Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Cognitive Evolution, Population, Transmission, and Material Culture

  • Long Article
  • Published:
Biological Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There has been much debate regarding when modern human cognition arose. It was previously thought that the technocomplexes and artifacts associated with a particular timeframe during the Upper Paleolithic could provide a proxy for identifying the signature of modern cognition. It now appears that this approach has underestimated the complexity of human behavior on a number of different levels. As the artifacts, once thought to be confined to Europe 40,000 years ago onwards, can now be found in other parts of the world well before this date, especially in South Africa, this suggests that modern cognition arose well before this period. Moreover, the variability of the archaeological record from the time when anatomically modern humans appeared 200,000 years ago suggests cognitive factors alone are unable to explain the obvious unevenness. In this article, it will be demonstrated how neuro-cognition can be assimilated with population dynamics and the transmission of information between individuals and groups that can provide important insights as to the nature and origins of modern human cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barham LS (2002) Systematic pigment use in the Middle Pleistocene of south-central Africa. Curr Anthropol 43:181–190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barham LS (2007) Modern is as modern does? Technological trends and thresholds in the South-central African record. In: Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O, Stringer C (eds) Rethinking the human revolution. McDonald Institute Monographs, Cambridge, pp 165–176

    Google Scholar 

  • Barham LS, Pinto Llona AC, Stringer CB (2002) Bone tools from Broken Hill (Kabwe) cave, Zambia, and their evolutionary significance. Before farming [online version] 2002/2 article 3 http://www.waspress.co.uk/journals/beforefarming/journal_20022/abstracts/papers/20022_03_s.pdf

  • Belfer-Cohen A, Goring-Morris AN (2011) Becoming farmers: the inside story. Curr Anthropol 52(S(4)):S209–S220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (2005) Culture, adaptation, and innateness. In: Carruthers P, Stich S, Laurence S (eds) The innate mind: culture and cognition. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 22–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Bressler SL, Menon V (2010) Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci 14:277–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan SF, Salwiczek L, Bshary R (2010) The interplay of cognition and cooperation. Philos T R Soc B 365:2699–2710

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown KS, Marean CW, Jacobs Z, Schoville BJ, Oestmo S, Fisher EC, Bernatchez J, Karkanas P, Matthews T (2012) An early and enduring advanced technology originating 71,000 years ago in South Africa. Nature. doi:10.1038/nature11660

  • Bruner E (2003) Fossil traces of the human thought: palaeoneurology and the evolution of the genus Homo. J Anthropol Sci 81:29–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E (2004) Geometric morphometrics and paleoneurology: brain shape evolution in the genus Homo. J Hum Evol 47:279–303

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E (2010) Morphological differences in the parietal lobes within the human genus: a neurofunctional perspective. Curr Anthropol 51(S(1)):S77–S88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E, Manzi G, Arsuaga JL (2003) Encephalization and allometric trajectories in the genus Homo: evidence from the Neanderthal and modern lineages. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100(26)15335–15340

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E, Martin-Loeches M, Colom R (2010) Human midsagittal brain shape variation: patterns, allometry and integration. J Anat 216:589–599

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum LJ, Kyle KM, Menon R (2005) On beyond mirror neurons: internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans. Cogn Brain Res 25:226–239

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catmur C, Walsh V, Heyes CM (2007) Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system. Curr Biol 17:1527–1531

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catmur C, Walsh V, Heyes CM (2009) Associative sequence learning: the role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system. Philos T R Soc B 364:2369–2380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Churchill SE (2001) Hand morphology, manipulation, and tool use in Neanderthals and early modern humans of the Near East. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:2953–2955

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole J (2012) The identity model: a theory to access visual display and hominin cognition within the Palaeolithic. Hum Orig 1:24–40. https://humanorigins.soton.ac.uk/

  • Coolidge F, Wynn T (2009) The Rise of Homo sapiens: the evolution of modern thinking. Wiley-Blackwell, Chichester

    Google Scholar 

  • Coqueugniot H, Hublin J–J, Veillon F, Houёt F, Jacob T (2004) Early brain growth in Homo erectus and implications for cognitive ability. Nature 431:299–302

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico F (2003) The invisible frontier: a multiple species model for the origin of behavioral modernity. Evol Anthropol 12:188–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico F, Nowell A (2000) A new look at the Berekhat Ram figurine: implications for the origins of symbolism. Camb Archaeol J 10:1–31

    Google Scholar 

  • de Leon MSP, Golovanova L, Doronichev V, Romanova G, Akazawa T, Kondo O, Ishida H, Zollikofer CPE (2008) Neanderthal brain size at birth provides insights into the evolution of human life history. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:13764–13768

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico F, Stringer CB (2011) Evolution, revolution or saltation scenario for the emergence of modern cultures? Philos T R Soc B 366:1060–1069

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dubreuil B (2010) Paleolithic public goods games: why human culture and cooperation did not evolve in one step. Biol Philos 25:53–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM (2009) Mind the gap: or why humans aren’t just great apes. Proc Br Acad 154:403–423

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM (2012) The social brain meets neuroimaging. Trends Cogn Sci 16:101–102

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elston GN, Benavides-Piccione R, Elston A, Zietsch B, Defelipe J, Manger P, Casagrande V, Kaas JH (2006) Specializations of the granular prefrontal cortex of primates: implications for cognitive processing. Anat Rec A 288A:26–35

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enquist M, Strimling P, Eriksson K, Laland K, Sjostrand J (2010) One cultural parent makes no culture. Anim Behav 79:1353–1362

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari PF, Rozzi S, Fogassi L (2005) Mirror neurons responding to observation of actions made with tools in monkey ventral premotor cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 17:213–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey SH (2007) Neurological specializations for manual gesture and tool use in humans. In: Kass J (ed) Evolution of nervous systems. Elsevier, New York, pp 395–406

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Frey SH (2008) Tool use, communicative gesture and cerebral asymmetries in the modern human brain. Philos T R Soc B 363:1951–1957

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese V (2007) Before and below “theory of mind”: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition. Philos T R Soc B 362:659–669

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gamble C (2003) Comment on C. Henshilwood and C. Marean, The Origin of modern human behavior. Curr Anthropol 43:638–639

    Google Scholar 

  • Gamble C, Gowlett J, Dunbar R (2011) The social brain and the shape of the Palaeolithic. Camb Archaeol J 21:115–135

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik A, Seiver E (2009) Reading minds: how infants come to understand others. Zero to Three 30(2):28–32

    Google Scholar 

  • Grèzes J, Costes N, Decety J (1998) Top down effect of the strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a PET investigation. Cogn Neuropsychol 15:553–582

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grove M, Coward F (2008) From individual neurons to social brains. Camb Archaeol J 18:387–400

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunz P, Neubauer S, Golovanova L, Doronichev V, Maureille B, Hublin JJ (2012) A uniquely modern human pattern of endocranial development. Insights from a new cranial reconstruction of the Neandertal newborn from Mezmaiskaya. J Hum Evol 62:300–313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J (2004) Demography and cultural evolution: why adaptive cultural processes produced maladaptive losses in Tasmania. Am Antiquity 69:197–221

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J (2006) Understanding cultural evolutionary models: a reply to Read’s critique. Am Antiquity 71:771–782

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, McElreath R (2003) The evolution of cultural evolution. Evol Anthropol 12:123–135

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood CS (2007) Fully symbolic Sapiens behavior: innovation in the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa. In: Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O, Stringer C (eds) Rethinking the human revolution. McDonald Institute Monographs, Cambridge, pp 123–132

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood CS, d’Errico F, Watts I (2009) Engraved ochres from the Middle Stone Age levels at Blombos Cave, South Africa. J Hum Evol 57:27–47

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes CM (2003) Four routes of cognitive evolution. Psychol Rev 110:713–727

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes CM (2009) Evolution, development and intentional control of imitation. Philos T R Soc B 364:2293–2298

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes C (2010) Where do mirror neurons come from? Neurosci Biobehav R 3:575–583

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes CM (2011a) What’s social about social learning? J Comp Psychol. doi:10.1037/a0025180

  • Heyes CM (2011b) Cultural inheritance of cultural learning. In: Workshop presented at University of Oxford. New thinking: advances in the study of human cognitive evolution. http://media.podcasts.ox.ac.uk/socanth/ehc2011/ehc00_intro.mp3

  • Hill K, Barton M, Hurtado AM (2009) The emergence of human uniqueness: characters underlying behavioral modernity. Evol Anthropol 18:187–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson D (2000) Art, perception and information processing: an evolutionary perspective. Rock Art Res 17:3–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson D (2006) Understanding the origins of palaeoart: the neurovisual resonance theory and brain functioning. Paleoanthropology http://www.paleoanthro.org/journal/content/PA20060054.pdf

  • Hodgson D (2009) Evolution of the visual cortex and the emergence of symmetry in the Acheulean techno-complex. C R Palevol 8:93–97

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson D (2010) Determining the behavioral profile of early modern humans: assimilating population dynamics and cognitive abilities. Before Farm 2(1) http://www.waspjournals.com/journals/beforefarming/about/index.php

  • Hodgson D (2012) Hominin tool production, neural integration and the social brain. Hum Orig 1:41–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Hovers E, Belfer-Cohen A (2007) “Now you see it, now you don’t”—modern human behavior in the Middle Paleolithic. In: Hovers E, Kuhn S (eds) Transitions before the transition—evolution and stability in the Middle Paleolithic and Middle Stone Age. Springer, New York, pp 295–304

    Google Scholar 

  • Iriki A (2006) The neural origins and implications of imitation, mirror neurons and tool use. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:660–667

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston-Frey S (2004) The neural bases of complex tool use in humans. Trends Cogn Sci 8:71–78

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keysers C, Gazzola V (2011) Social neuroscience: mirror neurons recorded in humans. Curr Biol 20:353–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilner JM, Neal N, Weiskopf N, Friston KJ, Frith CD (2009) Evidence of mirror neurons in human inferior frontal gyrus. J Neurosci 29:10153–10159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein R (2000) Archaeology and the evolution of human behavior. Evol Anthropol 9:17–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein R (2001) Southern Africa and modern human origins. J Anthropol Res 57:1–16

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline MA, Boyd R (2010) Population size predicts technological complexity in Oceania. P R Soc Lond B 277:2559–2564

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Atton N, Webster MM (2011) From fish to fashion: experimental and theoretical insights into the evolution of culture. Philos T R Soc B 366:958–968

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombard M (2012) Thinking through the Middle Stone Age of sub-Saharan Africa. Quatern Int 270:140–155

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombard M, Haidle MN (2012) Thinking a bow-and-arrow set: cognitive implications of Middle Stone Age bow and stone-tipped arrow technology. Camb Archaeol J 22:237–264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mania D, Mania U (2005) The natural and socio-cultural environment of Homo erectus at Bilzingsleben, Germany. In: Gamble CS, Porr M (eds) The hominin individual in context: archaeological investigations of lower and middle Palaeolithic landscapes, locales and artifacts. Routledge, London, pp 98–114

    Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty S (2007) Down with the revolution. In: Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O, Stringer C (eds) Rethinking the human revolution. McDonald Institute Monographs, Cambridge, pp 133–151

    Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty S, Brooks AS (2000) The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. J Hum Evol 39:453–563

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNabb J (2012) The importance of conveying visual information in Acheulean society: the background to the visual display hypothesis. Hum Orig 1:1–23

    Google Scholar 

  • McNally L, Brown SP, Jackson AL (2012) Cooperation and the evolution of intelligence. P R Soc Lond B. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.0206

  • Mellars P (1991) Cognitive changes and the emergence of modern humans in Europe. Camb Archaeol J 1:63–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellars P (1999) The Neanderthal problem continued. Curr Anthropol 40:341–364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellars P (2005) The impossible coincidence: a single-species model for the origins of modern human behavior in Europe. Evol Anthropol 14:12–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellars P, French JC (2011) Tenfold population increase in Western Europe at the Neandertal to modern human transition. Science 333:623–627

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mithen S (1996) The prehistory of the mind. Thames and Hudson, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll J, de Oliveria-Souza R, Passman LJ, Cunha FC, Souza-Lima F, Andreinolo PA (2000) Functional MRI correlates of real and imagined tool-use pantomimes. Neurology 54:1331–1336

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niewoehner WA (2001) Behavioral inferences from the Skhul Qafzeh early modern human hand remains. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:2979–2984

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petraglia M, Clarkson C, Boivin N, Haslam M, Korisettar R, Chaubey G, Ditchfield P, Fuller D, James H, Jones S, Kivisild R, Koshy J, Lahr MM, Metspalu M, Roberts R, Arnold L (2009) Population increase and environmental deterioration correspond with microlithic innovations in South Asia ca. 35,000 years ago. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:12261–12266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell A, Shennan SJ, Thomas MG (2009) Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324:1298–1301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell A, Shennan SJ, Thomas MG (2010) Demography and variation in the accumulation of culturally inherited skills. In: O’Brian MJ, Shennan SJ (eds) Innovation in cultural systems: contributions from evolutionary anthropology. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 137–160

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramayya AG, Glasser MF, Rilling JK (2010) A DTI investigation of neural substrates supporting tool use. Cereb Cortex 20:507–516

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rendell L, Boyd R, Enquist M, Feldman MW, Fogarty L, Laland KN (2011) How copying affects the amount, evenness and persistence of cultural knowledge: insights from the social learning strategies tournament. Philos T R Soc B 366:1118–1128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson PJ, Boyd R (2005) Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V (2001) Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:661–670

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth G, Dicke U (2005) Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Trends Cogn Sci 9:250–257

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozzi FVR, de Castro JMB (2004) Surprisingly rapid growth in Neanderthals. Nature 428:936–939

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shea N (2009) Imitation as an inheritance system. Philos T Roy Soc B 364:2429–2443

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shennan SJ (2001) Demography and cultural innovation: a model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Camb Archaeol J 11:5–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shennan SJ (2008) Canoes and cultural evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:3175–3176

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shennan SJ, Steele J (1999) Cultural learning in hominids: a behavioral ecological approach. In: Box H, Gibson K (eds) Mammalian social learning: comparative and ecological perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 367–388

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherwood CC, Stimpson CD, Raghanti MA, Wildmand DE, Uddin M, Grossmand LI, Goodmanb M, Redmondh JC, Bonar CJ, Erwin JM, Hofk PR (2006) Evolution of increased glia–neuron ratios in the human frontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:13606–13611

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith TM, Tafforeau P, Reid D, Gru R, Eggins S, Boutakiout M, Hublin JJ (2007) Earliest evidence of modern human life history in North African early Homo sapiens. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:6128–6133

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith TM, Tafforeau P, Reid DJ, Pouech J, Lazzari V, Zermeno JP, Guatelli-Steinberg D, Olejniczak AJ, Hoffman A, Radovčić J, Makaremi M, Toussaint M, Stringer C, Hublin JJ (2010) Dental evidence for ontogenetic differences between modern humans and Neanderthals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010906107

  • Sowell ER, Trauner DA, Gamst A, Jernigan TL (2002) Development of cortical and subcortical brain structures in childhood and adolescence: a structural MRI study. Dev Med Child Neurol 44:4–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stout D (2002a) Thinking and doing in cognitive archaeology: giving skill its due. Commentary on “Wynn: Archaeology and cognitive evolution”. Behav Brain Sci 25:421–422

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stout D (2002b) Skill and cognition in stone tool production: an ethnographic case study from Irian Jaya. Curr Anthropol 45:693–722

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Straus LG (2009) Has the notion of “transitions” in Paleolithic prehistory outlived its usefulness? The European record in wider context. In: Camps M, Chauhan P (eds) Sourcebook of Paleolithic transitions. Springer, New York, pp 3–18

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Teufel C, Fletcher PC, Davis G (2010) Seeing other minds: attributed mental states influence perception. Trends Cogn Sci 14:376–382

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thieme H (2005) The Lower Palaeolithic art of hunting: the case of Schöningen 13 11–4, Lower Saxony, Germany. In: Gamble C, Porr M (eds) The hominid individual in context: archaeological investigations of Lower and Middle Palaeolithic landscapes locales and artefacts. Routledge, Abingdon, Oxon, pp 115–132

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Turk M, Dimkaroski L (2011) Neandertalska piščal iz Divjih bab I: stara in nova spoznanja [Neanderthal flute from Divje babe I: old and new findings]. In: Toškan B (ed) Drobci ledenodobnega okolja. Zbornik ob življenjskem jubileju Ivana Turka [Fragments of Ice Age environments. Proceedings in Honour of Ivan Turk’s Jubilee] Založba ZRC, ZRC SAZU, Ljubljana, pp 251–265

  • Whiten A (2011) The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and ancestral apes. Philos T Roy Soc B 366:997–1007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten A, Byrne RW (1997) Machiavellian intelligence II: evaluations and extensions. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins J, Schoville BJ, Kyle S, Brown KS, Chazan M (2012) Evidence for early hafted hunting technology. Science 338:942–945

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams JHG, Whiten A, Suddendorf T, Perrett DI (2001) Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neurosci Biobehav R 25:287–295

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T, Coolidge F (2007) Did a small but significant enhancement in working memory capacity power the evolution of modern thinking? In: Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O, Stringer C (eds) Rethinking the human revolution. McDonald Institute Monographs, Cambridge, pp 79–90

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2010) How Levallios reduction is similar to, and not similar to, playing chess. In: Nowell A, Davidson I (eds) Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition. University Press, Colorado, pp 83–103

  • Zilhão J (2007) The emergence of ornaments and art: an archaeological perspective on the origins of “behavioral Modernity”. J Archaeol Res 15:1–54

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Thanks to Marlize Lombard for helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Derek Hodgson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hodgson, D. Cognitive Evolution, Population, Transmission, and Material Culture. Biol Theory 7, 237–246 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-012-0074-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-012-0074-y

Keywords

Navigation