Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Dead Certain

Confidence and Conservatism Predict Aggression in Simulated International Crisis Decision-Making

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Evolutionary psychologists have suggested that confidence and conservatism promoted aggression in our ancestral past, and that this may have been an adaptive strategy given the prevailing costs and benefits of conflict. However, in modern environments, where the costs and benefits of conflict can be very different owing to the involvement of mass armies, sophisticated technology, and remote leadership, evolved tendencies toward high levels of confidence and conservatism may continue to be a contributory cause of aggression despite leading to greater costs and fewer benefits. The purpose of this paper is to test whether confidence and conservatism are indeed associated with greater levels of aggression—in an explicitly political domain. We present the results of an experiment examining people’s levels of aggression in response to hypothetical international crises (a hostage crisis, a counter-insurgency campaign, and a coup). Levels of aggression (which range from concession to negotiation to military attack) were significantly predicted by subjects’ (1) confidence that their chosen policy would succeed, (2) score on a liberal-conservative scale, (3) political party affiliation, and (4) preference for the use of military force in real-world U.S. policy toward Iraq and Iran. We discuss the possible adaptive and maladaptive implications of confidence and conservatism for the prospects of war and peace in the modern world.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Following previous work (Johnson 2004; Johnson and Tierney 2011), we define confidence as the perceived probability that a specified outcome will occur. For example, low confidence may equate with a belief that one has a 25% chance of victory, while high confidence may equate with a belief that one has a 75% chance of victory. Overconfidence is defined as a level of confidence that exceeds the true likelihood of an outcome. For example, if a tennis player expects to win 75% of their matches but loses them all, this would imply overconfidence.

  2. Locus of control is a measure of how much control people perceive they have over the world. People with a relatively “internal” locus of control believe that they and their actions can influence events, whereas people with a relatively “external” locus of control tend to be more fatalist and to believe they cannot influence events, which are instead seen to be controlled by the environment, other people, or some higher power.

  3. Here, comparing the four-level preference for bombing Iran across the seven-level liberal-conservative scale might be better tested using a chi-square test, but the result is the same: χ 2 = 51.2, df = 18, p < 0.0001.

  4. Note that since Republicans/conservatives were more aggressive in the crisis scenarios, it may seem unsurprising that aggression should also correlate with preferences in Iraq and Iran—given that both were associated with President George W. Bush’s presidency. While certainly a plausible explanation for these specific results, it further highlights the central finding that conservatives were more aggressive in the crisis scenarios.

References

  • Achen, C. H. (2002). Parental socialization and rational party identification. Political Behavior, 24(2), 151–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akerlof, G. A. & Shiller R. J. (2009) Animal Spirits: How Human Psychology Drivers the Economy, and Why it Matters for Global Capitalism (Princeton: Princeton University Press).

  • Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. (1950). The authoritarian personality. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alford, J. R., Funk, C. L., & Hibbing, J. R. (2005). Are political orientations genetically transmitted? American Political Science Review, 99(2), 153–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of freedom: Understanding right-wing authoritarianism. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amodio, D. M., Jost, J. T., Master, S. L., & Yee, C. M. (2007). Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1246–1247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (1998). Situated optimism: specific outcome expectancies and self-regulation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 309–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barber, B. M., & Odean, T. (2001). Boys will be boys: gender, overconfidence, and common stock investment. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116, 261–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.). (1992). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (1989). The optimal margin of illusion. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 176–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-David, I., Graham, J. R., & Harvey, C. R. (2006). Managerial overconfidence and corporate policies. Fuqua School of Business, Duke University, Working Paper No. 81.

  • Bennett, D. S., & Stam, A. C. (2004). The behavioral origins of war. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blainey, G. A. (1973). The causes of war. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C., & Lovallo, D. (1999). Overconfidence and excess entry: an experimental approach. The American Economic Review, 89(1), 306–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J. (2007). Cognitive dissonance: 50 years of a classic theory. New York: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draper, R. (2007). Dead certain: The presidency of George W. Bush. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dueck, C. (2008). Reluctant crusaders: power, culture, and change in American grand strategy.

  • Dunning, D., Johnson, K., Ehrlinger, J., & Kruger, J. (2003). Why people fail to recognize their own incompetence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(3), 83–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlinger, J., Johnson, K., Banner, M., Dunning, D., & Kruger, J. (2008). Why the unskillled are unaware. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 105(1), 98–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Etheredge, L. (1978a). A world of men: The private sources of American foreign policy. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Etheredge, L. (1978b). Personality effects on American foreign policy, 1898–1968. American Political Science Review, 72(2), 434–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fallows, J. (2004). Blind into Baghdad. The Atlantic (January/February), 53–74.

  • Fearon, J. D. (1995). Rationalist explanations for war. International Organization, 49(3), 379–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feaver, P., & Gelpi, C. (2004). Choosing your battles: American civil-military relations and the use of force. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2007). Social cognition: From brains to culture. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, J. H., Settle, J. E., & Dawes, C. T. (2009). The heritability of partisan attachment. Political Research Quarterly, 62(3), 601–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gartner, S. S. (1997). Strategic assessment in war. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gat, A. (2006). War in human civilization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (2011). Mindset theory of action phases. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanksi, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, D. W., & Tversky, A. (2002). The weighing of evidence and the determinants of confidence. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 230–249). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatemi, P. K., Hibbing, J., & Alford, J. (2009). Is there a "party" in your genes? Political Research Quarterly, 62(3), 584–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatemi, P. K., et al. (2011). A genome-wide analysis of political attitudes. Journal of Politics, 73, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, M., McDermott, R., & Stam, A. (2005). Leader age, regime type and violence. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 49(5), 661–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jervis, R. (1976). Perception and misperception in international politics. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jervis, R. (2003). The confrontation between Iraq and the US: implications for the theory and practice of deterrence. European Journal of International Relations, 9(2), 315–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. D. P. (2004). Overconfidence and war: The havoc and glory of positive illusions. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. D. P., & Fowler, J. (2011). The evolution of overconfidence. Nature, 477, 317–320. doi:10.1038/nature10384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. D. P., & Tierney, D. R. (2006). Failing to win: Perceptions of victory and defeat in international politics. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. D. P., & Tierney, D. R. (2007). In the eye of the beholder: victory and defeat in U.S. military operations. In J. Angstrom & I. Duyvesteyn (Eds.), Understanding victory and defeat in contemporary war (pp. 46–76). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. D. P., & Tierney, D. R. (2011). The Rubicon theory of war: how the path to conflict reaches the point of no return. International Security, 36, 7–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. D. P., McDermott, R., Barrett, E., Cowden, J., Wrangham, R., McIntyre, M., et al. (2006). Overconfidence in wargames: experimental evidence on expectations, aggression, gender and testosterone. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 273(1600), 2513–2520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. D. P., Weidmann, N. B., & Cederman, L.-E. (2011). Fortune favours the bold: an agent-based model reveals adaptive advantages of overconfidence in war. PLoS One, 6(6), e20851.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Renshon, J. (2006). Why Hawks Win. Foreign Policy (158, January/February), 34–38.

  • Kanter, R. M. (2004). Confidence: How winning streaks and losing streaks begin and end. New York: Crown Business.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keeley, L. H. (1996). War before civilization: The myth of the peaceful savage. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, G., Tomz, M., & Wittenberg, J. (2000). Making the most of statistical analyses: improving interpretation and presentation. American Journal of Political Science, 44, 341–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, J. M., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1121–1134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeBlanc, S., & Register, K. E. (2003). Constant battles: The myth of the peaceful, noble savage. New York: St. Martin's Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lebow, R. N. (1981). Between peace and war: The nature of international crisis. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, J. S. (1983). Misperception and the causes of war: theoretical linkages and analytical problems. World Politics, 36(1), 76–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, J. S., & Thompson, W. R. (2010). Causes of war. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lim, R. G. (1997). Overconfidence in negotiation revisited. International Journal of Conflict Management, 8, 52–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, R. (2004). The feeling of rationality: the meaning of neuroscientific advances for political science. Perspectives on Politics, 2(4), 691–706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, R. (2007). Presidential leadership, illness, and decision making. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, R., & Cowden, J. (2001). The effects of uncertainty and sex in a simulated crisis game. International Interactions, 27, 353–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, R., & Cowden, J. (2008). The role of hostile communications in a crisis simulation game. Peace and Conflict, 14, 151–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, R., Johnson, D. D. P., Cowden, J., & Rosen, S. (2007). Testosterone and aggression in a simulated crisis game. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Sciences, 614(1), 15–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mearsheimer, J. J. (2001). The tragedy of Great Power politics. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, J. E. (1973). War, presidents and public opinion. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oxley, D. R., Smith, K. B., Alford, J. R., Hibbing, M. V., Miller, J. L., Scalora, M., et al. (2008). Political attitudes vary with physiological traits. Science, 321, 1667–1670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. (2006). A primer in positive psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, S. P. (2004). War and human nature. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvador, A., Suay, F., González-Bono, E., & Serrano, M. A. (2003). Anticipatory cortisol, testosterone and psychological responses to judo competition in young men. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28(3), 364–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schreiber, D., Simmons, A. N., Dawes, C. T., Flagan, T., Fowler, J. H., & Paulus, M. P. (2009). Red Brain, Blue Brain: Evaluative Processes Differ in Democrats and Republicans. Paper presented at the American Political Science Association annual meeting, Toronto. Available online at http://dmschreiber.ucsd.edu/Publications/RedBrainBlueBrain.pdf.

  • Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2009). Formidability and the logic of human anger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(35), 15073–15078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sell, A., Hone, L., & Pound, N. (2012). The importance of physical strength to human males. Human Nature, 23(1), doi to be added in proofs. doi: doi:10.1007/s12110-012-9131-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoessinger, J. G. (1998). Why nations go to war. New York: St. Martin's.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E. (1989). Positive illusions: Creative self-deception and the healthy mind. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1994). Positive illusions and well-being revisited: separating fact from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 21–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., Sage, R. M., & McDowell, N. K. (2003). Portrait of the self-enhancer: well adjusted and well liked or maladjusted and friendless. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), 165–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. L. (2000). The elements of a scientific theory of self-deception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 907, 114–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. L. (2011). Deceit and self-deception: Fooling yourself the better to fool others. London: Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuchman, B. W. (1984). The march of folly: from Troy to Vietnam. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van den Steen, E. (2004). Rational overoptimism (and other biases). American Economic Review, 94, 1141–1151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Evera, S. (1999). Causes of war: Power and the roots of conflict. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vasquez, J. A. (1993). The war puzzle. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vasquez, J. A. (Ed.). (2000). What do we know about war? Lanham: Rowan & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Clausewitz, C. (1976). On war (M. Howard & P. Paret, Trans.). Princeton: Princeton University Press. (Originally published in 1832)

  • Walt, S. M. (2011). Wishful thinking: Top 10 examples of the most unrealistic expectations in contemporary U.S. foreign policy. Foreign Policy, April 29, http://www.foreignpolicy.com/articles/2011/2004/2029/wishful_thinking.

  • Waltz, K. N. (1979). Theory of international politics. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, R. K. (1968). Nobody wanted war: Misperception in Vietnam and other wars. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D. (2004). Strangers to ourselves: discovering the adaptive unconscious. Cambridge: Belknap.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, B. (2005). State of denial. NY: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. W. (1999). Is military incompetence adaptive? Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 3–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. W., & Peterson, D. (1996). Demonic males: Apes and the origins of human violence. London: Bloomsbury.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank David Carrier and three anonymous referees for their help in improving the manuscript. DJ thanks the Branco Weiss Society In Science Fellowship for funding. Finally, we especially thank Elizabeth Cashdan for her encouragement and advice on the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dominic D. P. Johnson.

Appendix

Appendix

International Crises Scenarios

For each of the following scenarios, please read the description carefully. Then please make a decision about what you would do in this situation. As much as possible, try to really imagine being the leader confronted with this decision. Do not act as if you were any particular leader, but rather act as yourself in the role of leader. In each decision, you will be asked to mark your most preferred decision option. There is no right answer to any of these crisis scenarios.

[New page in original]

Hostage Scenario

A commercial airliner en route from New York to Tel Aviv, Israel, is hijacked and forced to land at the Riyadh International Airport in Saudi Arabia. The passengers are predominantly Israeli, but a number of Americans and other nationals are on board as well.

You know that the hijackers are Palestinians, but their specific affiliation is not clear at this point. They are demanding the release of a number of Palestinians held in Israeli jails. They also want safe passage for themselves to an undisclosed location.

The relevant parties are Israel, the Palestinian hijackers, the Saudis, and the Americans. Each group has a set of objectives. The Israelis primarily want the safe return of the passengers. They also want to maintain the credibility of their deterrent against terrorism and to make sure that their strategic interests remain unchanged. The Palestinian hijackers primarily want the release of their nationals from Israeli prisons. They also want to damage Israel’s image internationally and to assure their own safe passage out of Saudi Arabia. The Saudis primarily want to demonstrate their control of the situation and to maintain control among their own people. They would also like to maintain the status quo in their relations with other Arab nations. The Americans would like to assure the safety of their passengers, deter similar terrorist actions in the future, and broker this situation to a successful conclusion to demonstrate international leadership.

QUESTION A

As the president of the United States, faced with this situation, what is your most preferred option for action?

__ 1. Encourage a Special Forces assault in an attempt to rescue the hostages.

__ 2. Act as a broker between Israel and the Palestinian hijackers to arrange a trade, in which passengers are released in small groups, in return for the release of a few Palestinian prisoners.

__ 3. Put pressure on the Israelis to release the Palestinian prisoners in order to secure the release of the passengers unharmed.

QUESTION B

On a scale of 0 to 10, how likely do you think it is that your chosen course of action will succeed? Please circle your response, where 0 is very unlikely and 10 is very likely.

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

[New page in original]

Central Asian Scenario

The newly established democratic government of Afghanistan has become increasingly unpopular due to the continuing strength of Islamic fundamentalism, economic decline, internal political dissension and violence, and their crackdown on the highly profitable heroin trade. An armed rebellion by millennia-old clan-based [groups led by] warlords has been continuing in the southern and eastern regions. When government troops encircle some of these rebel forces, the resurgent Taliban, in concert with the local warlords, intervene with a sizeable military force. On the third day of the operation, it seems possible from the size and direction of these attacks, that these Taliban forces will once again take over the capital city of Kabul. The resurgent Taliban are calling for an “international brotherhood of Islamic states,” while the Russians warn that “Islamic fundamentalism will not be allowed to set up positions of strength along the Russian border.”

U.S. pentagon military strategists are pessimistic about the ability of the United States to successfully control this military action given the number of American troops that are currently pinned down in Iraq. They see a slim hope for success only if the United States seizes a window of opportunity to interdict key mountain passes and to forestall the resurgent Taliban from setting up air defenses and logistics to support their further operations. Renewed major U.S. military intervention might require the insertion of special mountain forces, conventional bombing, and the possible use of 15,000-ton “daisy cutter” bombs. The U.S. dominates in aerial forces, but is in an unfavorable position in the local military balance on the ground. The latest intelligence data suggests that the mountainous border region between Afghanistan and Pakistan continues to shelter most of the top leaders of Al Queda, including Osama bin Laden. Many Taliban leaders support these terrorists in their campaign against American forces in the region.

The Russians’ main goal is to prevent an unstable situation on its border, but the Russian government would not mind extending its influence throughout the region as well. The United States is reluctant to let the Russians do all the work, in case they take over the territory entirely and move into nearby areas that control important oil pipelines necessary to both the U.S. and Western Europe.

QUESTION A

As the president of the United States, faced with this situation, what is your most preferred option for action?

__ 1. Withdraw American forces from the increasing difficulties in Afghanistan and concede the area to the local Taliban forces, concentrating your military effort on Iraq.

__ 2. Use special mountain forces, conventional bombing, and the possible use of 15,000-ton “daisy cutter” bombs to block the key mountain passes in Afghanistan.

__ 3. Stress diplomatic solutions to the crisis and act to sponsor ceasefire negotiations between the local government, important warlords, and the Russian and Pakistani governments.

QUESTION B

On a scale of 0 to 10, how likely do you think it is that your chosen course of action will succeed? Please circle your response, where 0 is very unlikely and 10 is very likely.

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

[New page in original]

Latin American Scenario

A country in Latin America recently elected a socialist leader who has just socialized the nation’s industries, including a number of important American corporate concerns. This country is the leading exporter of coffee, cocoa, and bananas to the United States. In addition, it is an important supplier of certain raw materials, including copper. As a result, American companies stand to lose billions of dollars, and adequate supplies of these goods into the United States could be disrupted, likely leading to critical shortages of coffee, chocolate, fruit, and plumbing material in America. A strong military leader who is quite sympathetic to American interests is waiting in the wings, eager to take power from the elected socialist leader, but he needs outside help in order to accomplish this goal. As president of the United States, you have been under a great deal of pressure from major corporate businesspeople and important political donors to do something about this situation, and to roll back the government takeover of these American companies. These leaders have threatened to back your opponent in the next election unless you do something to reverse this seizure of American property.

QUESTION A

As the president of the United States, faced with this situation, what is your most preferred option for action?

__ 1. Attempt to negotiate fair restitution for American companies.

__ 2. Let this leader nationalize American interests, but cut diplomatic ties.

__ 3. Allow US special forces and the CIA to assist the local military leader in staging a coup.

QUESTION B

On a scale of 0 to 10, how likely do you think it is that your chosen course of action will succeed? Please circle your response, where 0 is very unlikely and 10 is very likely.

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Johnson, D.D.P., McDermott, R., Cowden, J. et al. Dead Certain. Hum Nat 23, 98–126 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-012-9134-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-012-9134-z

Keywords

Navigation