Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Kuhn and conceptual change: on the analogy between conceptual changes in science and children

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Science & Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article argues that the analogy between conceptual changes in the history of science and conceptual changes in the development of young children is problematic. We show that the notions of ‘conceptual change’ in Kuhn and Piaget’s projects, the two thinkers whose work is most commonly drawn upon to support this analogy, are not compatible in the sense usually claimed. We contend that Kuhn’s work pertains not so much to the psychology of individual scientists, but to the way philosophers and historians should describe developments in communities of scientists. Furthermore, we argue that the analogy is based on a misunderstanding of the nature of science and the relation between science and common sense. The distinctiveness of the two notions of conceptual change has implications for science education research, since it raises serious questions about the relevance of Kuhn’s remarks for the study of pedagogical issues.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Boden MA (1979) Piaget. Harvester Press, Brighton

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterfield H (1931) The Whig interpretation of history. Bell, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey S (1986) Cognitive science and science education. Am Psychol 41(10):1123–1130

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cawthron ER, Rowell JA (1978) Epistemology and science education. Stud Sci Educ 5:31–59

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi MTH, Roscoe RD (2002) The processes and challenges of conceptual change. In: Limón M, Mason L (eds) Reconsidering conceptual change. issues in theory and practice. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 3–27

  • Clement J (1982) Students’ preconception in introductory mechanics. Am J Phys 50(1):66–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook J (1969) Human beings. In: Winch P (ed) Studies in the philosophy of Wittgenstein. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London, pp 117–151

  • Coulter J (1989) Mind in action. Polity, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • di Sessa AA (1982) Unlearning Aristotelian physics: a study of knowledge-based learning. Cognitive Sci 6(1):37–75

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Driver R (1983) The pupil as scientist? Open University Press, Milton Keynes

    Google Scholar 

  • Driver R, Asoko H, Leach J, Mortimer E, Scott P (1994) Constructing scientific knowledge in the classroom. Educ Res 23(7):5–12

    Google Scholar 

  • Duit R, Treagust DF (2003) Conceptual change: a powerful framework for improving science teaching and learning. Int J Sci Educ 25(16):671–688

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans-Pritchard EE (1937) Witchcraft, oracles and magic among the Azande. Clarendon Press, Oxford

  • French J (1989) Accomplishing scientific instruction. In: Millar R (ed) Doing science: images of science in science education. Falmer, London, pp 10–37

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel H (1967) Studies in ethnomethodology. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ

  • Garrison JW, Bentley ML (1990) Science education, conceptual change and breaking with everyday experience. Stud Philos Educ 10:19–35

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson BS (1985) The convergence of Kuhn and cognitive psychology. New Ideas Psychol 2(3):211–221

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert JK, Osborne RJ, Fensham PJ (1982) Children’s science and its consequence for teaching. Sci Educ 66(4):623–633

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber H (1973) Courage and cognitive growth in children and scientists. In: Schwebel M, Raph J (eds) Piaget in the classroom. Basic Books, New York, pp 73–105

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewson PW (1981) A conceptual change approach to learning science. Eur J Sci Educ 3(4):383–396

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewson PW, Hewson MG (1984) The role of conceptual conflict in conceptual change and the design of science instruction. Instruct Sci 13(1):1–13

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher P (1988) The child as parent of the scientist. Mind & Language 3(3):217–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener RF (1987) Genetic epistemology: equilibration and the rationality of scientific change. Stud History Philos Sci 18(3):339–366

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn D (1989) Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychol Rev 96(4):674–689

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn TS (1957) The Copernican revolution: planetary astronomy in the development of Western thought. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn TS (1970a) Logic of discovery or psychology of research? In: Lakatos I, Musgrave A (eds) Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 1–23

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn TS (1970b) Reflections on my critics. In: Lakatos I, Musgrave A (eds) Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 231–278

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn TS (1977 [1959]) The essential tension: tradition and innovation in scientific research. In: Kuhn TS (ed) The essential tension: selected studies in scientific tradition and change. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 225–239. Originally published 1959 in Taylor CW (ed), The Third (1959) University of Utah research conference on the identification of scientific talent. University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City

  • Kuhn TS (1977 [1964]) A function for thought experiments. In: The essential tension: selected studies in scientific tradition and change. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 240–265. Originally published 1964 in L’aventure de la science. Mélanges Alexandre Koyré, Hermann, Paris

  • Kuhn TS (1977 [1971]) Concepts of cause in the development of physics. In: The essential tension: selected studies in scientific tradition and change. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 21–30. Originally published 1971 as ‘Les notions de causalité dans le developpement de la physique’ in Edudes d’épistémologie génétique 25

  • Kuhn TS (1977 [1974]) Second thoughts on paradigms. In: The essential tension: selected studies in scientific tradition and change. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 293–319. Originally published 1974 in Suppe F (ed) The structure of scientific theories. University of Illinois Press, Urbana

  • Kuhn TS (1989a) Possible worlds in history of science. In: Allén S (ed) Possible worlds in humanities, arts and sciences. Walter de Gruyter, Berlin, pp 9–32

  • Kuhn TS (1989b) Speaker’s reply. In: Allén S (ed) Possible worlds in humanities, arts and sciences. Walter de Gruyter, Berlin pp 49–51

  • Kuhn TS (1990) The road since structure. In: PSA 1990, vol. 2, pp 3–13

  • Kuhn TS (1996) The structure of scientific revolutions, 3rd edn. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos I, Musgrave A (eds) (1970) Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

  • Leach J, Scott P (2003) Individual and sociocultural views of learning in science education. Sci & Educ 12(1):91–113

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine AT (2000) Which ways is up? Thomas S. Kuhn’s analogy to conceptual development in childhood. Sci & Educ 9(1/2):107–122

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loving CC, Cobern WW (2000) Invoking Thomas Kuhn: what citation analysis reveals about science education. Sci & Educ 9(1/2):187–206

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch M, Macbeth DH (1998) Demonstrating physics lessons. In: Greeno JG, Goldman SV (eds) Thinking practices in mathematics and science learning. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp 269–297

  • Macbeth DH (2000) On an actual apparatus for conceptual change. Sci Educ 84(2):228–264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macbeth DH (2005) Respecifying concepts: the case of classroom research. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association (AERA), Montreal, April 2005

  • Matthews MR (2000) Editorial. Sci & Educ 9(1/2):1–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews MR (2003) Thomas Kuhn’s impact on science education: what lessons can be learned?. Sci Educ 88(1):90–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell G, Feigl H (1961) Why ordinary language needs reforming. J Philos 58(18):488–498

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey M (1983) Naive theories of motion. In: Gentner D, Stevens AL (eds) Mental models. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 299–324

  • Nersessian NJ (1989) Conceptual change in science and in science education. Synthese 80(1):163–183

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osborne RJ (1980) Some aspects of students’ views of the world. Res Sci Educ 10:11–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget J (1930) The child’s conception of physical causality. Routledge, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget J (1970) Genetic epistemology. Columbia University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Popper KR (1970) Against “Normal science”. In: Lakatos I, Musgrave A (eds) Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 51–58

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner GJ, Strike KA, Hewson PW, Gertzog WA (1982) Accommodation of a scientific conception: toward a theory of conceptual change. Sci Educ 66(2):211–227

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiner M, Chi MTH, Resnick LB (1988) Naive materialistic belief: an underlying epistemological commitment.In: Proceedings of the tenth annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (August 17–19, 1988, Montreal, Canada). Lawrence Erlbaum, NJ, pp 544–551

  • Resnick LB (1983) Mathematics and science learning: a new conception. Science 220(4596):477–478

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotman B (1977) Jean Piaget: psychologist of the real. Harvester Press, Hassocks

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle G (1954a) The world of science and the everyday world. In: Ryle G (ed) Dilemmas. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 68–81

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle G (1954b) Technical and untechnical concepts. In: Ryle G (ed) Dilemmas. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 82–92

    Google Scholar 

  • Schütz A (1962a) Common-sense and scientific interpretation of human action. In: Collected papers I: studies in social theory. Martinus Nijhoff, The Hague, pp 3–47

  • Schütz A (1962b) On multiple realities. In: Collected papers I: studies in social theory. Martinus Nijhoff, The Hague, pp 207–259

  • Shanker SG (2004) The roots of mindblindness. Theory Psychol 14(5):685–703

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharrock WW, Anderson DC (1982) The persistent evasion of technical issues in media studies. Sociology 16(1):108–115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharrock WW, Read R (2002) Kuhn: philosopher of scientific revolution. Polity, Oxford

  • Siegel H (1982) On the parallel between Piagetian cognitive development and the history of science. Philos Soc Sci 12(4):375–386

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou S (1989) On the nature of children’s naive knowledge. In: Proceedings of the eleventh annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (August 16–19, 1989, Ann Arbor, Michigan). Lawrence Erlbaum, NJ, pp 404–411

  • Vosniadou S, Brewer WF (1987) Theories of knowledge restructuring in development. Rev Educ Res 57(1):51–67

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou S, Brewer WF (1992) Mental models of the earth: a study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychol 24(4):535–585

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou S, Ioannides C (1998) From conceptual development to science education: a psychological point of view. Int J Sci Educ 20(10):1213–1230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winch P (1964) Understanding a primitive society. Am Philos Quart 1:304–324

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein L (1953) Philosophical investigations (Translated by GEM Anscombe). Basil Blackwell, Oxford

  • Wittgenstein L (1979) Remarks on Frazer’s “Golden Bough”. In: Luckhardt CG (ed) Wittgenstein: sources and perspectives. Harvester Press, Hassocks, pp 61–81

  • Woodfield A (1987) On the very idea of acquiring a concept. In: Russell J (ed) Philosophical perspectives on developmental psychology. Basil Blackwell, Oxford, pp 17–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert L (1992) The unnatural nature of science. Faber and Faber, London

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Wes Sharrock and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments and criticisms on an earlier version. Christian Greiffenhagen would like to thank the British Academy for a Postdoctoral Fellowship to support work on this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christian Greiffenhagen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Greiffenhagen, C., Sherman, W. Kuhn and conceptual change: on the analogy between conceptual changes in science and children. Sci & Educ 17, 1–26 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-006-9063-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-006-9063-5

Keywords

Navigation