Skip to main content
Log in

“Semiotic Canalization”: a Process Directing the Use and Interpretation of Signals in Animal Interactions?

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Biosemiotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

C. S. Peirce defined the sign as a means to communicate a form or habit embodied in the object to the interpretant, thus constraining (through a sign) the behavior of an interpreter to a limited series of effects. This is part of the process of “semiotic scaffolding” in which sign relations interlock and reinforce one another, providing directionality to the process. In biological evolutionary studies canalization is defined as the adjustment of developmental pathways by natural selection to bring about a uniform result despite genetic and environmental variations. Establishing parallelism between the two concepts allows the possibility of understanding how semiotic processes introduce information from the environment resulting in canalization (and assimilation), determining “fixed” pathways to elaborate signal repertoires for particular species adapted to particular environments sensed by particular organs. This kind of sign action can produce an “encapsulation” process of the sign meaning, further affecting the meaning-making process developing in the receiver, a process affecting signal usage by animals, constraining agency of biological systems’ behavior towards a definite sequence of events or signals and providing a certain orientation of the process (“semiotic canalization”). Therefore, we can also say that signs could potentially be built by senders (i.e. by the sign-vehicle) in a sort of pre-determined way and not extracted by the receiver from a “neutral” sender. Semiotic canalization is seen as a part of a more general process of scaffolding during development, acting mainly during some information exchanges between communicating subjects, providing adaptive responses to some standard communicative situations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andrade, E. (2007). A semiotic framework for evolutionary and developmental biology. BioSystems, 90, 389–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury, J. W., & Vehrencamp, S. L. (2011). Principles of animal communication. 2nd Edition. London: Sinauer.

  • Bruni, L. E. (2008). Cellular semiotics and signal transduction. In M. Barbieri (Ed.), Introduction to biosemiotics (pp. 365–407). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bundgaard, P. F. (2007). The cognitive import of the narrative schema. Semiotica, 165, 247–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, D. (2007). Semiotics. The basics. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dor, D., & Jablonka, E. (2010). Plasticity and canalization in th evolution of linguistic communication: an evolutionary-developmental approach. In R. K. Larson, V. Déprez & H. Yamakido (Eds.), The evolution of human language: Biolinguistic perspectives (pp. 135–147). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • El-Hani, C. N., Queiroz, J., & Stjernfelt, F. (2010). Firefly femmes fatales: A case study in the semiotics of deception. Biosemiotics, 3, 33–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emmeche, C. (2015). Semiotic scaffolding of the social self in reflexivity and friendship. Biosemiotics, 8, 275–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Favareau, D. (2015). Symbols are grounded not in things, but in scaffolded relations and their semiotic constraints (or how the referential generality of symbol scaffolding grows mind). Biosemiotics, 8, 235–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, J. (2011). Where is the information in animal communication? In R. Menzel & J. Fischer (Eds.), Animal thinking (pp. 151–161). Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francescoli, G. (1999). A preliminary report on the acoustic communication in uruguayan Ctenomys (Rodentia, Octodontidae): basic sound types. Bioacoustics, 10, 203–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francescoli, G. (2001). Vocal signals from Ctenomys pearsoni pups. Acta Theriologica, 46, 327–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francescoli, G. (2011). Tuco-tucos’ vocalization output varies seasonally (Ctenomys pearsoni; Rodentia, Ctenomyidae): implications for reproductive signaling. Acta Ethologica, 14, 1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francescoli, G. (2017). Environmental factors could constrain the use of long-range vocal signals in solitary tuco-tucos (Ctenomys; Rodentia, Ctenomyidae) reproduction. Journal of Ecoacoustics, 1, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francescoli, G. (2019). “Evolutionary stories”: Narratives as evolutionary tools to describe and analyse animal behaviour and animal signals. In: M. Silvera-Roig (Ed.) Cognitive and intermedial semiotics. https://doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.89209.

  • Fresco, N., Jablonka, E., & Ginsburg, S. (2017). The construction of learned information through selection processes. In R. Joyce (Ed.), The routledge handbook of evolution and philosophy (pp. 91–105). London: Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Harms, W. F. (2004). Primitive content, translation, and the emergence of meaning in animal communication. In D. Kimbrough Oller & U. Griebel (Eds.), Evolution of communication systems (pp. 31–48). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D. (1996). The evolution of communication. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer, J. (2008a). Semiotic scaffolding of living systems. In M. Barbieri (Ed.), Introduction to biosemiotics (pp. 149–166). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer, J. (2008b). The semiotic niche. Journal of Mediterranean Ecology, 9, 5–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jablonka, E. (2011). Introduction: Lamarckian problematics in biology. In S. B. Gissis & E. Jablonka (Eds.), Transformation of Lamarckism. From subtle fluids to Molecular Biology (pp. 145–155). Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kroodsma, D. E. (1996). Ecology of passerine song development. In D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller (Eds.), Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds (pp. 3–19). Ithaca: Comstock/Cornell University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kull, K. (2015). Evolution, choice, and scaffolding: Semiosis is changing its own building. Biosemiotics, 8, 223–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Uller, T., Feldman, M. W., Sterelny, K., Müller, G. B., Moczek, A., Jablonka, E., & Odling-Smee, J. (2015). The extended evolutionary synthesis: its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 282, 20151019. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.1019.

  • Maynard Smith, J., & Harper, D. (2003). Animal signals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owren, M. J., Rendall, D., & Ryan, M. J. (2010). Redefining animal signaling: influence versus information in communication. Biology and Philosophy, 25, 755–780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rendall, D., Owren, M. J., & Ryan, M. J. (2009). What do animal signals mean? Animal Behaviour, 78, 233240.

  • Schleich, C. E., & Busch, C. (2002). Acoustic signals of a solitary subterranean rodent Ctenomys talarum (Rodentia: Ctenomyidae): physical characteristics and behavioural correlates. Journal of Ethology, 20, 123–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2003). Signalers & receivers in animal communication. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 145–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon, C. T. (1990). A naturalistic view of categorical perception. In S. Harnad (Ed.), Categorical Perception. The Groundwork of Cognition (pp. 332–354). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Uexküll, J. (1926). Theoretical Biology. New York: Harcourt, Brace & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Uexküll, J. (1940/2010). A theory of meaning. In: von Uexküll, J. A foray into the worlds of animals and humans, with A theory of meaning (pp. 139–208). Minneapolis: Minnesota Press.

  • Waddington, C. H. (1942). Canalization of development and the inheritance of acquired characters. Nature, 150, 563–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gabriel Francescoli.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Francescoli, G. “Semiotic Canalization”: a Process Directing the Use and Interpretation of Signals in Animal Interactions?. Biosemiotics 14, 199–207 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-020-09400-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-020-09400-0

Keywords

Navigation