Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Sidama Model of Human Development

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Human ontogeny has been shaped through evolution, resulting in markers of physical, cognitive, and social development that are widely shared and often used to demarcate the lifespan. Yet, development is demonstrably biocultural and strongly influenced by context. As a result, emic age categories can vary in duration and composition, constituted by both common physical markers as well as culturally meaningful indicators, with implications for our understanding of the evolution of human life history. Semi-structured group interviews (n = 24) among Sidama adults and children, as well as individual interviews with children (n = 30), were used to identify age categories across the lifespan and to specifically investigate acquisition of sociocultural skills and cognitive development. Ten major age categories were identified, covering birth through death. These largely map onto patterning of human universals, but specific cultural beliefs and behaviors were indicated as important markers of development. Adults and children are oriented toward the dynamic relationships between physical development and acquisition of skills tied to social and cultural success. Culture, ecology, and ontogeny are co-determinants of human development, and the interactions among them should be considered in studies examining human life history and its evolution.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

Data are available upon request from the authors.

Code Availability

Not applicable.

Notes

  1. Here, minority world refers to populations residing in “developed” countries, alternatively termed WEIRD populations—those that are Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (Gurven & Lieberman, 2020). Majority world is an alternative term for populations living in regions or countries often labeled as “developing” (Alam, 2008).

  2. Hereafter, masculine nouns are reported first, followed by the feminine spelling.

  3. An age category wherein men have the wealth to distribute to their sons, the authority to aid in decision-making around policy and disputes, and the wisdom to guide others through their insights.

References

  • Alam, S. (2008). Majority world: Challenging the West’s rhetoric of democracy. Amerasia Journal, 34(1), 88–98. https://doi.org/10.17953/amer.34.1.l3176027k4q614v5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Areru, H. A., Dangisso, M. H., & Lindtjørn, B. (2020). Births and deaths in Sidama in southern Ethiopia: Findings from the Dale-Wonsho Health and Demographic Surveillance System (HDSS). Global Health Action, 13(1), https://doi.org/10.1080/16549716.2020.1833511.

  • Bogin, B. (1999). Evolutionary perspective on human growth. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 109–153. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogin, B. (2010). Evolution of human growth. In M. P. Muehlenbein (Ed.), Human evolutionary biology (pp. 379–395). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bogin, B., Bragg, J., & Kuzawa, C. (2016). Childhood, biocultural reproduction, and human lifetime reproductive effort. In C. L. Meehan & A. N. Crittenden (Eds.), Childhood: Origins, evolution, and implications (pp. 45–72). School for Advanced Research Press.

  • Brögger, J. (1986). Belief and experience among the Sidamo: A case study towards an anthropology of knowledge. Norwegian University Press.

  • Campbell, B. (2006). Adrenarche and the evolution of human life history. American Journal of Human Biology, 18, 569–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Central Statistical Agency (CSA) – Ethiopia. (2007). The 2007 population and housing census of Ethiopia: Statistical report for Southern Nations, Nationalities and Peoples’ region, part I: Population size and characteristics. Addis Ababa, Ethiopia: Central Statistical Agency.

  • Charnov, E. (1993). Life history invariants. Oxford University Press.

  • Chisholm, J. S., Quinlivan, J. A., Petersen, R. W., & Coall, D. A. (2005). Early stress predicts age at menarche and first birth, adult attachment, and expected lifespan. Human Nature, 16(3), 233–265. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-005-1009-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cillessen, A. H. N., & Bellmore, A. D. (2002). Social skills and interpersonal perception in early and middle childhood. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood social development (pp. 355–374). Blackwell Publishers, Ltd.

  • Collins, A. (1984). Introduction. In A. Collins (Ed.), Development during middle childhood: The years from six to twelve (pp. 1–23). Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crittenden, A. N., & Marlowe, F. W. (2008). Allomaternal care among the Hadza of Tanzania. Human Nature, 19(3), 249–262. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-008-9043-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crittenden, A. N., Conklin-Brittain, N. L., Zes, D. A., Schoeninger, M. J., & Marlowe, F. W. (2013). Juvenile foraging among the Hadza: Implications for human life history. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(4), 299–304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.04.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Del Giudice, M., Angeleri, R., & Manera, V. (2009). The juvenile transition: A developmental switch point in human life history. Developmental Review, 29(1), 1–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2008.09.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Del Giudice, M., Barrett, E. S., Belsky, J., Hartman, S., Martel, M. M., Sangenstedt, S., & Kuzawa, C. W. (2018). Individual differences in developmental plasticity: A role for early androgens? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 90, 165–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.02.025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dira, S., & Hewlett, B. (2016). Learning to survive ecological risks among the Sidama of southwestern Ethiopia. Journal of Ecological Anthropology, 18(1), https://doi.org/10.5038/2162-4593.18.1.7.

  • Ellis, B. J., & Garber, J. (2000). Psychosocial antecedents of variation in girls’ pubertal timing: Maternal depression, stepfather presence, and marital and family stress. Child Development, 71(2), 485–501. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Esho, T., Enzlin, P., & van Wolputte, S. (2013). Borders of the present: Maasai tradition, modernity, and female identity. In S. van Wolputte (Ed.), Borderlands and frontiers in Africa (pp. 213–233). LIT Verlag.

  • Ezra, M. (2003). Factors associated with marriage and family formation processes in southern Ethiopia. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 34(4), 509–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gall, B. J., Wang, H., Dira, S. J., & Helfrecht, C. (2022). Effects of family demographics and household economics on Sidama children’s nutritional status. Human Nature, 33(3), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-022-09432-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, A. H. (2013). Bringing culture into human biology and biology back into anthropology. American Anthropologist, 115(3), 359–373. https://doi.org/10.1111/aman.12022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossmann, I., Karasawa, M., Izumi, S., Na, J., Varnum, M. E. W., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2012). Aging and wisdom: Culture matters. Source: Psychological Science, 23(10), 1059–1066. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612446025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M. D., & Lieberman, D. E. (2020). WEIRD bodies: Mismatch, medicine and missing diversity. Evolution and Human Behavior41, 330–340. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.04.001

  • Hailu, A., & Regassa, N. (2008). Characterization of women under polygamous marital relationship (an examination of socio-cultural contexts of gender disparities through qualitative approach study in Sidama Zone of southern Ethiopia). The Oriental Anthropologist, 8(1–2), 169–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamer, J. H. (1987). Human development: Participation and change among the Sadama of Ethiopia. The University of Alabama Press.

  • Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (1983). The cultural construction of child cevelopment. Ethos, 11(4), 221–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. The Free Press.

  • Harris, K. M., & McDade, T. W. (2018). The biosocial approach to human development, behavior, and health across the life course. RSF: The Russell Sage Foundation Journal of the Social Sciences4(4), 2–26. https://doi.org/10.7758/rsf.2018.4.4.01

  • Hartup, W. W. (1989). Social relationships and their developmental significance. American Psychologist, 44(2), 120–126. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.2.120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1997). Hadza women’s time allocation, offspring provisioning, and the evolution of long postmenopausal life spans. Current Anthropology, 38(4), 551–577.

  • Helfrecht, C., Hagen, E. H., DeAvila, D., Bernstein, R. M., Dira, S. J., & Meehan, C. L. (2018). DHEAS patterning across childhood in three sub-Saharan populations: Associations with age, sex, ethnicity, and cortisol. American Journal of Human Biology, 30(2), e23090. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23090.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helfrecht, C., Roulette, J. W., Lane, A., Sintayehu, B., & Meehan, C. L. (2020). Life history and socioecology of infancy. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 173(4), 619–629. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.24145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. S. (1991). Intimate fathers: The nature and context of Aka pygmy paternal infant care. University of Michigan Press.

  • Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2005). Emerging issues in the study of hunter-gatherer children. In B. S. Hewlett, & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental, & cultural perspectives (pp. 3–18). Aldine/Transaction.

  • Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (1996). Ache life history: The ecology and demography of a foraging people. Aldine Transaction.

  • Hill, K., & Kaplan, H. (1999). Life history traits in humans: Theory and empirical studies. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 397–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, N. (2010). Life histories of the Dobe !Kung: Food, fatness, and well-being over the lifespan. University of California Press.

  • Hrdy, S. B. (1999). Mother Nature: A history of mothers, infants, and natural selection. Pantheon Books.

  • Hrdy, S. B. (2016). Development plus social selection in the emergence of “emotionally modern” humans. In C. L. Meehan & A. N. Crittenden (Eds.), Childhood: Origins, evolution, and implications (pp. 11–44). School for Advanced Research Press.

  • Kaplan, H., & Dove, H. (1987). Infant development among the Ache of eastern Paraguay. Developmental Psychology, 23(2), 190–198. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.2.190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keith, J., Fry, C., Glascock, A., Ikels, C., Dickerson-Putman, J., Harpending, H., & Draper, P. (2012). Age and the life course. In Keith et al., The aging experience: Diversity and commonality across cultures (chap. 6). SAGE Publications. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781452243382.

  • Kitzinger, J. (1994). The methodology of focus groups: The importance of interaction between research participants. Sociology of Health and Illness, 16(1), 103–121. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9566.ep11347023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konner, M. J. (1976). Maternal care, infant behavior and development among the !Kung. In R. B. Lee, & I. DeVore (Eds.), Kalahari hunter-gatherers: Studies of the !Kung San and their neighbors (pp. 218–248). Harvard University Press.

  • Konner, M. J. (2010). The evolution of childhood: Relationships, emotion, mind. The evolution of childhood: Relationships, emotion, mind. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

  • Kramer, K. L. (2002). Variation in juvenile dependence: Helping behavior among Maya children. Human Nature, 13(2), 299–325. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-002-1011-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ladd, G. W. (1999). Peer relationships and social competence during early and middle childhood. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 333–359. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D. F. (1996). Playing on the mother ground: Cultural routines for children’s development. Guilford.

  • Lancy, D. F. (2008). The anthropology of childhood: Cherubs, chattel, changelings. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D. F. (2012). The chore curriculum. In G. Spittler, & M. Bourdillion (Eds.), African children at work: Working and learning in growing up (pp. 23–56). LIT Verlag.

  • Lancy, D. F., & Grove, M. A. (2011). Getting noticed: Middle childhood in cross-cultural perspective. Human Nature, 22(3), 281–302. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-011-9117-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D. F., & Grove, M. A. (2015). Cultural views of life phases. Sociology, Social Work and Anthropology Faculty Publications, 573. https://digitalcommons.usu.edu/sswa_facpubs/573

  • LeVine, R. A., Dixon, S., LeVine, S., Richman, A., Leiderman, P. H., Keefer, C. H., & Brazelton, T. B. (1994). Child care and culture: Lessons from Africa. Cambridge University Press.

  • Majewska, M. D. (1995). Neuronal actions of dehydroepiandrosterone. Possible roles in brain development, aging, memory, and affect. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 774, 111–120. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8597451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard, A. E. (2002). Cultural teaching: The development of teaching skills in Maya sibling interactions. Child Development, 73(3), 969–982.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, M. (1947). Age patterning in personality development. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 17(2), 231–240. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1947.tb04994.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meehan, C. L., Helfrecht, C., & Malcom, C. D. (2016). Implications of lengthy development and maternal life history: Allomaternal investment, peer relationships, and social networks. In C. L. Meehan & A. N. Crittenden (Eds.), Childhood: Origins, evolution, and implications (pp. 199–220). School for Advanced Research Press.

  • Montgomery, H. (2009). An introduction to childhood: Anthropological perspectives on children’s lives. Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Page, A. E., Emmott, E., Dyble, M., Smith, D., Chaudhary, N., Viguier, S., & Migliano, A. (2021). Children are important too: Juvenile playgroups and maternal childcare in a foraging population, the Agta. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0026.

  • Piaget, J. (1963). The origins of intelligence in children. W.W. Norton.

  • Quinlan, R. J. (2003). Father absence, parental care, and female reproductive development. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(6), 376–390. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00039-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., Sellers, M. J., Pirotta, S., Fox, N., & White, S. (1975). Age of assignment of roles and responsibilities to children: A cross-cultural survey. Human Development, 18(5), 353–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sear, R., Mace, R., & McGregor, I. A. (2000). Maternal grandmothers improve nutritional status and survival of children in rural Gambia. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 267, 1641–1647. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1190

  • Sellen, D. W., & Smay, D. B. (2001). Relationship between subsistence and age at weaning in “preindustrial” societies. Human Nature, 12(1), 47–87. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-001-1013-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stearns, S. (1992). The evolution of life histories. Oxford University Press.

  • Takada, A. (2005). Mother-infant interactions among the !Xun: Analysis of gymnastic and breastfeeding behaviors. In B. S. Hewlett & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental, and cultural perspectives (pp. 289–308). Aldine/Transaction.

  • Tanner, J. M. (1962). Growth at adolescence: With a general consideration of the effects of hereditary and environmental factors upon growth and maturation from birth to maturity (2nd ed.). Blackwell Scientific.

  • Tessema, M., Belachew, T., & Ersino, G. (2013). Feeding patterns and stunting during early childhood in rural communities of Sidama South Ethiopia. Pan African Medical Journal, 14, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.11604/pamj.2013.14.75.1630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veile, A., & Kramer, K. (2015). Birth and breastfeeding dynamics in a modernizing indigenous community. Journal of Human Lactation, 31(1), 145–155. https://doi.org/10.1177/0890334414557177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weisner, T. S., & Gallimore, R. (1977). My brother’s keeper: Child and sibling caretaking. Current Anthropology, 18(2), 169–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, C. P. (1988). Children of different worlds: The formation of social behavior. Harvard University Press.

  • Whiting, B. B., & Whiting, J. W. M. (1975). Children of six cultures: A psycho-cultural analysis. Harvard University Press.

  • Wilbert, J. (1976). To become a maker of canoes: An essay in Warao enculturation. In J. Wilbert (Ed.), Enculturation in Latin America (pp. 303–358). UCLA Latin American Center Publications.

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by the National Science Foundation (BCS# 1260428). We offer special thanks to all those who participated in this research, Filate Fissa, and Hawassa University.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Science Foundation (BCS-1260428).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CH conceived and designed the study, and collected the data. Data was analyzed by CH and SD. The manuscript was written by CH and SD. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Courtney Helfrecht.

Ethics declarations

Conflicts of Interest/Competing Interests

Not applicable.

Ethics Approval

The protocol and procedures presented herein were approved by Washington State University’s Institutional Review Board (where the corresponding author was a PhD student at the time of data collection).

Consent to Participate

Following explanation of the proposed research to participants and prior to data collection, informed assent was obtained from child participants and parent permission obtained from all adults.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Helfrecht, C., Dira, S.J. The Sidama Model of Human Development. Hum Nat 34, 202–228 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-023-09449-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-023-09449-z

Keywords

Navigation