Skip to main content
Log in

Cultural Evolution and the Evolution of Cultural Information

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Biological Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cultural evolution is normally framed in informational terms. However, it is not clear whether this is an adequate way to model cultural evolutionary phenomena and what, precisely, “information” is supposed to mean in this context. Would cultural evolutionary theory benefit from a well-developed theory of cultural information? The prevailing sentiment is that, in contradistinction to biology, informational language should be used nontechnically in this context for descriptive, but not explanatory, purposes. Against this view, this article makes the case for the need to take a proper biology-based “informational turn” in the cultural evolutionary sciences. I argue that the current vague use of informational language misses out on the potential benefits for advancing understanding of phenomena that information-theoretic reasoning has provided in other sciences, especially genetics. In particular, by emphasizing the informational aspects of cultural evolutionary processes, this approach can clarify some conceptual and methodological problems that have plagued cultural evolutionary theory since its inception, including (1) how to determine the channel conditions of cultural information flow, (2) the nature and scope of cultural information, and (3) how to quantify trends of cultural cumulation. More generally, theories of cultural evolution will be incomplete until the mechanisms underlying cultural processes are better understood and integrated into the explanations. This article explores the adequacy of an information-theoretic framework to accomplish these purposes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

(adapted from Laland et al. 2000, p. 134)

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Not everybody agrees that information talk has played a positive role in biological explanations. Some classic criticisms are Sarkar (1996) and Griffiths (2001). See Griffiths and Stotz (2013) and Godfrey-Smith and Sterelny (2016) for reviews of the controversies.

  2. Defining culture at the phenotypic level does not necessarily exclude the possibility of using informational terminology for other descriptive purposes within the same conceptual framework. For instance, even though Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman define culture as “the total pattern of human behavior and its products embodied in thought, speech, action and artefacts” (1981, p. 3), they nevertheless define relationships between cultural individuals in terms of transmission and flow of information.

  3. It is important to bear in mind that it is in the context of Darwinian approaches to cultural evolution where “culture as information” is proposed to be a useful conceptualization—i.e., whether this definition is applicable outside of CET does not concern cultural evolutionists, nor this article.

  4. Given the tremendous impact that human culturally driven behavior has had on planet Earth, it is likely that these sorts of cases are rather common and may include the flora and fauna.

References

  • Adami C (2012) The use of information theory in evolutionary theory. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1256:49–65

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adami C, Ofria C, Collier TC (2000) Evolution of biological complexity. PNAS 97:4463–4468

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atran S (1998) Folk biology and the anthropology of science: cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behav Brain Sci 21:547–569

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aunger R (2000) Darwinizing culture. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Barbieri M (2003) The organic codes: an introduction to semantic biology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergstrom CT, Rosvall M (2011) The transmission sense of information. Biol Philos 26:159–176

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blackmore S (1999) The meme machine. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1996) Why culture is common but cultural evolution is rare. Proc Br Acad 88:73–93

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer J, Gelfand M, Jackson JC, MacDonald IF, Peregrine PN, Richerson PJ et al (2017) Grand challenges for the study of cultural evolution. Nat Ecol Evol 1:1–3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buskell A (2020) Cumulative culture and complex cultural traditions. Mind Lang. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12335

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell D, Fiske DW (1959) Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychol Bull 56:81–105

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Charbonneau M (2019) Understanding cultural fidelity. Br J Philos Sci. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjps/axy052

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cladieré N, Scott-Phillips TC, Sperber D (2014) How Darwinian is cultural evolution? Philos Trans R Soc B 369:20130368

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb M (2015) Life’s greatest secret. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Collard M, Kemery M, Banks S (2005) Causes of toolkit variation among hunter-gatherers: a test of four competing hypotheses. Can J Archaeol 29:1–19

    Google Scholar 

  • Collard M, Buchanan B, Morin J, Costopoulos A (2011) What drives the evolution of hunter–gatherer subsistence technology? A reanalysis of the risk hypothesis with data from the pacific northwest. Philos Trans R Soc B 366:1129–1138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collard M, Ruttle A, Buchanan B, O’Brien MJ (2012) Risk of resource failure and toolkit variation in small-scale farmers and herders. PLoS ONE 7:e40975

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collard M, Ruttle A, Buchanan B, O’Brien MJ (2013) Population size and cultural evolution in nonindustrial food-producing societies. PLoS ONE 8:e72628

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crick FHC (1970) Central dogma of molecular biology. Nature 227:561–563

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra G, György G (2011) Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philos Trans R Soc B 366:1149–1157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins R (1989) The selfish gene, 2nd edn. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Dean LG, Vale GL, Laland KN, Flynn E, Kendal RL (2014) Human cumulative culture: a comparative perspective. Biol Rev 89:284–301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennet D (1995) Darwin’s dangerous idea: evolution and the meaning of life. Penguin Books, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Driscoll C (2017) The evolutionary culture concepts. Philos Sci 84:35–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durham WH (1991) Coevolution: genes, culture, and human diversity. Stanford University Press, Stanford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fogarty L, Wakano JY, Feldman MW, Kenichi A (2017) The driving forces of cultural complexity: Neanderthals, modern humans, and the question of population size. Hum Nat 28:39–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith P (2000) On the theoretical role of “genetic coding." Philos Sci 67:26–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith P, Sterelny K (2016) Biological information. In: Zalta EN (ed) The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy, summer 2016 edn. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2016/entries/information-biological/. Accessed 3 November 2020.

  • Griffiths P (2001) Genetic information: a metaphor in search of a theory. Philos Sci 68:394–412

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths P, Stotz K (2013) Genetics and philosophy: an introduction. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hahn MW, Wray G (2002) The g-value paradox. Evol Dev 4:73–75

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J (2004) Demography and cultural evolution: how adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses: the Tasmanian case. Am Antiq 69:197–214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer J (1996) Signs of meaning in the universe. University of Indiana Press, Bloomington

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoppit W, Laland KN (2013) Social learning: an introduction to mechanisms, methods, and models. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jablonka E, Lamb MJ (2005) Evolution in four dimensions: genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Jablonka E, Szathmáry E (1995) The evolution of information storage and heredity. Trends Ecol Evol 10:206–211

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kauffman S (1993) The origins of order: self-organization and selection in evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay L (2000) Who wrote the book of life? A history of the genetic code. Stanford University Press, Stanford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kimura M (1961) Natural selection as the process of accumulating genetic information in adaptive evolution. Genet Res 2:127–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kobayashi Y, Aoki K (2012) Innovativeness, population size and cumulative cultural evolution. Theor Popul Biol 82:38–47

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kronfeldner M (2021) Digging the channels of inheritance: on how to distinguish between cultural and biological inheritance. Philos Trans R Soc 376:20200042

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, O’Brien MJ (2011) Cultural niche construction: an introduction. Biol Theory 6:191–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Odling-Smee J, Feldman M (2000) Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behav Brain Sci 23:131–146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Odling-Smee J, Feldman M (2001) Cultural niche construction and human evolution. J Evol Biol 14:22–33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewens T (2015) Cultural evolution: conceptual challenges. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J (2000) The concept of information in biology. Philos Sci 67:177–194

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J, Szathmáry E (1995) The major transitions in evolution. Freeman, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr E (1982) The growth of biological thought: diversity, evolution, and inheritance. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A (2011) Cultural evolution: how Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A (2017) Pursuing Darwin’s curious parallel: prospects for a science of cultural evolution. PNAS 114:7853–7860

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A, Thornton A (2018) What is cumulative cultural evolution. Proc R Soc B: Biol Sci 285:20180712

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A, Whiten A, Laland KN (2006) Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behav Brain Sci 29:329–347

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miton H, Cladieré N, Mercier H (2015) Universal cognitive mechanisms explain the cultural success of bloodletting. Evol Hum Behav 36:303–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morin O (2011) How traditions live and die. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Odling-Smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman MW (2003) Niche construction: the neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG (2009) Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324:1298–1301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Querbes A, Vaesen K, Houkes W (2014) Complexity and demographic explanations of cumulative culture. PLoS ONE 9:e102543

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramsey G (2013) Culture in humans and other animals. Biol Philos 28:457–479

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Read D, Andersson C (2019) Cultural complexity and complexity evolution. Adapt Behav 28:329–358

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rendell L, Fogarty L, Hoppitt WJ, Morgan TJH, Webster MM, Laland KN (2011) Cognitive culture: theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends Cogn Sci 15:68–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson PJ, Boyd R (2005) Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson PJ, Christiansen MH (eds) (2013) Cultural evolution: society, technology, language, and religion. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sarkar S (1996) Biological information: a skeptical look at some central dogmas of molecular biology. In: Sarkar S (ed) The philosophy and history of molecular biology: new perspectives. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 187–231

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Shannon CE (1948) A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst Tech J 27:379–423

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shannon CE, Weaver W (1949) The mathematical theory of communication. University of Illinois Press, Urbana

    Google Scholar 

  • Shea N (2013) Inherited representations are read in development. Br J Philos Sci 64:1–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D (1996) Explaining culture: a naturalistic approach. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D, Cladière N (2008) Defining and explaining culture (comments on Richerson and Boyd, not by genes alone). Biol Philos 23:283–292

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stegmann U (2016) ‘Genetic coding’ reconsidered: an analysis of actual usage. Br J Philos Sci 67:707–730

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2012) The evolved apprentice: how evolution made humans unique. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2009) Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 364:2405–2415

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas CA Jr (1971) The genetic organization of chromosomes. Annu Rev Genet 5:237–256

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Kruger A, Ratner HH (1993) Cultural learning. Behav Brain Sci 16:495–552

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaesen K, Collard M, Cosgrove R, Roebroeks W (2016) Population size does not explain past changes in cultural complexity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113:E2241–E2247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vasas V, Fernando C, Santos M, Kauffman S, Szathmáry E (2012) Evolution before genes. Biol Direct 7:1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten A (2017) A second inheritance system: the extension of biology through culture. Interface Focus 7:20160142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten A, Hinde RA, Laland KN, Stringer CB (2011) Culture evolves. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 366(1567):938–948

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yockey HP (2005) Information theory, evolution, and the origin of life. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Thanks goes to Grant Ramsey for reading and commenting on an earlier version of this article. I’d also like to thank James Difrisco, Andreas de Block, Karim Baraghith, Lin-Qing Chen, and the anonymous reviewers for their helpful suggestions. Part of this research was presented at the International Society for the History, Philosophy and Social Studies of Biology (Oslo, 2019). I thank the audience for their feedback.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Council for Science and Technology (Mexico; Grant Number: 710049).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alejandro Gordillo-García.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

 The author has no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gordillo-García, A. Cultural Evolution and the Evolution of Cultural Information. Biol Theory 18, 30–42 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-022-00422-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-022-00422-0

Keywords

Navigation