Skip to main content

Is It Time to Phase Out the Use of All Nonhuman Primates in Invasive Research?

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Handbook of Bioethical Decisions. Volume I

Part of the book series: Collaborative Bioethics ((CB,volume 2))

Abstract

The use of some nonhuman primates in invasive research—unlike that on animals more generally—has been severely restricted or banned in much of the world. This trend toward severe restrictions or bans raises the question: Has the time come to end invasive research with all primates? In this chapter, we offer an overview of the main ethical questions surrounding the use of primates in invasive research, evaluate some of the leading arguments in favor of and against such research, and propose some ethical recommendations for conducting this research. As we argue, the case for phasing out the use of primates in invasive research is not as straightforward as some might think. Stringent restrictions must be adopted if scientifically and ethically justifiable invasive research with primates is to continue.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    We will understand ‘invasive research’ as research that is potentially harmful and not primarily aimed at benefiting the individual animal. Thus, veterinary research that serves a therapeutic purpose, as well as research that is purely observational (e.g., some behavioral studies) will be outside of our scope.

  2. 2.

    A related, but more elaborate view is that great apes possess these capacities to an extent that qualifies them as persons, or what some have called ‘near persons’, or ‘borderline persons’ (Varner, 2012; DeGrazia, 2010).

  3. 3.

    Arguably, some chimpanzees have even met the diagnostic criteria for post-traumatic disorder (Ferdowsian et al., 2011).

  4. 4.

    Most Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (IACUCs) may not actually be prepared for doing such maximally careful analyses (Carbone, 2020, p. 51). The allocation of more resources and the creation of specialized IACUCs to evaluate protocols that involve primates, might be needed to accomplish this goal.

  5. 5.

    Note that moving directly from studies on lower animals (e.g., rodents) to studies on humans—instead of experimenting on primates as an intermediate step—has several drawbacks, including imposing a much greater risk on human research participants. Note, also, that studies may take longer to conduct or could be less controllable than studies on primates. See Phillips et al., 2014; Sughrue et al., 2009; Barnhill et al., 2016.

  6. 6.

    It is controversial whether the harm of premature death should be assessed as ‘severe.’ In the case of great apes, however, studies that resulted in the death or euthanasia of them were forbidden even before research with great apes was phased out. Given the high degree of moral status that primates have, and in particular their capacity for self-consciousness, we believe that a similar position should be taken in the case of primates used in invasive research. But a detailed defense of this point is a project for another time. See McAndrew and Helms (2016).

  7. 7.

    As we explain below, Beauchamp and DeGrazia admit exceptions to this principle. In our view, however, appropriately applying this principle to the case of invasive research with primates would leave outside the scope of ethically permissible invasive research with primates’ studies that involve severe and long-lasting harms.

  8. 8.

    It is worth noting that they do acknowledge that in the case of the principle of Upper Limits to Harm, exceptions correspond to “rare cases of extraordinary urgent social need” (Beauchamp & DeGrazia, 2019, p. 20).

References

  • Abee, C., Adams, K., Amaral, D., Andersen, R., Ator, N., Bayne, K., Beauchamp, T., Bradberrry, C., Brown, P., Carbone, L., Clarke, C., Collins, F. S., DeLong, M., Dennis, J., Fazleabas, A., Flynn, J., Franchini, G., Goldberg, M., Grady, C., Haigwood, N., Hankenson, F. C., Hudson, K., Kahn, J., Kastner, S., Kirk, A., Lifson, K., McNicholl, J., Morrison, J., Meunier, L., Murry, C., Newcomer, C., Newsome, W., Niemi, S., O’Connor, D., Orwig, K., Platt, M., Porrino, L., Prentice, E., Foster, M., Rogers, J., Rosene, D., Schapiro, S., Shenoy, K., Strick, P., Wolinetz, C., Woodruff, T., & Zurlo, J. (2016). NIH workshop on ensuring the continued responsible oversight of research with non-human primates. Resource document. National Institutes of Health (NIH).

    Google Scholar 

  • Aguilera, B., Perez Gomez, J., & DeGrazia, D. (2021). Should biomedical research with great apes be restricted? A systematic review of reasons. BMC Medical Ethics, 22, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., & Gallup, G. G. (2015). Mirror self-recognition: A review and critique of attempts to promote and engineer self-recognition in primates. Primates, 56(4), 317–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnason, G. (2020). The emergence and development of animal research ethics: A review with a focus on nonhuman primates. Science and Engineering Ethics, 26, 2277–2293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnason, G., & Clausen, J. (2016). On balance: Weighing harms and benefits in fundamental neurological research using nonhuman primates. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy, 19, 229–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnhill, A., Joffe, S., & Miller, F. G. (2016). The ethics of infection challenges in primates. Hastings Center Report, 46, 20–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchamp, T., & DeGrazia, D. (2019). Principles of animal research ethics. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beauchamp, T., & Wobber, V. (2014). Autonomy in chimpanzees. Theoretical Medicine and Bioethics, 35(2), 117–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchamp, T., Capitanio, J., Carbone, L., Conlee, K., Gruen, L., Hudson, K., Hutchinson, E., Kahn, J., Kordower, J., Landi, M., Peña-Guzmán, D., Redmond, E., Richmond, J., Schapiro, S., Tennenbaum, J., Zurio, J. (2017). The necessity of the use of non-human primate models in research. Resource document. Johns Hopkins Berman Institute of Bioethics. https://speakingofresearch.files.wordpress.com/2017/04/animal-working-group-meeting-1-briefing-book.pdf

  • Begley, C. G., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2015). Reproducibility in science. Improving the standard for basic and preclinical research. Circulation Research, 166, 116–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhogal, N., Hudson, M., Balls, M., & Combes, R. D. (2005). The use of non-human primates in biological and medical research: Evidence submitted by FRAME to the Academy of Medical Sciences/Medical Research Council/Royal Society/Wellcome Trust working group. Alternatives to Laboratory Animals: ATLA, 33, 519–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bobrowicz, K., Johansson, M., & Osvath, M. (2020). Great apes selectively retrieve relevant memories to guide action. Scientific Reports, 10, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, A., Mennie, N., Bibby, P. A., & Cassaday, H. J. (2020). Some animals are more equal than others: Validation of a new scale to measure how attitudes to animals depend on species and human purpose of use. PLoS One, 15, 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buffalo, E. A., Movshon, J. A., & Wurtz, R. H. (2019). From basic brain research to treating human brain disorders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116, 26167–26172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carbone, L. (2020). The potential and impacts of practical application of Beauchamp and DeGrazia’s six principles. In T. Beauchamp & D. DeGrazia (Eds.), Principles of animal research ethics (pp. 45–60). Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Carvalho, C., Gaspar, A., Knight, A., & Vicente, L. (2018). Ethical and scientific pitfalls concerning laboratory research with non-human primates, and possible solutions. Animals, 9, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cauvin, A. J., Peters, C., & Brennan, F. (2015). Advantages and limitations of commonly used nonhuman primate species in Research and Development of biopharmaceuticals. In J. Bluemel, S. Korte, E. Schenck, & G. F. Weinbauer (Eds.), The nonhuman primate in nonclinical drug development and safety assessment (pp. 379–395). Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chang, M. C., Hild, S., & Grieder, F. (2021). Nonhuman primate models for SARS-CoV-2 research: Consider alternatives to macaques. Laboratory Animals, 50, 113–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conlee, K. M., & Rowan, A. N. (2012). The case for phasing out experiments on primates. The Hastings Center Report, 31, 20–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F. (2019). Fish, mirrors, and a gradualist perspective on self-awareness. PLoS Biology, 17, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeGrazia, D. (2010). Great apes, dolphins, and the concept of personhood. The Southern Journal of Philosophy, 35, 301–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Directive. (2010). Of the European Parliament and of the council of 22 September 2010 on the protection of animals used for scientific purposes. Resource Document. https://eur-lex.europa.eu/legal-content/EN/TXT/PDF/?uri=CELEX:32010L0063&from=EN

  • European Commission. (2017). The need for non-human primates in biomedical research, production and testing of products and devices. https://ec.europa.eu/health/scientific_committees/non-human-primates-testing-2-background_en#fragment

  • Fanelli, D. (2018). Is science really facing a reproducibility crisis? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115, 2628–2631.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feng, G., Jensen, F. E., Greely, H. T., Okano, H., Treue, S., Roberts, A. C., Fox, J. G., Caddick, S., Poo, M., Newsome, W. T., & Morrison, J. H. (2020). Opportunities and limitations of genetically modified nonhuman primate models for neuroscience research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117, 24022–24031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenton, A. (2012). On the need to redress an inadequacy in animal welfare science: Toward an internally coherent framework. Biology and Philosophy, 27(1), 73–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferdowsian, H. R., Durham, D. L., Kimwele, C., Kranendonk, G., Otali, E., Akugizibwe, T., Mulcahy, J. B., Ajarova, L., & Meré, C. (2011). Signs of mood and anxiety disorders in chimpanzees. PLoS One, 6, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferdowsian, H., Johnson, L., Johnson, J., Fenton, A., Shriver, A., & Gluck, J. (2020). A Belmont report for animals? Cambridge Journal of Healthcare Ethics, 29, 19–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, H., Ator, N., Haigwood, N., Newsome, W., Allan, J. S., Golos, T. G., Kordower, J. H., Shade, R. E., Goldberg, M. E., Bailey, M. R., & Bianchi, P. (2017). The critical role of nonhuman primates in medical research. Pathogens & Immunity, 2, 352–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, M. B., & Luciw, P. A. (2008). Macaque models of human infectious disease. ILAR Journal, 49, 220–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grimm, D. (2015). Decision to end monkey experiments based on finances, not animal rights, NIH says. Science. https://www.sciencemag.org/news/2015/12/decision-end-monkey-experiments-based-finances-not-animal-rights-nih-says

  • Grimm, D. (2018). Record number of monkeys being used in U.S. research. Science. https://www.sciencemag.org/news/2018/11/record-number-monkeys-being-used-us-research

  • Grimm, H., Olsson, I. A. S., & Sandøe, P. (2019). Harm–benefit analysis—What is the added value? A review of alternative strategies for weighing harms and benefits as part of the assessment of animal research. Laboratory Animals, 53, 17–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Honess, P. E., Johnson, P. J., & Wolfensohn, S. E. (2004). A study of behavioural responses of non-human primates to air transport and re-housing. Laboratory Animals, 38, 119–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaworska, A., & Tannenbaum, J. (2021). The grounds of moral status. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Metaphysics Research Lab. http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2013/entries/grounds-moral-status/

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagira, J. M., Ngotho, M., Thuita, J. K., Maina, N. W., & Hau, J. (2007). Hematological changes in vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops) during eight months’ adaptation to captivity. American Journal of Primatology, 69, 1053–1063.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, J. (2015). NIH to end all support for chimpanzee research. Science. https://www.sciencemag.org/news/2015/11/nih-end-all-support-chimpanzee-research, 350, 1013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krupenye, C., & Call, J. (2019). Theory of mind in animals: Current and future directions. WIREs Cognitive Science, 10, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lilley, E., Armstrong, R., Clark, N., Gray, P., Hawkins, P., Mason, K., López-Salesansky, N., Stark, A., Jackson, S. K., Thiemermann, C., & Nandi, M. (2015). Refinement of animal models of sepsis and septic shock. Shock, 43, 304–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsuzawa, T. (2016). SAGA and GAIN for great apes. Primates, 57, 1–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAndrew, R., & Helms, S. I. (2016). Laboratory primates: Their lives in and after research. Temperature, 3, 502–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, R., Boeckle, M., Jelbert, S. A., Frohnwieser, A., Wascher, C. A. F., & Clayton, N. S. (2019). Self-control in crows, parrots and nonhuman primates. WIREs Cognitive Science, 10, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, A. S., Hartig, R., Basso, M. A., Jarrett, W., Kastner, S., & Poirier, C. (2021). International primate neuroscience research regulation, public engagement and transparency opportunities. NeuroImage, 229, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakayama, E., & Saijo, M. (2013). Animal models for Ebola and Marburg virus infections. Frontiers in Microbiology, 4, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Institutes of Health. (2021). Office of Laboratory Animal Welfare Vertebrate Animals Section. https://olaw.nih.gov/guidance/vertebrate-animal-section.htm

  • Ogden, B. E., Wanyong, W., Agui, T., & Han, B. (2016). Laboratory animal Laws, regulations, guidelines and standards in China mainland, Japan, and Korea. ILAR Journal, 57, 301–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, K. A., Bales, K. L., Capitanio, J. P., Conley, A., Czoty, P. W., ‘t Hart, B. A., Hopkins, W. D., Hu, S. L., Miller, L. A., Nader, M. A., Nathanielsz, P. W., Rogers, J., Shively, C. A., & Voytko, M. L. (2014). Why primate models matter. American Journal of Primatology, 76, 801–827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prince, A. M., Goodall, J., Brotman, B., Dienske, H., Schellekens, H., & Eichberg, J. W. (1989). Appropriate conditions for maintenance of chimpanzees in studies with blood-borne viruses: An epidemiologic and psychosocial perspective. Journal of Medical Primatology, 18, 27–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reardon, S. (2019, May 9). US lawmakers propose plan to reduce primate research at National Institutes of Health. Nature. https://www.nature.com/articles/d41586-019-01511-0

  • Reynolds, V. (1995). Moral issues in relation to chimpanzees field studies and experiments. Alternatives to Laboratory Animals, 23(5), 621–625. 02300512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, W. M. S., & Burch, R. L. (1959). The principles of humane experimental technique. Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuppli, C. A., Fraser, D., & McDonald, M. (2004). Expanding the three Rs to meet new challenges in humane animal experimentation. Alternatives to Laboratory Animals, 32, 525–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scientific Committee on Health and Environmental Risks. (2009). The need for non-human primates in biomedical research, production and testing of products and devices. Resource document. SCHER. https://ec.europa.eu/health/ph_risk/committees/04_scher/docs/scher_o_110.pdf

  • Scientific Committee on Health, Environmental and Emerging Risks. (2017). The need for non-human primates in biomedical research, production and testing of products and devices (update 2017). Resource document. SCHEER. https://ec.europa.eu/health/scientific_committees/non-human-primates-testing

  • Shanks, N., & Greek, R. (2008). Experimental use of nonhuman primates is not a simple problem. Nature Medicine, 14, 1012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, D., Anderson, D., Degryse, A. D., Bol, C., Criado, A., Ferrara, A., et al. (2018). Classification and reporting of severity experienced by animals used in scientific procedures: FELASA/ECLAM/ESLAV working group report. Laboratory Animals, 52(1_suppl), 5–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strech, D., & Dirnagl, U. (2019). 3Rs missing: Animal research without scientific value is unethical. BMJ Open Science, 3, 1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sughrue, M. E., Mocco, J., Mack, W. J., Ducruet, A. F., Komotar, R. J., Fischbach, R. L., Martin, T. E., & Connolly, E. S. (2009). Bioethical considerations in translational research: Primate stroke. The American Journal of Bioethics: AJOB, 9, 3–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, T., Wu, J., Si, C., Dai, S., Zhang, Y., Sun, N., Zhang, E., Shao, H., Si, W., Yang, P., Wang, H., Chen, Z., Zhu, R., Kang, Y., Hernandez-Benitez, R., Martinez, L., Nuñez, E., Berggren, W. T., Schwarz, M., Ai, Z., Li, T., Deng, H., Rodriguez, C., Ji, W., Niu, Y., & Izpisua, J. C. (2021). Chimeric contribution of human extended pluripotent stem cells to monkey embryos ex vivo. Cell, 184, 2020–2032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tannenbaum, J., & Bennett, B. T. (2015). Russell and Burch’s 3Rs then and now: The need for clarity in definition and purpose. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science: JAALAS, 54(2), 120–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, K., & Alvarez, L. R. (2019). An estimate of the number of animals used for scientific purposes worldwide in 2015. Alternatives to laboratory animals: ATLA, 47, 196–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thew, M. (2012, April 11). Primate studies: Trials don’t always translate. Nature, 167.

    Google Scholar 

  • US Department of Agriculture. (2021). Research Facility Annual Summary & Archive Reports. https://www.aphis.usda.gov/aphis/ourfocus/animalwelfare/sa_obtain_research_facility_annual_report/ct_research_facility_annual_summary_reports

  • Varner, G. (2012). Personhood, ethics, and animal cognition: Situating animals in Hare’s two level utilitarianism. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, R. L. (2016). Beyond primates: Research protections and animal moral value. Hastings Center Report, 46, 28–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Würbel, H. (2017). Importance of scientific validity for harm-benefit analysis of animal research. Laboratory Animals, 46(4), 164–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, X. L., Pang, W., Hu, X. T., Li, J. L., Yao, Y. G., & Zheng, Y. T. (2014). Experimental primates and non-human primate (NHP) models of human diseases in China: Current status and progress. Dong wu xue yan jiu = Zoological research, 35, 447–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, K. (2018). As primate research drops in Europe, Overseas Options Appeal. The Scientist. https://www.the-scientist.com/news-opinion/as-primate-research-drops-in-europe%2D%2Doverseas-options-appeal-64668

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Javiera Farga for her help during the preparation of this manuscript. This work was partly supported by the Chilean National Agency for Research and Development (ANID) under Grant FONDECYT 11200897.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bernardo Aguilera .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2023 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Aguilera, B., Perez Gomez, J. (2023). Is It Time to Phase Out the Use of All Nonhuman Primates in Invasive Research?. In: Valdés, E., Lecaros, J.A. (eds) Handbook of Bioethical Decisions. Volume I. Collaborative Bioethics, vol 2. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-29451-8_32

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics