Skip to main content
Log in

Connecting emotions and words: the referential process

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper outlines the process of verbal communication of emotion as this occurs through the phases of the referential process, including arousal of an emotion schema; detailed and specific descriptions of images and episodes that are exemplars of emotion schemas; and reflection and reorganization, which may include emotion labels and other types of categorical terms. The concepts of emotion schemas and the referential process are defined in the theoretical framework of multiple code theory which includes subsymbolic sensory, visceral and motoric processes, symbolic images and words. Emotion schemas are defined as clusters of representations of events incorporating similar bodily, sensory and motoric processes activated in relation to different people in different contexts. Through the referential process subsymbolic components of a schema that have been activated in a speaker or writer and that may be connected only partially to words may be evoked in a listener or reader. The concept of the emotion schemas is examined in relation to current work in emotion theory and neuroscience. The unique effects of detailed descriptions of episodes in conveying complex aspects of emotional experience are discussed, as recognized by writers, and as demonstrated in empirical research. Computerized measures of the phases of the referential process are presented, focusing particularly on the central measure, the Weighted Referential Activity Dictionary (WRAD) which identifies points of narrative and imagery. The operation of the function words that dominate the WRAD are examined in relation to the structure of narrative expression underlying the verbal representation of emotion schemas.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Lyrics of popular song by M. Gordon, 1945; information downloaded from wikipedia.org, Sept. 5, 2013.

  2. But see Ramachandran 2004, Chapter 4, pp. 60–82, for discussion of the relation between the sounds of speech and meaning; this relationship provides an additional perspective on the connection of emotional experience to language.

  3. The term ‘subsymbolic’, was taken from connectionist and Parallel Distributed Processing approaches and is used here to mean nonsymbolic, not as characterizing this form as less systematic or complex than symbolic forms.

  4. Geneva Appraisal Questionnaire (GAQ): Format, development, and utilization; http://www.affective-sciences.org/researchmaterial. Also see Scherer (2001)

  5. For each such measure the DAAP program constructs a visually smooth curve with a value at each word; this curve is relatively high when there are a relatively greater number of nearby words with the corresponding feature.

  6. In all therapy excerpts, any identifying information has been changed for purposes of confidentiality.

  7. The English language versions of DAAP and WRAD, along with other dictionaries currently in use, can be downloaded from the website: www.thereferentialprocess.org.

  8. The weights are reported as lying between −1 and +1 in Bucci and Maskit (2006), but in practice they are linearly rescaled to lie between 0 and 1.

  9. The full list of WRAD words in alphabetical order with their dictionary weights can be found in Bucci & Maskit (2014).

  10. The DAAP software transforms most filled pauses, such as ‘uhm’ or ‘hm’, to the word ‘mm’.

References

  • Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychological Science, 19, 33–41. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02043.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, M. W. (1992). Relational schemas and the processing of social information. Psychological Bulletin, 112(3), 461–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckett, S. (1958). The unnameable in S. Beckett three novels. New York: Grove.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., Bertenthal, B. I., Hoerger, M., & Carr, T. H. (2008). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14(4), 340–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brand, B. L., Lanius, R., Vermetten, E., Loewenstein, R. J., & Spiegel, D. (2012). Where are we going? an update on assessment, treatment and neurobiological research in dissociative disorders as we move toward the DSM-5. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 13(1), 9–31. doi:10.1080/15299732.2011.620687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I. (1994). Infants’ subjective world of relatedness: moments, feeling shapes, protonarrative envelopes, and internal working models. Infant Mental Health Journal, 15(1), 36–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bucci, W. (1984). Linking words and things: basic processes and individual variation. Cognition, 17, 137–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science: A multiple code theory. NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bucci, W. (2002). The referential process consciousness, and the sense of self. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 22, 766–793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bucci, W. (2011). The role of embodied communication in therapeutic change: A multiple code perspective. In W. Tschacher & C. Bergomi (Eds.), The implications of embodiment: Cognition and communication (pp. 209–228). Exeter: Imprint Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bucci, W., & Freedman, N. (1978). Language and hand: the dimension of referential competence. Journal of Personality, 46, 594–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bucci, W. and Kabasakalian, R. (1992). Instructions for scoring Referential Activity (RA) in transcripts of spoken narrative texts. Ulm, Germany; Ulmer Textbank.

  • Bucci, W., & Maskit, B. (2006). A weighted dictionary for referential activity. In J. G. Shanahan, Y. Qu, & J. Wiebe (Eds.), Computing attitude and affect in text (pp. 49–60). The Netherlands: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bucci, W., & Maskit, B. (2007). Beneath the surface of the therapeutic interaction; the psychoanalytic method in modern dress. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 55, 1355–1397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bucci, W. and McKay, R.K. (2014): Manual for Scoring RA Scales. (Original publication, 1992) figshare. doi10.6084/m9.figshare.962956

  • Corballis, M. S. (2009). Mental time travel and the shaping of language. Experimental Brain Research, 192, 553–560. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1491-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csíkszentmihályi, M. (1990). Flow: the psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, H., Kächele, H., & Thomä, H. (Eds.). (1988). Psychoanalytic process research strategies. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow and the feeling brain. New York: Harcourt, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deonna, J. A., & Scherer, K. R. (2010). The case of the disappearing intentional object: constraints on a definition of emotion. Emotion Review, 2(1), 44–52. doi:10.1177/1754073909345544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Didion, J. (1998). Last words. The New Yorker, 9, 74–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eliot, T.S. (1950) Hamlet and His Problems. In T.S. Eliot The Sacred Wood., (pp. 95–103), London: Methuen & Co. Ltd. (Original work published 1920)

  • Evans, J., & St, B. T. (2007). Hypothetical thinking: Dual processes in reasoning and judgment. England: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fainsilber, L., & Ortony, A. (1987). Metaphorical uses of language in the expression of emotions. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 2(4), 239–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E. B., Zoellner, L. A., Feeny, N. C., Hembree, E. A., & Alvarez–Conrad, J. (2002). Does imaginal exposure exacerbate PTSD symptoms? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 1022–1028. doi:10.1037//0022-006X.70.4.1022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fontaine, Johnny R. J.; Scherer, Klaus R.; Soriano, Cristiana (2013). Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (Series in Affective Science), Oxford University Press, USA. Kindle Edition.)

  • Hadamard, J. (1996) The mathematician’s mind: The psychology of invention in the mathematical field. New York: Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Copyright 1945, Princeton University Press)

  • Halberstadt, J. (2005) Language, emotion attribution, and emotional experience. Psychological Inquiry, 16 (1), pp. 1821. doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli1601_02

  • Hanakawa, Y. (2004) Comparison of linguistic and acoustic analyses of schizophrenic language. (Doctoral Dissertation) Retrieved from ProQuest (314887) .

  • Hupka, R. B., Lenton, A. P., & Hutchison, K. S. (1999). Universal development of emotion categories in natural language. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 247–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E., King, K. A., Trentacosta, C. J., Laurenceau, J. P., Morgan, J. K., Krauthamer-Ewing, E. S., et al. (2008). Accelerating the development of emotion competence in head start children. Development & Psychopathology, 20, 369–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Klein, G. (2009). Conditions for intuitive expertise: a failure to disagree. American Psychologist, 64(6), 515–526. doi:10.1037/a0016755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kingsley, G. (2009). The clinical validation of measures of the referential process. (Doctoral dissertation) Retrieved from ProQuest. (AAT 3377938).

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1987). Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: a computational approach. Psychological Review, 94(2), 148–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W. (1997). Some further steps in narrative analysis. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 7, 395–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: oral versions of personal experience. In J. Helm (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts (pp. 12–44). Seattle: University of Washington Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, S. (1942) Philosophy in a new key. New York, NY, Penguin

  • Lanius, R. A., Bluhm, R., Lanius, U., & Pain, C. (2006). A review of neuroimaging studies in PTSD: heterogeneity of response to symptom provocation. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40, 709–729. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.07.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lanius, R. A., Vermetten, E., Loewenstein, R.J., Brand, B., Schmahl, C., Bremner. J.D., & Spiegel, D. (2009) American Journal of Psychiatry in Advance, 1–8. DOI:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09081168).

  • LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Touchstone.

  • Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 677–689. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.17.4.677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J., Tom, S. M., Pfeifer, J. H., & Way, B. M. (2007). Putting feelings into words: affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Psychological Science, 18(5), 421–428. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01916.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mariani, R., Maskit, B., Bucci, W., & DeCoro, A. (2013). Linguistic measures of the referential process in psychodynamic treatment: The English and Italian versions. Psychotherapy Research, 23(4), 430–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maskit, B. (2014) The Discourse Attributes Analysis Program (DAAP) Operating Instructions. Figshare. doi10.6084/m9.figshare.947740

  • Mergenthaler, E., & Bucci, W. (1999). Linking verbal and nonverbal representations: computer analysis of referential activity. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 72, 339–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, K. (1992) An application of linguistic narrative analysis to psychoanalytic process research. (Doctoral dissertation) Retrieved from ProQuest. (9217759).

  • Mulligan, K., & Scherer, K. R. (2012). Toward a working definition of emotion. Emotion Review, 4(4), 345–347. doi:10.1177/1754073912445818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, S. M. (2012) Components of the referential process as measures of therapeutic change: Development of normative and psychometric properties, Adelphi University, Institute of Advanced Psychological Studies. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses, 112.

  • Nelson, K. L. (2010, June). The relevance of time and place coding in language to psychotherapy process research. Paper presented at the 41st International Annual Meeting of The Society for Psychotherapy Research, Asilomar, CA.

  • Nelson, K. L., Moskovitz, D. J., & Steiner, H. (2008). Narration and vividness as measures of event-specificity in autobiographical memory. Discourse Processes, 45, 195–209. doi:10.1080/01638530701792891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D.A. (1986) Reflections on cognition and parallel distributed processing. In Rumelhart, D.E., McClelland, J.L., & the PDP Research Group, Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. (pp. 531-546) Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.

  • Ogarkova, A. (2013) Folk emotion concepts: Lexicalization of emotional experiences across languages and cultures. In Fontaine, Johnny R. J.; Scherer, Klaus R.; Soriano, Cristiana (2013) Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (Series in Affective Science), Oxford University Press, USA. Kindle Edition.)

  • Ogden, P., Pain, C., & Fisher, J. (2006). A sensorimotor approach to the treatment of trauma and dissociation. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 29, 263–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A., Clark, J. M., Digdon, N., & Bons, T. (1989). Referential processing: reciprocity and correlates of naming and imaging. Memory and Cognition, 17(2), 163–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature, 9, 148–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. A. (2006). Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdala. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 27–53. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S. (2004). A brief tour of human consciousness Pearson education. New York: Inc. Pi Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reisenzein, R. (2000). Exploring the strength of association between the components of emotion syndromes: the case of surprise. Cognition and Emotion, 14(1), 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, T.B. (1981) A model of the self as an aspect of the human information processing system. In N. Cantor & J.F. Kihlstrom (Eds.), Personality, cognition and social interaction (pp. 193–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Ross, B. H., & Makin, V. S. (1999). Prototype versus exemplar models in cognition. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of cognition (pp. 205–241). Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roussos, A., Etchebarne, I., & Waizmann, V. (2009). The interaction between psychoanalytic interpretations and referential activity (s-wrad): A single case study. Anu. Investig. [online], 16, 95–102.

  • Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–172. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samstag, N. (1996) A meta-analysis of referential activity (Doctoral Dissertation) Retrieved from ProQuest (9705478).

  • Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multi-level sequential checking. In K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 92–120). New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (2005). What are emotions? and how can they be measured? social science information, 44 (4), (695–729), London, Thousand Oaks (pp. 0539–0184). New Delhi: SAGE Publications. doi:10.1177/0539018405058216 058216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (1988). Structure and controlling subsymbolic processing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 51–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schottenbauer, M. A., Glass, C. R., Arnkoff, D. B., Tendick, V., & Gray, S. H. (2008). Nonresponse and dropout rates in outcome studies on PTSD: review and methodological considerations. Psychiatry, 71(2), 134–168. doi:10.1521/psyc.2008.71.2.134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. T., & Smith, K. D. (1995). Turkish emotion concepts: A prototype analysisi. In J. S. Russell, J.-M. Fernandez-Dols, A. S. Manstead, & J. C. Wellenkamp (Eds.), Everyday conceptions of emotion: An introduction to the psychology, anthropology, and linguistics of emotion (pp. 103–120). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Solano, L. (2001). Tra mente e corpo. Milano: Raffaello Cortina Editore.

  • Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. NY: Basic Books, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (1994). One way to build a clinically relevant baby. Infant Mental Health Journal, 15(1), 9–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, V. (2006). The moral dimensions of human social intelligence: Domain-specific and domain-general mechanisms. Philosophical Explorations, 9(1), 55–68. doi:10.1080/13869790500492516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299–351. doi:10.1017/S0140525X07001975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabibnia, G., Lieberman, M. D., & Craske, M. G. (2008). The lasting effect of words on feelings: words may facilitate exposure effects to threatening images. Emotion, 8, 307–317. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tattersall, I. (1998). Becoming human: Evolution and human uniqueness. New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todorov, E. (2004). Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nature Neuroscience, 7(9), 907–915. doi:10.1038/nn1309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolstoy, L. (2000) Anna Karenina (R. Pevear & L. Volokhonsky, Trans.) New York: Penguin Books (Original work published 1878)

  • Treisman, A. M. (1964). Selective attention in man. British Medical Bulletin, 20, 12–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and Language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press (Original work published 1934).

  • Wechsler, D. (1955). Manual for the wechsler adult intelligence scale. New York: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, M. (2009) The emotional effects of storytelling. (Doctoral Dissertation) Retrieved from ProQuest (3394526)

  • Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotional universaals. Language Design, 2, 23–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., Simmons, W. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2011). Grounding emotion in situated conceptualization. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1105–1127. doi:10.1016/2010.12.032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson-Mendenhall, C.D., Barrett, L.F., & Barsalou, L.W. (2013) Neural evidence that human emotions share core affective properties. Psychological Science (on line) doi: 10.1177/0956797612464242

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wilma Bucci.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bucci, W., Maskit, B. & Murphy, S. Connecting emotions and words: the referential process. Phenom Cogn Sci 15, 359–383 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-015-9417-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-015-9417-z

Keywords

Navigation