Skip to main content
Log in

Cognitivism or Situated-Distributed Cognition? Assessing Kashmiri Carpet Weaving Practice from the Two Theoretical Paradigms

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cognition is predominantly seen as information processing in multidisciplinary landscape of cognition studies, despite having had a formidable opposition from embodied and embedded perspectives in the last few decades. This paper analyses cognitive processes involved in different task domains of Kashmiri carpet weaving practice from the theoretical frameworks of cognitivism and situated-distributed cognition. After introducing the practice and its task domains (Section −1), paradigmatic cognitive activities involved in them are discussed and how these are explained by the two theoretical paradigms and why the latter framework explains these processes better (Section-2). During this discussion (Section – 3), a way via culture impacts the practice and resultant cognitive operations turns out to be workspace of the actors.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We restrict to manual setting in the paper.

  2. The final design of this carpet was later drawn on ‘inch-square graph sheet’ and similar manipulations were observed.

  3. This is a design heuristic used by designers whereby for every feet, 2-in. of border is carved.

  4. The analysis of these sessions is reserved for a separate paper

  5. Since think-aloud sessions are under analysis, it is yet to be seen whether this fact had a systematic influence on the accuracy of the code generation. That distance is reduced, however, is clearly visible.

  6. In single weaver setting, the weaver, after reaching the mid-point on the loom, reads the instruction as right to left, and adjusts actions accordingly.

References

  • Ball, L.J., J. St. B. T Evans, I. Dennis, and T.C. Ormerod. 1997. Problem solving strategies and expertise in engineering design. Thinking and Reasoning 3 (4): 247–270. https://doi.org/10.1080/135467897394284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, T.A. 2012. Initial mental representations of design problems: Differences between experts and novices. Design Studies 34: 135–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2012.08.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bokil, P. (2012). Making grids flexible: A systematic approach to understand the behavior of grid. Typography- 2012. Available at http://www.typoday.in/2012/spk_papers/prasad-bokil-typographyday2012.pdf. Accessed 30 May 2018.

  • Brereton, M.F., D.M. Cannon, A. Mabogunje, and L.J. Leifer. 1996. Collaboration in design teams: How social interaction shapes the product. In Analyzing design activity, ed. N. Cross, K. Dorst, and H. Christiaans, 319–341. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casakin, H., and P. Badke-Schaub. 2017. Sharedness of team mental models in the course of design-related interaction between architects and clients. Design Science: e14. https://doi.org/10.1017/dsj.2017.15.

  • Chandrasekhran, B. 1990. Design problem solving: A task analysis. AI Magazine 11 (4): 59–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cross, N. 2001. Design cognition: Results from protocol and other empirical studies of design activity. In Design knowing and learning: Cognition in design education, ed. C. Eastman, M. McCracken, and W. Newstetter, 79–103. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.

  • Davis, M.C., D.J. Leach, and C.W. Clegg. 2011. The physical environment of the office: Contemporary and emerging issues. In International review of industrial and organizational psychology, ed. G.P. Hodgkinson and J.K. Ford, vol. 26, 193–235. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dorst, K., and J. Dijkhuis. 1995. Comparing paradigms for describing design activity. Design Studies 16: 261–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dumas, D., L.C. Schmidt, and P.A. Alexander. 2016. Predicting creative problem solving in engineering design. Thinking Skills and Creativity 21: 50–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2016.05.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eastman, C. M. (1969). Cognitive processes and ill-defined problems: A case study of design. In Proceedings of the 1st International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence (IJCAI’69), (pp. 669–690). Washington, D. C.

  • Eastman, C.M. 1970. On the analysis of intuitive design processes. In Emerging methods in environmental design and planning, ed. G.T. Moore. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M.A. 1991. Cognitive processes and mechanisms in language comprehension: The structure building framework. In The psychology of learning and motivation, ed. G.H. Bower, 217–263. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gero, J.S. 1994. Towards a model of exploration in computer aided design. In Formal design methods for computer aided design, ed. J.S. Gero and E. Tyugu, 315–336. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollan, J., E. Hutchins and D. Kirsh. 2000. Distributed Cognition: Toward a new foundation for human-computer interaction research. In ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction (TOCHI), vol. 7(2), 174–196. https://doi.org/10.1145/353485.353487.

  • Hutchins, E. 1995. Cognition in the wild. Boston: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. 2006. The distributed cognition perspective on human cognition. In Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction, ed. N.J. Enfield and S.C. Levinson, 375–398. Oxford: Berg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. 2008. The role of cultural practices in the emergence of modern human intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of Royal Society, 363, 2011–2019. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0003.

  • Kamarulzaman, N., A.A. Saleh, S.Z. Hashim, H. Hashim, and A.A. Abdul-Ghani. 2011. An overview of the influence of physical office environments towards employees. Procedia Engineering 20: 262–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.proeng.2011.11.164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Khan, F.A. 1993. Cognitive organization and work activity: A study of carpet weavers in Kashmir. The Quarterly Newsletter of The Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition 15 (2): 48–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., and R. de Dear. 2013. Workspace satisfaction: The privacy-communication trade-off in open-plan offices. Journal of Environmental Psychology 36: 18–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.06.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. 1988. Role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction integration model. Psychological Review 95 (2): 163–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsh, D. 1995. Intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence 73: 31–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsh, D. 2009. Problem solving and situated cognition. In The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition, ed. P. Robbins and M. Ayedede, 264–306. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. 1988. Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics and culture in everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Le Dantec, C.A. 2010. Situating design as social creation and cultural cognition. CoDesign 6 (4): 207–224. https://doi.org/10.1080/15710882.2010.527009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linsey, J.S., A.B. Markman, and K.L. Wood. 2012. Design by analogy: A study of the WordTree method for problem re-representation. Journal of Mechanical Design 134: 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1115/1.4006145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Y.T. 1996. Is designing one search or two? A model of design thinking involving symbolism and connectionism. Design Studies 17: 435–449. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0142-694X(96)00018-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, W. 2003. Problem solving through design. New Directions in Teaching and Learning 95: 39–44. https://doi.org/10.1002/tl.111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J., Kurz-Milcke, E., Newstetter, W. C., & Davies, J. (2003). Research laboratories as evolving distributed cognitive systems. R. Alterman & D. Kirsh (Eds.) Proceedings of 25th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, (pp.857–862).

  • Oldham, G.R., and N.L. Rotchford. 1983. Relationships between office characteristics and employee reactions: A study of the physical environment. Administrative Science Quarterly 28: 542–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orr, J. 1990. Sharing knowledge, celebrating identity: Community memory in a service culture. In Collective remembering inquiries in social construction, ed. D. Middleton and D. Edwards. London: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rashid, M., and C. Zimring. 2003. Organizational constructs and the structure of space: A comparative study of office layouts. In Proceedings of 4th Intl, 43.1–43.20. London: Space Syntax Symposium.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rashid, M., K. Kampschroer, J. Wineman, and C. Zimring. 2006. Spatial layout and face-to-face interaction in offices: A study of the mechanisms of spatial effects on face-to-face interaction. Environment and Planning B: Planning and Design 33: 825–844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, E. 2016. Situating the embodied mind in a landscape of standing affordances for living without chairs: Materializing a philosophical worldview. Sports Medicine 46: 927–932. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-016-0520-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saraf, D.N. 1990. Carpets. In Crafts of Jammu, Kashmir and Ladakh, ed. J. Jaitly, 81–98. Middletown: Grantha Corporation with Mapin Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schon, D.A. 1983. The reflective practitioner: How professionals think in action. USA: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seitamaa-Hakkarainen, P., and K. Hakkaranainen. 2001. Composition and construction in experts’ and novices’ weaving design. Design Studies 22: 44–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0142-694X(99)00038-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shagrir, O. 2006. Why we view brain as a computer. Synthese 153: 393–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. 1971/75. Style in design. In Proceedings of the 2nd AEDRAC, ed. J. Archea and C. Eastman, 1–10. Stroudsbury, PA: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross, Inc.

  • Simon, H. 1995. Problem framing, problem finding and problem solving in design. In Design and systems: General applications of methodology, ed. A. Collen and W.W. Gasparski, vol. 3, 245–257. Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers, New.

  • Suchman, L.A. 2007. Human-machine reconfigurations: Plans and situated actions. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallee-Tourangeau, G., and F. Vallee-Tourangeau. 2013. Cognition beyond the classical information processing model: Cognitive interactivity and the systemic thinking model. In Cognition beyond the brain: Computation, interactivity and human artifice (2nd Ed 2017), ed. S.J. Cowley and F. Vallee-Tourangeau, 133–154. London: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Visser, W. 1995. Use of episodic knowledge and information in design problem solving. Design Studies 16 (2): 171–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, J. 2015. Designing work: A study of collaboration and concentration in open-plan offices. Iterations 2: 44–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yilmaz, S., S.R. Daly, C.M. Seifert, and R. Gonzalez. 2016. Evidence-based design heuristics for idea generation. Design Studies 46: 95–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2016.05.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to Homi Bhabha Fellowships Council, Mumbai for supporting and funding this work and Homi Bhabha Centre For Science Education (HBCSE), TIFR, Mumbai for providing me the required research environment. Part of data collection was done as Postdoctoral Associate in the Consciousness Studies Programme of the National Institute of Advanced Studies (NIAS), Bengaluru. I am thankful to them for funding the fieldwork during 2015. I am thankful to Sajad Nazir and Abdul Rashid Bhat, both in Srinagar, for permitting me to reproduce the extracts of their talim roll and graph in this paper. The written consent of respondents, for reproducing their photographs and works, has been obtained.

Funding

This work has been supported and funded by two-year research fellowship (2017–2019) by Homi Bhabha Fellowships Council, Mumbai, India.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gagan Deep Kaur.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kaur, G.D. Cognitivism or Situated-Distributed Cognition? Assessing Kashmiri Carpet Weaving Practice from the Two Theoretical Paradigms. Rev.Phil.Psych. 11, 917–937 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-019-00455-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-019-00455-8

Navigation