Skip to main content
Log in

Perceived Access to Self-relevant Information Mediates Judgments of Privacy Violations in Neuromonitoring and Other Monitoring Technologies

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Neuroethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Advances in technology are bringing greater insight into the mind, raising a host of privacy concerns. However, the basic psychological mechanisms underlying the perception of privacy violations are poorly understood. Here, we explore the relation between the perception of privacy violations and access to information related to one’s “self.” In two studies using demographically diverse samples, we find that privacy violations resulting from various monitoring technologies are mediated by the extent to which the monitoring is thought to provide access to self-relevant information, and generally neuromonitoring did not rate among the more invasive monitoring types. However, brain monitoring was judged to be more of a privacy violation when described as providing access to self-relevant information than when no such access was possible, and control participants did not judge the invasiveness of neuromonitoring any differently than those told it provided no access to self-relevant information.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A principal axis factor analysis extracted a single factor, accounting for 63.71 % of the overall variability in the nine items. The resulting composite factor score represents a single measure of the extent to which participants believe that a monitoring type provides access to self-related information.

  2. We were able to track timing data for Experiment 2 therefore participants who completed the experiment in fewer than 240 s (a pre-determined minimum time cutoff based on pilot testing) were excluded from our analysis.

  3. Prior to our manipulation participants were administered a brief pretest measure of preconceived notions about the capabilities of current neuromonitoring technology. A box-plot of this pretest suggested that participants were less likely to believe that neuromoniting technology has the capability to monitor self-related information--such as likes or dislikes--than they were to believe physiological information--such as signs of disease--could be monitored. (A repeated-measures ANOVA on these pretest items is not particularly useful as the large sample size resulted in nearly every item being significantly different from every other item.)

References

  1. A.H., et al. v. Northside Independent School District, et al. 5:12-cv-01113-LOG US (Western District of Texas San Antonio Division, 2013).

  2. Moore, A.D. 2003. Privacy: its meaning and value. American Philosophical Quarterly 40: 215–227.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Parent, W.A. 1983. Privacy, Morality, and the Law. Philosophy and Public Affairs 12: 269–288.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Parker, R.B. 1973. A definition of privacy. Rutgers Law Review 27: 275–296.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Nowak, A., R.R. Vallacher, A. Tesser, and W. Borkowski. 2000. Society of self: the emergence of collective properties in self-structure. Psychological Review 107: 39–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Leary MR, Tangney JP. 2005. Handbook of self and identity. In eds. Leary MR, Tangney JP, New York: Guilford Press.

  7. Andersen, S.M., and L. Ross. 1984. Self-knowledge and social inference: I. The impact of cognitive/affective and behavioral data. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46: 280–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Andersen, S.M. 2000. Self-knowledge and social inference: II. The diagnosticity of cognitive/affective and behavioral data. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78: 772–790.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Risko, E.F., and A. Kingstone. 2011. Eyes wide shut: implied social presence, eye tracking and attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 73: 291–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Parens E, Johnston J. 2007. Does it make sense to speak of neuroethics? Three problems with keying ethics to hot new science and technology. EMBO reports 8 Spec No: S61–4. Available: doi:10.1038/sj.embor.7400992. Accessed 6 July 2012.

  11. Garland, B., and P.W. Glimcher. 2006. Cognitive neuroscience and the law. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 16: 130–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Wilfond BS, Ravitsky V. 2005. On the proliferation of bioethics sub-disciplines: do we really need “genethics” and “neuroethics”? The American Journal of Bioethics 5: 20–1; discussion W: 3–4. Available: doi:10.1080/15265160590960924. Accessed 16 June 2012.

  13. Schweitzer, N.J., M.J. Saks, E.R. Murphy, A.L. Roskies, W. Sinnott-Armstrong, et al. 2011. Neuroimages as evidence in a mens rea defense: no impact. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law 17: 357–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Farah, M.J., M.E. Smith, C. Gawuga, D. Lindsell, and D. Foster. 2009. Brain imaging and brain privacy: a realistic concern? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 325: 119–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Rand, D.G. 2012. The promise of mechanical turk: how online labor markets can help theorists run behavioral experiments. Journal of Theoretical Biology 299: 172–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Buhrmester, M., T. Kwang, and S.D. Gosling. 2011. Amazon’s mechanical turk: a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science 6: 3–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Paolacci, G., J. Chandler, and P.G. Ipeirotis. 2010. Running experiments on amazon mechanical turk. Judgment and Decision Making 5: 411–419.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Olson, S. 2005. Brain scans raise privacy concerns. Science 307: 1548–1550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Räikkä, J. 2010. Brain imaging and privacy. Neuroethics 3: 5–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Leggett H. 2009. The next hacking frontier: Your brain? Wired.com. Available: http://www.wired.com/wiredscience/2009/07/neurosecurity/. Accessed 10 July 2012.

  21. Wardlaw JM, O’Connell G, Shuler K, DeWilde J, Haley J, et al. 2011. “Can it read my mind?” - What do the public and experts think of the current (mis)uses of neuroimaging? PloS one 6: e25829. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3186771&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. Accessed 27 March 2012.

  22. Covin R. 2012. Mind-reading advance lets brain scientists ‘eavesdrop’ on thoughts. Huffington Post.com. Available: http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2012/02/01/mind-reading-breakthrough_n_1246752.html. Accessed 8 August 2012.

  23. Duncan DE. 2012, April 3. iBrain, a device that can read thoughts—NYTimes.com. The New York Times D2.

  24. Paul, A. M. March 18, 2012. Your brain on fiction. The New York times SR6. Available http://www.nytimes.com/2012/03/18/opinion/sunday/the-neuroscience-of-your-brain-on-fiction.html?pagewanted=al. Accessed 6 July 2012

  25. Venkataramanan, M. 2012. Brain-reading bike helmet shows how stress you are. Wired.com Available: http://www.wired.com/playbook/2012/11/mit-media-lab-bike-helmet/. Accessed 11 November 2012.

  26. Bloom, P., and D.S. Weisberg. 2007. Childhood origins of adult resistance to science. Science 316: 996–997.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Johnson, C.N. 2008. If you had my brain, where would I be? Children’s understanding of the brain and identity. Child Development 61: 962–972.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Miresco, M.J., and L. Kirmayer. 2006. The persistence of mind-brain dualism in psychiatric reasoning about clinical scenarios. The American Journal of Psychiatry 163: 913–918.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to D. A. Baker.

Appendix A

Appendix A

List of 15 possible monitoring types from Experiment 1

As part of a study, researchers have developed a system for monitoring…

  1. 1.

    a person’s heart activity in a way that would provide them with a constant record of the person’s heart rate.

  2. 2.

    a person’s blink activity in a way that would provide them with a constant record of the person’s blink rate.

  3. 3.

    a person’s internet activity in a way that would provide them with a constant record of the person’s internet usage.

  4. 4.

    a person’s head orientation in a way that would provide them with a constant record of the direction a person’s head is facing.

  5. 5.

    a person’s spending activity in a way that would provide them with a constant record of the person’s purchase history.

  6. 6.

    a person’s movement in a way that would provide them with a constant record of how a person is moving their body.

  7. 7.

    a person’s gaze orientation in a way that would provide them with a constant record of the direction a person’s eyes are facing.

  8. 8.

    a person’s gaze orientation in a way that would provide them with a constant visual record of what the person is looking at.

  9. 9.

    a person’s written communication in a way that would provide them with a constant record of anything the person’s writes or types.

  10. 10.

    a person’s respiratory activity in a way that would provide them with a constant record of the person’s breathing rate.

  11. 11.

    a person’s body orientation in a way that would provide them with a constant record of the direction a person’s body is facing.

  12. 12.

    a person’s brain activity in a way that would provide them with a constant record of which areas of the brain are active.

  13. 13.

    a person’s speech in a way that would provide them with a constant record of anything the person’s says aloud.

  14. 14.

    a person’s geographical position in a way that would provide them with a constant record of where a person is physically located.

  15. 15.

    a person’s muscle activity in a way that would provide them with a constant record of which muscles a person is using.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Baker, D.A., Schweitzer, N.J. & Risko, E.F. Perceived Access to Self-relevant Information Mediates Judgments of Privacy Violations in Neuromonitoring and Other Monitoring Technologies. Neuroethics 7, 43–50 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-013-9181-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-013-9181-0

Keywords

Navigation