Skip to main content
Log in

Social Learning and Innovation in Adolescence

A Comparative Study of Aka and Chabu Hunter-Gatherers of Central and Eastern Africa

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper examines how innovative skills and knowledge are transmitted and acquired among adolescents in two hunter-gatherer communities, the Aka of southern Central African Republic and the Chabu of southwestern Ethiopia. Modes of transmission and processes of social learning are addressed. Innovation as well as social learning have been hypothesized to be key features of human cumulative culture, enhancing the fitness and survival of individuals in diverse environments. The innovation literature indicates adult males are more innovative than children and female adults and therefore predicts that adolescents will seek out adult males. Further, the mode of transmission should be oblique (i.e., learning from adults other than parents). Thus, learning of innovations should be oblique or horizontal rather than vertical, with adolescents paying particular attention to “successful” innovative individuals (prestige bias). The social learning literature indicates that complex skills or knowledge is likely to be learned through teaching, and therefore that teaching will be an important process in the transmission of innovations. In-depth and semi-structured interviews, informal observations, and systematic free-listing were used to evaluate these hypotheses. The study found that (1) cultural context patterned whether or not adolescents sought out adult male or female innovators; (2) oblique modes of transmission were mentioned with greater frequency than horizontal or vertical modes; (3) knowledge and skill bias was notable and explicitly linked by the adolescents to reproductive effort; and (4) teaching was biased toward same-sex individuals and was an important but not an exclusive means of transmitting complex skills and social knowledge.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.

Notes

  1. “Small scale” refers to societies generally lacking significant market integration, having limited interaction with state-based entities, and being strongly influenced by long-standing social norms that minimize social stratification and inequality (Garfield et al., 2019; Reyes-García et al., 2017).

  2. “Foundational schema” refers to modes of thought that pervade life, from subsistence activities to who sleeps together to how to organize a dance.

  3. The publications by Scheinfeldt et al. (2019) and Schnoebelen (2009) use the Majang word Sabue, or Shabu, rather than the ethnonym Chabu.

References

  • Anderson-Fye, E. A. (2004). “Coca-cola” shape: Cultural change, body image, and eating disorders in San Andres, Belize. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 28(4), 561–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aoki, K. (2013). Determinants of cultural evolutionary rates. In T. Akazawa, Y. Nishiaki, K. Aoki (Eds.), The dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans, vol. 1: Cultural perspectives (pp. 199–210). Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series. Springer Japan

  • Arias, L., Schröder, R., Hübner, A., Barreto, G., Stoneking, M., & Pakendorf, B. (2018). Cultural innovations influence patterns of genetic diversity in northwestern Amazonia. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 35(11), 2719–2735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arom, S., Fernando, N., Fürniss, S., le Bomin, S., Marandola, F., & Molino, J. (2008). La categorization des patrimoinesmusicaux de tradition orale. In F. Alvarez-Pereyre (Ed.), Categories and categorization: Une perspective interdisciplinaire (pp. 273–313). Peeters-Selaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barton, C. M., Riel-Salvatore, J., Anderies, J. M., & Popescu, G. (2011). Modeling human ecodynamics and biocultural interactions in the Late Pleistocene of Western Eurasia. Human Ecology, 39, 705–725.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berl, R. E. W., & Hewlett, B. S. (2015). Cultural variation in the use of overimitation by the Aka and Ngandu of the Congo Basin. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0120180.

  • Biondi, I. M., Bo, M. S., & Vassallo, A. I. (2010). Inter-individual and age differences in the exploration, neophilia and problem solving ability in a Neotropical raptor (Milvago chimango). Animal Cognition, 13, 701–710.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C. (1995). Innovation in wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology, 16(1), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C. (2003). Is culture a golden barrier between human and chimpanzee? Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 82–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogin, B. (2009). Childhood, adolescence, and longevity: A multilevel model of the evolution of reserve capacity in human life history. American Journal of Human Biology, 21, 567–577.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogin, B. (2013). Childhood, adolescence, and longevity: A chapter on human evolutionary life history. In B. L. Hewlett (Ed.), Adolescent identity: Evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyette, A. H. (2013). Social learning during middle childhood among Aka foragers and Ngandu farmers of the Central African Republic. PhD dissertation, Washington State University, Pullman.

  • Boyette, A. H. (2016a). Children’s play and culture learning in an egalitarian foraging society. Child Development, 87(3), 759–769.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyette, A. H. (2016b). Children’s play and the integration of social and individual learning: a cultural niche construction perspective. In H. Terashima & B. S. Hewlett (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives (pp. 159–169). Springer Japan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. S. (2017). Autonomy, equality, and teaching among Aka foragers and Ngandu farmers of the Congo Basin. Human Nature, 28, 289–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caro, T. M., & Hauser, M. D. (1992). Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? Quarterly Review Biology, 67(2), 151–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr, K., Kendal, R. L., & Flynn, E. G. (2016). Eureka! What is innovation, how does it develop, and who does it? Child Development, 87, 1505–1519.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cashdan, E., & Gaulin, S. (2016). Why go there? Evolution of mobility and spatial cognition in women and men: An introduction to the special issue. Human Nature, 27, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chudek, M., Heller, S., Birch, S., & Henrich, J. (2012). Prestige-biased cultural learning: Bystander’s differential attention to potential models influences children’s learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(1), 46–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, E. F., & Quinn, J. L. (2012). Personality and problem solving performance explain competitive ability in the wild. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 279, 1168–1175.

  • Coward, F., & Grove, M. (2011). Beyond the tools: Social innovation and Hominin evolution. In Special issue: Innovation and the evolution of human behavior. PaleoAnthropology, 2011, 111–129. https://doi.org/10.4207/PA.2011.ART46.

  • Crittenden, A. N. (2016). Children’s foraging and play among the Hadza: The evolutionary significance of “work play.” In C. L. Meehan & A. N. Crittenden (Eds.), Childhood: Origins, evolution and implications (pp. 155–170). SAR Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, C., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(4), 148–153.

  • Csibra, C., & Gergely, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1149–1157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science, 335, 1114–1118.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeBoer, W. (1990). Interaction, imitation, and communication as expressed in style: the Ucayali experience. In M. Conkey & C. Hastorf (Eds.), The uses of style in archaeology (pp. 82–104). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, J. (1999). Guns, germs and steel. W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A. (2005). Why we are social animals: The high road to imitation as social glue. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: from neuroscience to social science, vol. 2: Imitation, human development, and culture (pp. 207–220). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dira, S. J., & Hewlett, B. S. (2016). Learning to spear hunt among Ethiopian Chabu adolescents. In H. Terashima & B. S. Hewlett (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives (pp. 71–81). Springer Japan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dira, S. J., & Hewlett, B. S. (2018a). The Chabu hunter-gatherers of the highland forests of southwestern Ethiopia. Hunter-Gatherer Research, 3(2), 323–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dira, S. J., & Hewlett, B. S. (2018b). Cultural resilience among the Chabu foragers in southwestern Ethiopia. African Study Monographs, 39(3), 97–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elias, S. (2012). Origins of human innovation and creativity: Breaking old paradigms. In S. Elias (Ed.), Origins of human innovation and creativity (pp. 1–13). Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, B. (2013). Risky adolescent behavior: An evolutionary perspective. In B. L. Hewlett (Ed.), Adolescent identity: Evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 40–72). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enquist, M., Strimling, P., Eriksson, K., Laland, K., & Sjostrand, J. (2010). One cultural parent makes no culture. Animal Behavior, 79, 1353–1362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enquist, M., Ghirlanda, S., & Eriksson, K. (2011). Modelling the evolution and diversity of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 412–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fouts, H. N., Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2012). A biocultural approach to breastfeeding interactions in central Africa. American Anthropologist, 114, 123–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fürniss, S., & Joiris, D. V. (2010). Ritual and musical creation in Baka culture. Paper presented at the International Conference on Congo Basin Hunter-Gatherers, September 22–24, Montpellier, France.

  • Galef, B. G. (1992). The question of animal culture. Human Nature, 3, 157–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfield, Z. H., & Hagen, E. H. (2019). Investigating evolutionary models of leadership among recently settled Ethiopian hunter-gatherers. The Leadership Quarterly, 31(2), 101290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2019.03.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garfield, Z., Garfield, M., & Hewlett, B. S. (2016). A cross-cultural analysis of hunter-gatherer social learning. In H. Terashima & B. S. Hewlett (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers (pp. 19–34). Springer Japan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfield, Z. H., von Rueden, C., & Hagen, E. H. (2019). The evolutionary anthropology of political leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 30(1), 59–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2018.09.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2006). Sylvia’s recipe: The role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of human culture. In N. J. Enfield & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: culture, cognition and human interaction (pp. 229–255). Berg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., Egyed, K., & Király, I. (2007). On pedagody. Developmental Science, 10(1), 139–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis, H., Bowles, S., Boyd, R., & Fehr, E. (2003). Explaining altruistic behavior in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 153–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gladwell, M. (2013). Complexity and the ten-thousand-hour rule. The New Yorker. https://www.newyorker.com/sports/sporting-scene/complexity-and-the-ten-thousand-hour-rule.

  • Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., Nugent, T. F., Herman, D. H., Clasen, L. S., Toga, A. W., Rapoport, J. L., & Thompson, P. M. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(21), 8174–8179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosselain, O. P. (1998). Social and technical identity in a clay crystal ball. In M. Stark (Ed.), The archaeology of social boundaries (pp. 78–106). Smithsonian Institution Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P. M. (1984). A theory of the teacher in the learning activities of everyday life. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.), Everyday cognition (pp. 117–138). Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2001). Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations: adoption dynamics indicate that biased cultural transmission is the predominate force in behavioral change. American Anthropologist, 103, 992–1013.

  • Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: Why adaptive cultural processes produced maladaptive losses in Tasmania. American Antiquity, 69, 197–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2010). The evolution of innovation enhancing institutions. In M. O’Brien & S. J. Shennan (Eds.), Innovations in cultural systems contributions from evolutionary anthropology (pp. 99–120). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(3), 165–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Broesch, J. (2011). On the nature of cultural transmission networks: Evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1139–1148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S. C., Fehr, E., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Smith, N., Henrich, N., Hill, K., Gil-White, F., Gurven, M., Marlowe, F., Patton, J., & Tracer, D. (2005). “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral Brain Science, 28(6), 795–855.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. L. (2012). Listen, here is a story: Ethnographic life narratives from Aka and Ngandu women of the Congo Basin. Oxford University Press.

  • Hewlett, B. L. (2013). Adolescent identity: Evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. L. (2014). “Ekeloko”: The spirit to create. Innovation and social learning among Aka adolescents of the Central African rainforest. In T. Akazawa, Y. Nishiaki, K. Aoki (Eds.), The dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans, vol. 1: Cultural perspectives (pp. 187–195). Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series. Springer Japan.

  • Hewlett, B. L. (2016). Innovation, processes of social learning, and modes of cultural transmission among the Chabu adolescent forager-farmers of Ethiopia. In H. Terashima & B. S. Hewlett (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers (pp. 203–213). Springer Japan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. S. (1991). Intimate fathers: The nature and context of Aka Pygmy paternal infant care. University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. S., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1986). Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. American Anthropology, 88, 922–934.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. (2002). Integrating evolution, culture and developmental psychology: Explaining caregiver-infant proximity and responsiveness in Central Africa and the USA. In H. Keller, Y. H. Poortinga, & A. Schölmerich (Eds.), Between culture and biology: Perspectives on ontogenetic development (pp. 241–69). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. L., & Hewlett, B. S. (2013). Hunter-gatherer adolescence. In B. L. Hewlett (Ed.), Adolescent identity: Evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 73–101). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. S., & Roulette, C. J. (2016). Teaching in hunter-gatherer infancy. Royal Society Open Science, 3, 150403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. S., Fouts, H., Boyette, A., & Hewlett, B. L. (2011). Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1168–1178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. S., Berl, R. E. W., & Roulette, C. J. (2016). Teaching and over-imitation in Aka hunter-gatherers. In H. Terashima & B. Hewlett (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers (pp. 35–45). Springer Japan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. (2002). Altruistic cooperation during foraging by the Ache, and the evolved human predisposition to cooperate. Human Nature, 13, 105–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. R., Walker, R. S., Bozicević, M., Eder, J., Headland, T., Hewlett, B., Hurtado, A. M., Marlowe, F., Wiessner, P., & Wood, B. (2011). Co-residence patterns in hunter-gatherer societies show unique human social structure. Science, 331, 1286–1289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. R., Wood, B. M., Baggio, J., Hurtado, A. M., & Boyd, R. T. (2014). Hunter-gatherer inter-band interaction rates: Implications for cumulative culture. PLoS ONE, 9(7), e102806.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inkeles, A. (2000). Measuring social capital and its consequences. Policy Science, 33, 245–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez, Á. V., & Mesoudi, A. (2019). Prestige-biased social learning: Current evidence and outstanding questions. Palgrave Communications, 5(1), 20. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-019-0228-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kameda, T., & Nakanishi, D. (2002). Cost–benefit analysis of social/cultural learning in a nonstationary uncertain environment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 373–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katzir, G. (1982). Relationships between social structure and response to novelty in captive jackdaws, Corvus monedula, response to novel space. Behavior, 81, 231–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendal, R. L., Coolen, I., van Bergen, Y., & Laland, K. N. (2005). Tradeoffs in the adaptive use of social and asocial learning. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 35, 333–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleibeuker, S. W., Koolschijn, P. C. M. P., Jolles, D. D., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Crone, E. A. (2013). The neural coding of creative idea generation across adolescence and early adulthood. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 905.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline, M. A., Boyd, R., & Henrich, J. (2013). Teaching and the life history of cultural transmission in Fijian villages. Human Nature, 24, 351–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kummer, H., & Goodall, J. (1985). Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 308, 203–214.

  • Kurland, J. A., & Gaulin, S. J. C. (1984). The evolution of male parental investment: Effects of genetic relatedness and feeding ecology on the allocation of reproductive effort. In D. M. Taub (Ed.), Primate paternalism (pp. 259–308). Van Nostrand Reinhold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K., & Reader, S. (1999). Foraging innovation is inversely related to competitive ability in male but not in female guppies. Behavioral Ecology, 10, 270–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K., & Reader, S. (2010). Comparative perspectives on human innovation. In M. O’Brien & S. J. Shennan (Eds.), Innovations in cultural systems contributions from evolutionary anthropology (pp. 37–52). MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • le Bomin, S. (2010). The Pygmies of Gabon and their neighbors: Musical systematics, categorization and phylogeny. Paper presented at the International Conference on Congo Basin Hunter-Gatherers, September 22–24, Montpelier, France.

  • Lehmann, L., Feldman, M. W., & Kaeuffer, R. (2010). Cumulative cultural dynamics and the coevolution of cultural innovation and transmission: An ESS model for panmictic and structured populations. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 23, 2356–2369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, J. (2008). Ekila: Blood, Bodies and Egalitarian Societies. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 14, 297–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, J. (2015). Where goods are free but knowledge costs: huntergatherer ritual economics in western central Africa. Hunter Gatherer Research, 1(1), 1–27.

  • Lewis, H. M., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Transmission fidelity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 5367(1599), 2171–2180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lew-Levy, S., & Boyette, A. H. (2018). Evidence for the adaptive learning function of work and work-themed play among Aka forager and Ngandu farmer children from the Congo Basin. Human Nature, 29, 157–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2017). How do hunter-gatherer children learn subsistence skills? A meta-ethnographic review. Human Nature, 28, 367–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liebenberg, L. (1990). The art of tracking: The origin of science. David Phillip.

    Google Scholar 

  • Little, A. C., Caldwell, C. A., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2015). Observer age and the social transmission of attractiveness in humans: Younger women are more influenced by the choices of popular others than older women. British Journal of Psychology, 106(3), 397–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, D. H., & Hewlett, B. S. (1999). Reproductive interests and forager mobility. Current Anthropology, 40, 501–523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, M. (1930). Growing up in New Guinea. Morrow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meehan, C. (2005). The effects of residential locality on parental and alloparental care among the Aka foragers of the Central African Republic. Human Nature, 16(1), 58–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural evolution: How Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morand-Ferron, J., Cole, E. F., Rawles, J. E. C., & Quinn, J. L. (2011). Who are the innovators? A field experiment with 2 passerine species. Behavioral Ecology, 22, 1241–1248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muthukrishna, M., Doebeli, M., Chudek, M., & Henrich, J. (2018). The cultural brain hypothesis: How culture drives brain expansion, sociality, and life history. PLoS Computational Biology, 14(11), e1006504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadel, J., Gue’rini, C., Peze’, A., & Rivet, C. (1999). The evolving nature of imitation as a format for communication. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 209–234). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M. (2012). Imitation, pretend play and childhood: Essential elements in the evolution of human culture? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 170–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., Bennett, G., Subiaul, F., & Zentall, T. (2012). Introduction social learning in humans and nonhuman animals: theoretical and empirical dissections. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 109–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., Tomaselli, K., Mushin, I., & Whiten, A. (2014). Exploring tool innovation: A comparison of Western and Bushman children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 384–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishida, T. (1987). Local traditions and cultural transmission. In S. S. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham, & T. T. Strusaker (Eds.), Primate societies (pp. 462–474). University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M., & Shennan, S. J. (2010a). Series foreword. In M. O’Brien & S. J. Shennan (Eds.), Innovations in cultural systems: Contributions from evolutionary anthropology (pp. vii–ix). MIT Press.

  • O’Brien M., & Shennan, S. J. (2010b). Issues in anthropological studies of innovation. In M. O’Brien & S. J. Shennan (Eds.), Innovations in cultural systems: Contributions from evolutionary anthropology (pp. 1–18). MIT Press.

  • Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012). Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 182–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, A., Shennan, S., & Thomas, M. G. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324(5932), 1298–1301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, A., Shennan, S., & Thomas, M. G. (2010). Demography and variation in the accumulation of culturally inherited skills. In M. O’Brien & S. J. Shennan (Eds.), Innovations in cultural systems contributions from evolutionary anthropology (pp. 137–173). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinlan, M. (2005). Considerations for collecting freelists in the field: Examples from ethnobotany. Field Methods, 17, 219–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, N. (2018). Advances in culture theory from psychological anthropology. Springer International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. (2001). Primate innovation: sex, age and social rank differences. International Journal of Primatology, 22(5), 787–805.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. N. (2002). Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 99, 4436–4441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. N. (2003). Animal innovation: an introduction. In S. M. Reader & K. N. Laland (Eds.), Animal innovation (pp. 3–35). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyes-García, V., Broesch, J., Calvet-Mir, L., Fuentes-Peláez, N., McDade, T. W., Parsa, S., Tanner, S., Huanca, T., Leonard, W. R., & Martínez-Rodríguez, M. R. (2009). Cultural transmission of ethnobotanical knowledge and skills: An empirical analysis from an Amerindian society. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 274–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyes-García, V., Demps, K., & Gallois, S. (2016). A multistage learning model for cultural transmission: Evidence from three indigenous societies. In B. S. Hewlett & H. Terashima (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives (pp. 47–60). Springer Japan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyes-García, V., Zurro, D., Caro, J., & Madella, M. (2017). Small-scale societies and environmental transformations: coevolutionary dynamics. Ecology and Society, 22(1), 15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheinfeldt, L. B., Soi, S., Lambert, C., Ko, W.-Y., Coulibaly, A., Ranciaro, A., Thompson, S., Hirbo, J., Beggs, W., Ibrahim, M., Nyambo, T., Omar, S., Woldemeskel, D., Belay, G., Froment, A., Kim, J., & Tishkoff, S. A. (2019). Genomic evidence for shared common ancestry of East African hunting-gathering populations and insights into local adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(10), 4166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnoebelen, T. (2009). (Un)classifying Shabo: phylogenetic methods and results. In P. K. Austin, O. Bond, M. Charette, D. Nathan, & P. Sells (Eds.), Proceedings of conference on language documentation and linguistic theory 2. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. J., & Steele, J. (1999). Cultural learning in hominids: A behavioural ecological approach. In H. Box & K. Gibson (Eds.), Mammalian social learning: Comparative and ecological perspectives (pp. 367–88). Symposia of the Zoological Society of London 72. Cambridge University Press.

  • Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture, and the problem of meaning. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. K. (1982). Does play matter? Functional and evolutionary aspects of animal and human play. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5(1), 139–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spikins, P., Hitchens, G., Needham, A., & Rutherford, H. (2014). The cradle of thought: growth, learning, play and attachment in Neanderthal children. Oxford Journal of Archaeology, 33, 111–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spisak, B. R., Grabo, A. E., Arvey, R. D., & van Vugt, M. (2014). The age of exploration and exploitation: Younger-looking leaders endorsed for change and older-looking leaders endorsed for stability. The Leadership Quarterly, 25(5), 805–816.

  • Tamnes, C. K., Ostby, Y., Fjell, A. M., Westlye, L., Due-Tonnessen, P., & Walhovd, K. B. (2010). Brain maturation in adolescence and young adulthood: regional age-related changes in cortical thickness and white matter volume and microstructure. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 534–548.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taye, K. T. (2015). Documentation and grammatical description of Chabu. PhD thesis, Addis Ababa University.

  • Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological, 364, 2405–2415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, A., & Raihani, N. J. (2008). The evolution of teaching. Animal Behaviour, 75, 1823–1836.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, A., & Samson, J. (2012). Innovative problem solving in wild meerkats. Animal Behavior, 83, 1459–1468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A., & Ratner, H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 16, 450–548.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, R., Hill, K., Kaplan, H., & McMillan, G. (2002). Age-dependency in skill, strength and hunting ability among the Ache of eastern Paraguay. Journal of Human Evolution, 42, 639–657.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I would like to express my gratitude to the Aka and Chabu people with whom I conducted this research. And for the thorough, careful reading and thoughtful contributions of Barry Hewlett, Zachary Garfield and the four reviewers. Lastly, to JSPS Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas “Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans: Testing Evolutionary Models of Learning” Grant headed by Takeru Akazawa.

Funding

This work was fully funded by Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (Hideaki Tereshima, PI) Grant-in-aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bonnie Hewlett.

Ethics declarations

Ethics Approval

Certification of Exemption, IRB No. 12339. Based on the Exemption Determination Application submitted for the studies “Social Learning among Aka Adolescents” and “Social Learning among Chabu Adolescents,” the WSU Office of Research Assurances has determined that the study satisfies the criteria for Exempt Research at 45 CFR 46.101(b)(2). This study was conducted according to the protocol described in the Application and reviewed by the IRB.

Consent to Participate

All informed consent forms, both written and verbal, were obtained from individuals to participate in the study prior to study and were freely given from participants (or their parent or legal guardian in the case of children under 16).

Consent for Publication

The author affirms that human research participants’ parents provided informed verbal consent of Figs. 2 and 3.

Competing Interests

The author declares no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hewlett, B. Social Learning and Innovation in Adolescence. Hum Nat 32, 239–278 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09391-y

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09391-y

Keywords

Navigation