Skip to main content
Log in

Language and the Free-Rider Problem: An Experimental Paradigm

  • Published:
Biological Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Change and variation, while inherent to language, might be seen as running counter to human communicative needs. However, variation also gives language the power to convey reliable indexical information about the speaker. This has been argued to play a significant role in allowing the establishment of large communities based on cooperative exchange (M. Enquist and O. Leimar 1993, The evolution of cooperation in mobile organisms. Animal Behaviour 45: 747–757; R. I. M. Dunbar 1996, Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language. London: Faber and Faber), although there has been little experimental investigation of the hypothesis. Here I present a preliminary study intended to help fill this gap. Participants played an online team game in which they negotiated anonymously for resources using an artificial language. Players succeeded in using linguistic cues to distinguish between their teammates and their opponents, and displayed between-team variation in the use of the language.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Axelrod R (1990) The Evolution of Co-operation. London: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Gintis H, Bowles S, Richerson PJ, eds (2003) The evolution of altruistic punishment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 100: 3532–3535.

  • Christiansen MH, Chater N (2007) Language as shaped by the brain. http://www.isrl.uiuc.edu/∼amag/langev/paper/christiansen07languageBrain.html

  • Cornish H (2006) Iterated learning with human subjects: An empirical framework for the emergence and cultural transmission of language. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Edinburgh, UK. http://www.ling.ed.ac.uk/lec/LEC/Research.html

    Google Scholar 

  • Croft W (2000) Explaining Language Change: An Evolutionary Approach. Harlow, UK: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cruttenden A (1986) Intonation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon TW (1997/1998) The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dediu D, Ladd DR (2007) Linguistic tone is related to the population frequency of the adaptive haplogroups of two brain size genes, ASPM and Microcephalin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104: 10944–10949.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM (1996) Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language. London: Faber and Faber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enquist M, Leimar O (1993) The evolution of cooperation in mobile organisms. Animal Behaviour 45: 747–757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans B (2004) The role of social network in the acquisition of local dialect norms by Appalachian immigrants in Ypsilanti, Michigan. Language Variation and Change 16: 153–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagen LK (2008) The bilingual brain: Human evolution and second language acquisition. Evolutionary Psychology 6: 43–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrington J (2006) An acoustic analysis of ‘happy tensing’ in the Queen’s Christmas broadcasts. Journal of Phonetics 34: 439–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby S (2000) Syntax without natural selection: How compositionality emerges from vocabulary in a population of learners. In: The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: Social Function and the Origins of Linguistic Form (Knight C, ed), 303–323. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby S, Hurford J (2002) The emergence of linguistic structure: An overview of the iterated learning model. In: Simulating the Evolution of Language (Cangelosi A, Parisi D, eds), 121–148. London: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Labov W (1963) The social motivation of a sound change. Word 19: 273–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov W (1990) The intersection of sex and social class in the course of linguistic change. Language Variation and Change 2: 205–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labov W (2001) Principles of Linguistic Change, Vol. 2: Social Factors. Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman E, Michel J, Jackson J, Tang T, Nowak MA (2007) Quantifying the evolutionary dynamics of language. Nature 449: 713–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Livingstone D (2002) The evolution of dialect diversity. In: Simulating the Evolution of Language (Cangelosi A, Parisi D, eds), 99–117. London: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Livingstone D, Fyfe C (1999) Modelling the evolution of linguistic diversity. In: Advances in Artificial Life. Proceedings of the 5th European Conference in Artificial Life, ECAL’ 99, Lausanne, Switzerland (Floreano D, Nicoud J-D, Mondada F, eds), 704–708. London: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nettle D (1999) Linguistic Diversity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nettle D, Dunbar RIM (1997) Social markers and the evolution of cooperative exchange. Current Anthropology 38: 93–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pagel M, Atkinson QD, Meade A (2007) Frequency of word-use predicts rates of lexical evolution throughout Indo-European history. Nature 449: 717–721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn M (2001) Evolving communication without dedicated communication channels. In: Advances in Artificial Life. 6th European Conference on Artificial Life, ECAL 2001 (Kelemen J, Sosik P, eds), 357–366. London: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Steels L (2000) Language as a complex adaptive system. In: Proceedings of PPSN VI (Schoenauer M, ed), 17–26. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby J, Cosmides L (1990) On the universality and the uniqueness of the individual: The role of genetics and personality. Journal of Personality 27: 17–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trudgill P (1972) Sex, covert prestige and linguistic change in the urban British English of Norwich. Language in Society 1: 179–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinreich U, Labov W, Herzog M (1968) Empirical foundations for a theory of language change. In: Directions for Historical Linguistics: A Symposium (Lehmann WP, ed), 95–195. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Worden RP (1995) A speed limit for evolution. Journal of Theoretical Biology 176: 127–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wray A, Grace GW (2007) The consequences of talking to strangers: Evolutionary corollaries of influences on linguistic form. Lingua 117: 543–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gareth Roberts.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Roberts, G. Language and the Free-Rider Problem: An Experimental Paradigm. Biol Theory 3, 174–183 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1162/biot.2008.3.2.174

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/biot.2008.3.2.174

Keywords

Navigation