Skip to main content
Log in

Material representations in mathematical research practice

  • S.I. : MathCogEncul
  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Mathematicians’ use of external representations, such as symbols and diagrams, constitutes an important focal point in current philosophical attempts to understand mathematical practice. In this paper, we add to this understanding by presenting and analyzing how research mathematicians use and interact with external representations. The empirical basis of the article consists of a qualitative interview study we conducted with active research mathematicians. In our analysis of the empirical material, we primarily used the empirically based frameworks provided by distributed cognition and cognitive semantics as well as the broader theory of cognitive integration as an analytical lens. We conclude that research mathematicians engage in generative feedback loops with material representations, that they use representations to facilitate the use of experiences of handling the physical world as a resource in mathematical work, and that their use of representations is socially sanctioned and enabled. These results verify the validity of the cognitive frameworks used as the basis for our analysis, but also show the need for augmentation and revision. Especially, we conclude that the social and cultural context cannot be excluded from cognitive analysis of mathematicians’ use of external representations. Rather, representations are socially sanctioned and enabled in an enculturation process.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The aspects of the interviews not discussed in the analysis in this paper mainly relate to the selection of mathematical problems and the choice of representation connected to different communicative contexts. These aspects of the investigation are reported in Misfeldt and Johansen (2015) and Johansen and Misfeldt (2016). We refer to the analysis from these papers where appropriate. A general description of the educational implication of the study has been given in Johansen and Misfeldt (2014). In contrast to the papers previously published, the paper at hand focuses on cognitive aspects of the use of representations and seeks to understand the relationship between the cognitive and the social aspects of representation use.

  2. It should be noted that we do not interpret this to suggest that mathematics is always performed in solitude behind closed doors as an individual activity. Rather, the point is that the thinking process (alone or in groups) most often involves writing and sketching.

  3. As pointed out by one of the anonymous reviewers the role of diagrams in mathematics is very diverse, and diagrams may also in some cases be used as epistemic artefact in a way similar to symbols (see e.g. De Toffoli and Giardino 2014).

  4. As one of the anonymous reviewers kindly pointed out such dynamic processes are not confined to mathematical reasoning or reasoning with diagrams, but could also occur in connection to other representations (see e.g. Clark 1998).

  5. Note that by this we do not mean that diagrams cannot support formal thinking. Furthermore, certain diagrams can also be subject to purely formal manipulations (for in depth treatment of this aspect, see Shin 1994, Mumma 2010).

  6. In mathematics, a permutation is an operation that reorganizes the sequential ordering of the elements of a set.

  7. See also Carter (2010) where the role played by the same type of circular diagrams in finding and constructing a mathematical proof is discussed in depth.

  8. Square matrices have the same number of rows and columns and will consequently typically have a square typographical outline in the usual representational form. In contrast, in rectangular matrices, the number of rows does not coincide with that of the columns.

References

  • Barany, M. J., & MacKenzie, D. (2014). Chalk: Materials and concepts in mathematics research. In C. Coopman, J. Vertesi, M. Lynch, & S. Woolgar (Eds.), Representation in scientific practice revisited (pp. 107–129). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bloor, D. (1981). Hamilton and Peacock on the essence of algebra. In H. Mehrtens, H. M. Bos, & I. Schneider (Eds.), Social history of nineteenth century mathematics (pp. 202–232). Boston: Birkhäuser.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Burton, L. (2004). Mathematicians as enquirers: Learning about learning mathematics. Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, J. (2010). Diagrams and proofs in analysis. International Studies in the Philosophy of Science, 24(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1080/02698590903467085.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory. London, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1989). Microcognition, philosophy, cognitive science, and parallel distributed processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1998). Magic words: How language augments human computation. In P. Carruthers & J. Boucher (Eds.), Language and thought: Interdisciplinary themes (pp. 162–183). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • De Cruz, H., & De Smedt, J. (2013). Mathematical symbols as epistemic actions. Synthese, 190(1), 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Toffoli, S. (2017). ‘Chasing’ the diagram: The use of visualizations in algebraic reasoning. The Review of Symbolic Logic, 10(1), 158–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Toffoli, S., & Giardino, V. (2014). Roles and forms of diagrams in knot theory. Erkenntnis, 79(3), 829–842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epple, M. (2004). Knot invariants in Vienna and Princeton during the 1920s: Epistemic configurations of mathematical research. Science in Context, 17(1/2), 131–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2002). The way we think, conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 307–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, M. C., Everett, D. L., Fedorenko, E., & Gibson, E. (2008). Number as a cognitive technology: Evidence from Pirahã language and cognition. Cognition, 108(3), 819–824.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greiffenhagen, C. W. K. (2014). The materiality of mathematics: Presenting mathematics at the blackboard. The British Journal of Sociology, 65(3), 502–528. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-4446.12037.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. D., Hutchins, E., & Kirsh, D. (2000). Distributed cognition: Toward a new foundation for human–computer interaction research. ACM Transactions on Computer–Human Interaction, 7(2), 174–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (2005). Material anchors for conceptual blends. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(10), 1555–1577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (2011). Enculturating the supersized mind. Philosophical Studies, 152(3), 437–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johansen, M. W. (2010). Embodied strategies in mathematical cognition. In B. Löwe & T. Müller (Eds.), PhiMSAMP. Philosophy of mathematics: Sociological aspects and mathematical practice, texts in philosophy (Vol. 11, pp. 179–196). London: College Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johansen, M. W. (2014). What’s in a diagram? On the classification of symbols, figures and diagrams. In L. Magnani (Ed.), Model-based reasoning in science and technology (Studies in applied philosophy, epistemology and rational ethics 8) (pp. 89–108). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johansen, M. W., & Misfeldt, M. (2014). Når matematikere undersøger matematik-og hvilken betydning det har for undersøgende matematikundervisning. MONA, 2014(4), 42–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johansen, M. W., & Misfeldt, M. (2015). Semiotic scaffolding in mathematics. Biosemiotics, 8(2), 325–340. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-014-9228-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johansen, M. W., & Misfeldt, M. (2016). An empirical approach to the mathematical values of problem choice and argumentation. In B. Larvor (Ed.), Mathematical cultures: The London meetings 2012–2014 (pp. 259–269). Switzerland: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P. (1994). On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action. Cognitive Science, 18(4), 513–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kjeldsen, T. H. (2009). Egg-forms and measure-bodies: Different mathematical practices in the early history of the modern theory of convexity. Science in Context, 22(1), 85–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kvale, S. (1996). InterViews. An introduction to qualitative research interviewing. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J. H., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words. Cognitive Science, 11(1), 65–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marghetis, T., & Núñez, R. (2010). Dynamic construals, static formalisms: Evidence from co-speech gesture during mathematical proving. In A. Pease, M. Guhe, & A. Smaill (Eds.), Proceedings of the international symposium on mathematical practice and cognition (pp. 23–29). New York: AISB.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. (2015). Mathematical cognition: A case of enculturation. In T. Metzinger & J. M. Windt (Eds.), Open MIND: 25(T). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group. https://doi.org/10.15502/9783958570818.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Misfeldt, M. (2011). Computers as medium for mathematical writing. Semiotica, 186, 239–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Misfeldt, M., & Johansen, M. W. (2015). Research mathematicians’ practices in selecting mathematical problems. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 89(3), 357–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mumma, J. (2010). Proofs, pictures and euclid. Synthese, 175(2), 255–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Núñez, R. (2009). Numbers and arithmetic: Neither hardwired nor out there. Biological Theory, 4(1), 68–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Núñez, R. (2004). Do real numbers really move? Language, thought, and gesture: The embodied cognitive foundations of mathematics. In F. Iida, R. Pfeifer, L. Steels, & Y. Kuniyoshi (Eds.), Embodied artificial intelligence (pp. 54–73). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pycior, H. (1997). Symbols, impossible numbers, and geometric entanglements. British algebra through the commentaries on Newton’s Universal Arithmetick. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schlimm, D., & Neth, H. (2008). Modeling ancient and modern arithmetic practices: Addition and multiplication with Arabic and Roman numerals. In V. Sloutsky, B. Love, & K. McRae (Eds.), 30th Annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 2097–2102). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shin, S. (1994). The logical status of diagrams. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steensen, A. K., & Johansen, M. W. (2016). The role of diagram materiality in mathematics. Cognitive Semiotics, 9(2), 183–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thurston, W. P. (1994). On proof and progress in mathematics. Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society, 30(2), 161–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J., & Norman, D. A. (1995). A representational analysis of numeration systems. Cognition, 57(3), 271–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This paper was developed as part of an investigation of mathematicians’ practice. The investigation and the analysis presented here springs from conversations we have had with a number of mathematicians. These mathematicians were extremely generous with their time and allowed us to learn about their concerns and strategies while conducting mathematical research. We are truly thankful for their indispensable contribution to our work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mikkel W. Johansen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Johansen, M.W., Misfeldt, M. Material representations in mathematical research practice. Synthese 197, 3721–3741 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-018-02033-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-018-02033-4

Keywords

Navigation