biological sex should not be taken as a proxy of abilities, skills or performance. There is much more in an individual’s characteristics that could better explain choices and behaviours of men and women than the binary approach to sex does. Each time, the researchers adopt a broader approach including at least some—perhaps imperfect—approximation of gender, the results are more comprehensive and informative. This, in turn, boosts our understanding of the sources of differences between men and women and allows to pin point at least some of the differences to persistent gender stereotypes and norms requiring men and women to behave in accordance with social expectations.

As our knowledge increases, we expect more research to incorporate measures of gender as well as self-ascribed sex and notions of gender as a socially constructed influence on behaviour. More gender-sensitive research could bring better and more reliable knowledge about the scope and depth of differences between men and women. Given the complexity of the phenomenon and the fact that sex and gender are mutually intertwined and inseparable, our ambition was not to disentangle social and biological factors, but to indicate methods for investigating the social influences reflected in the gender construction in a parsimonious, quantitative design. Including a simple measure of the gender-related self-concept could help us understand the dimensions and sources of the differences.

Acknowledging that the measures we selected do not sufficiently capture the multidimensionality of the phenomenon of gender-related self, we think they describe a small but important part of gender identity and one that is sufficient for predicting behaviours that are stereotypically attributed overwhelmingly to one gender. Even simple selective instruments can be insightful and provide valuable information about the sources of the differences and (where important) their relation to the person’s gender-role identity. The aim of the paper was to stress that until proven beyond reasonable doubt sources of the differences between men and women should be approached with caution. It is clear that the questions we ask determine the answers we get and that the methodology we use determines our interpretations of the findings (Hyde et al., 2019). A simple conclusion could be that there are differences in attitudes towards risk in men and women, but a more observant person would notice that the differences are much more complex than first appears and, perhaps, would abstain from labelling them as innate and immutable.

5.1 The Study Limitations and Future Directions

Despite our best efforts, the study is not free from limitations. Primarily, our sample was too small and homogenous for us to be able to address the issue of intersectionality, which can play a significant role in gender differences research and particularly the role of stereotypes in behaviour. Second, as all the variables were elicited through self-reports, it is possible that more instrumental individuals presented themselves as more risk-seeking, believing that both risk-seeking and instrumentality are desirable characteristics (self-serving attributional bias).

6 Conclusions

Despite these limitations, our study strongly suggests that using a measure of the gender-related aspects of identity in domain-specific quantitative research could enhance gender-related research. We do not claim that the quantitative method is best suited to investigating sources of differences between men and women. Properly framed, however, it would enable us to investigate the sources of behaviour and, going one step further, to explain observed differences between men and women, beyond treating biological sex as a final explanation. Failing to recognise the sociocultural impacts of gender places limits on the quality, applicability and generalisability of research (Smith & Koehoorn, 2016; Westbrook & Saperstein, 2015). This simplistic approach reifies the status quo by indicating that women are either uninterested, unmotivated or not sufficiently gifted to engage in forms of behaviour that are socially prescribed for males. We do not attempt to suggest that there are no differences between men and women or that all the differences are of social rather than biological origin. Instead, we contend that although they do not provide the full picture by any means, results obtained using simple gender-related instruments could be seen to provide a counterpoint to essentialist interpretations.


Corresponding author: Eva Ballová Mikušková, Centre of Social and Psychological Sciences, Slovak Academy of Sciences, Bratislava, Slovakia, E-mail:

Funding source: The study was supported by VEGA grant 2/0146/22: Psychological constructs and contextual frameworks determining the intention of girls and women to study ICT fields.

  1. Research funding: The study was supported by VEGA grant 2/0146/22: Psychological constructs and contextual frameworks determining the intention of girls and women to study ICT fields.

References

Adamus, M., Čavojová, V., & Šrol, J. (2021a). The impact of stereotyped perceptions of entrepreneurship and gender-role orientation on Slovak women’s entrepreneurial intentions. Gender in Management, 36(6), 745–761. https://doi.org/10.1108/gm-06-2020-0179.Search in Google Scholar

Adamus, M., Grežo, M., & Dudeková, K. (2021b). Stereotypes concerns and discreet existence of differences between men and women in risk-aversion – a replication study. Studia Psychologica, 63(3), 221–251. https://doi.org/10.31577/sp.2021.03.824.Search in Google Scholar

Athenstaedt, U. (2003). On the content of the gender role self-concept : Including gender-stereotypical behaviors in addition to traits. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27(4), 309–318. https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-6402.00111.Search in Google Scholar

Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42(2), 155–162. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0036215.Search in Google Scholar

Bittner, A., & Goodyear-Grant, E. (2017). Sex isn’t gender: Reforming concepts and measurements in the study of public opinion. Political Behavior, 39, 1019–1041. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-017-9391-y.Search in Google Scholar

Bruine de Bruin, W., Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2007). Individual differences in adult decision-making competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(5), 938–956. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.938.Search in Google Scholar

Campbell, T., Gillaspy, A., & Thompson, B. (1997). The factor structureof the Bem Sex-Role Inventory (BSRI): Confirmatory analysis of long and short forms. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 57(1), 118–124. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164497057001008.Search in Google Scholar

Connell, R. (2006). Glass ceilings or gendered institutions? Mapping the gender regimes of oublic sector worksites. Public Administration Review, 66(6), 837–849. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6210.2006.00652.x.Search in Google Scholar

Constantinople, A. (1973). Masculinity-fEMININITY: An exception to a famous dictum? Psychological Bulletin, 80(5), 389–407. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035334.Search in Google Scholar

Csermely, T., & Rabas, A. (2016). How to reveal people’s preferences: Comparing time consistency and predictive power of multiple price list risk elicitation methods. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 53(2–3), 107–136. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-016-9247-6.Search in Google Scholar

Cukrowska-Torzewska, E., & Lovasz, A. (2020). The role of parenthood in shaping the gender wage gap – a comparative analysis of 26 European countries. Social Science Research, 85(102355), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2019.102355.Search in Google Scholar

Fernández, J., & Coello, M. T. (2010). Do the BSRI and PAQ really measure masculinity and femininity? Spanish Journal of Psychology, 13(2), 1000–1009. https://doi.org/10.1017/S113874160000264XSearch in Google Scholar

Filippin, A., & Crosetto, P. (2016). A reconsideration of gender differences in risk attitudes. Management Science, 62(11), 3138–3160. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2015.2294.Search in Google Scholar

Gupta, V. K., Turban, D. B., Wasti, A. S., & Sikdar, A. (2009). The role of gender stereotypes in perceptions of entrepreneurs and intentions to become an entrepreneur. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice, 33(2), 397–417. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6520.2009.00296.x.Search in Google Scholar

Hoffman, R. M., Hattie, J. A., & Borders, D. L. (2005). Personal definitions of masculinity and femininity as an aspect of gender self-concept. Journal of Humanistic Counseling Education and Development, 44(1), 66–83. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2164-490x.2005.tb00057.x.Search in Google Scholar

Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6), 581–592. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.60.6.581.Search in Google Scholar

Hyde, J. S., Bigler, R. S., Joel, D., Tate, C. C., & van Anders, S. M. (2019). The future of sex and gender in psychology: Five challenges to the gender binary. American Psychologist, 74(2), 171–193. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000307.Search in Google Scholar

Kachel, S., Steffens, M. C., & Niedlich, C. (2016). Traditional masculinity and femininity: Validation of a new scale assessing gender roles. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(JUL), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00956.Search in Google Scholar

Maney, D. L. (2016). Perils and pitfalls of reporting sex differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1688), 1–11 https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0119.Search in Google Scholar

Meier-Pesti, K., & Penz, E. (2008). Sex or gender? Expanding the sex-based view by introducing masculinity and femininity as predictors of financial risk taking. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29(2), 180–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2007.05.002.Search in Google Scholar

Muehlenhard, C. L., & Peterson, Z. D. (2011). Distinguishing between sex and gender: History, current conceptualizations, and implications. Sex Roles, 64(11–12), 791–803. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-9932-5.Search in Google Scholar

Nelson, J. A. (2015). Are women really more risk-averse than men? Journal of Economic Surveys, 29(3), 566–585. https://doi.org/10.1111/joes.12069.Search in Google Scholar

Smiler, A. P., & Epstein, M. (2010). Measuring gender: Options and issues. In J. Chrisler, & D. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology (pp. 133–157).10.1007/978-1-4419-1465-1_7Search in Google Scholar

Smith, P. M., & Koehoorn, M. (2016). Measuring gender when you don’t have a gender measure: Constructing a gender index using survey data. International Journal for Equity in Health, 15(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12939-016-0370-4.Search in Google Scholar

Spence, J. T., & Buckner, C. E. (2000). Instrumental and expressive traits, trait stereotypes, and sexist attitudes: What do they signify? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24(1), 44–62. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb01021.x.Search in Google Scholar

Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. (1978). Masculinity and femininity: Their psychological dimensions, correlates, and antecedents. University of Texas Press.10.1037/h0076857Search in Google Scholar

Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R., & Stapp, J. (1975). Ratings of self and peers on sex role attributes and their relation to self-esteem and conceptions of masculinity and femininity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(1), 29–39. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076857.Search in Google Scholar

Unger, R. K. (1979). Toward a redefinition of sex and gender. American Psychologist, 34(11), 1085–1094. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.34.11.1085.Search in Google Scholar

Westbrook, L., & Saperstein, A. (2015). New categories are not enough: Rethinking the measurement of sex and gender in social surveys. Gender & Society, 29(4), 534–560. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891243215584758.Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2022-01-27
Revised: 2022-10-03
Accepted: 2022-10-18
Published Online: 2022-12-19
Published in Print: 2023-02-23

© 2022 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 7.6.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/humaff-2022-2029/html
Scroll to top button