Skip to main content
Log in

Plio-Pleistocene Foundations of Hominin Musicality: Coevolution of Cognition, Sociality, and Music

  • Thematic Issue Article: Symbols, Signals, and the Archaeological Record II
  • Published:
Biological Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Today, music is ubiquitous, highly valued in all known cultures, playing many roles in human daily life. The ethnographic study of the music of extant human foragers makes this quite apparent. Moreover, music is ancient. Sophisticated bird-bone and ivory flutes dated from 40 kya reveal an even earlier musical-technological tradition. So is music likely to be an entrenched feature of human social life during the long passage to behavioral modernity—say, by 150 kya—or earlier? In this article I sketch an evolutionary model that focuses on hominin vocal musicality and communication in the Pleistocene, tracking between series of phenotypes and changes in ecological, social, cognitive, and informational contexts. The model links musicality and protomusic to a bigger picture of hominin socio-cognitive evolution, making some connections clearer, motivating further theorizing and the search for new evidence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See Tomlinson (2015), Fitch (2006), Currie and Killin (2016), Killin (2013, 2016a), in press; Davies in press. Briefly, Tomlinson’s worry is that the adaptationist hypotheses propose a unitary functional explanation for a manifold phenomenon (i.e., musicality in its many guises) and that in hybrid accounts like Mithen’s, the multiple functions proposed “sit uneasily side by side” (Tomlinson 2015, p. 33). Fitch argues that purported progress in the adaptation/by-product debate is an unproductive distraction from researching the biology and cognition of musicality. Moreover, I have argued that the usefulness of the adaptation/by-product/technology/exaptation distinctions even within the discussion is undermined by the plausibility of gene-culture coevolution and niche construction. The standard adaptationist framework acquiesces in a one-way causal conception of evolution (i.e., environment begetting phenotype) that underemphasises the dynamic consequences of coevolution and niche construction, promoting an unjustified separation of the cultural from the biological. Musicality is a dynamic mosaic; it requires explanation in terms of coevolving social and individual elements, rendering the adaptation/byproduct framework unhelpful. The same is true, e.g., of the evolution of religion (Sterelny 2017).

  2. For instance, Tomlinson envisions Acheulean toolmakers “entraining” to a chaîne opératoire (operational sequence, conceived as unplanned, embodied chains of patterned gesture) vis-à-vis an “external oscillator.” For a critique, see Killin (2016b).

  3. For a richer methodological defense of evolutionary scenario building see Currie and Sterelny (2017), Sterelny (2012), especially §8.

  4. I cannot argue the point here, but in my view, full-fledged music and musical instruments, as well as advances that enable cognizing of musical systems’ theoretic properties (hierarchical ordering/tonal encoding, combinatoriality, and so on—see, e.g., Tomlinson 2015), emerged in coevolutionary tandem with the incremental development of behavioral modernity in general, changes in social organization (from largely mutualistic to reciprocal arrangements—see, e.g., Sterelny 2016), and concretization of very reliable social learning through highly invested teaching, among other factors, throughout the onset and development of behavioral modernity (c. 250–50 kya; McBrearty and Brooks 2000; Sterelny 2011).

  5. This scenario plausibly selects for vocally aggressive intergroup challenges for territory defense: a kind of proto-haka (named after the vocally aggressive greeting chant of New Zealand Māori). This conjecture is not wild: chimpanzees too “produce long range calls [and] also combine drumming and loud vocalizations in coordinated group displays that appear to play an important role in agonistic intergroup relations” (Hagen and Hammerstein 2009, p. 299).

  6. For the dual system theory of the mind (System 1/System 2 cognition) see Evans (2003), Kahneman (2011). The simple/systematic/speculative designation comes from Shaw-Williams (2014), following Liebenberg (1990).

  7. Some rudimentary form of intentional listening may well be very ancient, if we consider the auditory vigilance of predator/prey species as intentionally listening for danger or a target. Still, I take it that humans have developed this top-down capacity, as a matter of degree, beyond that of any other species—so I speak in terms of “increases in intentional listening” rather than the onset of intentional listening per se. A full analysis of intentional listening will need to be more nuanced, but this will do for my purposes here.

  8. Gibbons (Hylobatidae) diverged from the great ape lineage around 15–18.5 mya (Raum et al. 2005).

  9. Stronger versions of the social brain hypothesis—that social complexity as measured by group size is the primary driver of brain size evolution—have recently come under fire, with evidence suggesting that quality of diet plays a significant role (DeCasien et al. 2017), as does technological innovation and other aspects I discuss.

  10. See Morley (2013) for a detailed discussion of the paleoanthropological record with respect to evolution of musicality.

  11. At that time many animals in the Olduvai Basin were forced in the dry season to travel to the only freshwater spring for miles around, because the lake nearby was too saline for many of them to drink. This butchering site was just over 200 m away from the spring, in a lightly wooded plain of shrubbery, trees, and swampland (Ashley et al. 2010; Bunn and Gurtov 2013).

  12. There is also evidence of hominins procuring aquatic prey including crocodiles at 1.95 mya (Braun et al. 2010), presumably quite a feat.

  13. For example, ancient, intentional striking on stalactites and stalagmites has been evidenced in the booming, echoing caves of the Réseau Clastres in the French Pyrenees (Lewis-Williams 2002; Montelle 2004). And there are many simple ways to be musically creative with natural resources that would not surface in the material record too.

  14. For instance, maternal singing has been demonstrated to effectively modulate arousal levels in human infants (as evidenced through, for example, salivary cortisol levels testing; see Shenfield et al. 2003).

References

  • Aich H, Moos-Heilen R, Zimmermann E (1990) Vocalizations of adult gelada baboons (Theropithecus gelada): acoustic structure and behavioural context. Folia Primatol 55:109–132

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Antón SC, Snodgrass JJ (2012) Origins and evolution of genus Homo: new perspectives. Curr Anthropol 53(suppl. 6):s479–496

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Antón SC, Potts R, Aiello LC (2014) Evolution of early Homo: an integrated biological perspective. Science 345(6192):1236828

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashley GM, Barboni D, Domínguez-Rodrigo M et al (2010) A spring and wooded habitat at FLK Zinj and their relevance to origins of human behavior. Quatern Res 74(3):304–314

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Attwell L, Kovarovic K, Kendal JR (2015) Fire in the Plio-Pleistocene: the functions of hominin fire use, and the mechanistic, developmental, and evolutionary consequences. J Anthropol Sci 93:1–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Balter M (2015) World’s oldest stone tools discovered in Kenya. Science. doi:10.1126/science.aab2487

    Google Scholar 

  • Barham L (2013) From hand to handle: the first industrial revolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Berthelet A, Chavaillon J (2001) The early Palaeolithic butchery site of Barogali (Republic of Djibouti). In: Cavarretta G, Gioia P, Mussi M, Palombo MR (eds) The world of elephants: International Congress Proceedings, Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, Rome, pp 176–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Braun DR, Harris JWK, Levin NE et al (2010) Early hominin diet included diverse terrestrial and aquatic animals 1.95 Ma in East Turkana, Kenya. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107(22):10002–10007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown S (2000) Evolutionary models of music: from sexual selection to group selection. In: Tonneau F, Thompson N (eds) Perspectives in ecology 13: behavior, evolution and culture. Plenum, New York, pp 231–281

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunn HT, Gurtov AN (2013) Prey mortality profiles indicate that Early Pleistocene Homo at Olduvai was an ambush predator. Quat Int 322/323:44–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bunn HT, Pickering TR (2010a) Methodological recommendations for ungulate mortality analyses in paleoanthropology. Quatern Res 74(3):388–394

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bunn HT, Pickering TR (2010b) Bovid mortality profiles in paleoecological context falsify hypotheses of endurance running-hunting and passive scavenging by early Pleistocene hominins. Quatern Res 74(3):395–404

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burkart J, Hrdy S, van Schaik C (2009) Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolut Anthropol 18(5):175–186

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calcott B (2009) Lineage explanations: explaining how biological mechanisms change. Br J Philos Sci 60(1):51–78

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Call J, Tomasello M (2008) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends Cogn Sci 12(5):187–192

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crompton RH, Pataky T, Savage R et al (2012) Human-like external function of the foot, and fully upright gait, confirmed in the 3.66 million year old Laetoli hominin footprints by topographic statistics, experimental footprint-formation and computer simulation. J R Soc Interface 9(69):707–719

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Currie A, Killin A (2016) Musical pluralism and the science of music. Eur J Philos Sci 6(1):9–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Currie A, Sterelny K (2017) In defence of story-telling. Stud Hist Philos Sci. doi:10.1016/j.shpsa.2017.03.003

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin C (1871) The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. Appleton, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Davies S (2012) The artful species: aesthetics, art, and evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Davies S (in press) Evolution of music. In: Nielsen N, Levinson J, McAuley T (eds) Oxford handbook of Western music and philosophy. Oxford University Press, Oxford

  • DeCasien AR, Williams SA, Higman JP (2017) Primate brain size is predicted by diet but not sociality. Nature Ecol Evol 1:0112, doi:10.1038/s41559-017-0112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett DC (1995) Darwin’s dangerous idea. Simon & Schuster, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Dissanayake E (2000) Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother-infant interaction. In: Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S (eds) The origins of music, MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 389–410

    Google Scholar 

  • Domínguez-Rodrigo M, Pickering TR, Bunn HT (2012) Experimental study of cut marks made with rocks unmodified by human flaking and its bearing on claims of ~3.4-million-year-old butchery evidence from Dikka, Ethiopia. J Archaeolog Sci 39(2):205–214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dufour V, Poulin N, Curé C, Sterck EHM (2015) Chimpanzee drumming: a spontaneous performance with characteristics of human musical drumming. Sci Rep 5:11320

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar R (1996) Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. Faber & Faber, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar R (2004) The human story: a new history of mankind’s evolution. Faber & Faber, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar R (2012) On the evolutionary function of song and dance. In: Bannan N (ed) Music, language, and human evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 201–214

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar R (2014) Human evolution. Penguin, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar R, Kaskatis K, MacDonald I, Barra V (2012) Performance of music elevates pain threshold and positive affect: implications for the evolutionary function of music. Evolut Psychol 10(4):688–702

    Google Scholar 

  • Dutton D (2009) The art instinct. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans JSBT (2003) In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning. TRENDS Cogn Sci 7(10):454–459

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch WT (2006) The biology and evolution of music: a comparative perspective. Cognition 100:173–215

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ford J (1991) Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal waters of British Columbia. Can J Zool 69(6):1454–1483

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gamble C, Gowlett J, Dunbar R (2011) The social brain and the shape of the Palaeolithic. Cambridge Archaeol J 21(1):115–135

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gamble C, Gowlett J, Dunbar R (2014) Thinking big: how the evolution of social life shaped the human mind. Thames & Hudson, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Geissmann T (2002) Duet-splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs. Biol Rev 77:57–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt S (2004) Why love matters: how affection shapes a baby’s brain. Routledge, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons A (2015) Deep roots for the genus. Science 347:1056–1057

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goren-Inbar N, Alperson N, Kislev ME et al (2004) Evidence of hominin control of fire at Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, Israel. Science 304:725–727

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gowlett J, Gamble C, Dunbar R (2012) Human evolution and the archaeology of the social brain. Curr Anthropol 53(6):693–722

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grieser DL, Kuhl PK (1988) Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: support for universal prosodic features in motherese. Dev Psychol 24(1):14–20

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gustison ML, le Roux A, Bergman T (2012) Derived vocalizations of geladas (Theropithecus gelada) and the evolution of vocal complexity in primates. Philos Trans R Soc B 367:1847–1859

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagen EH, Bryant GA (2003) Music and dance as a coalitional signalling system. Hum Nat 14:21–51

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagen EH, Hammerstein P (2009) Did Neanderthals and other early humans sing? Seeking the biological roots of music in the territorial advertisements of primates, lions, hyenas, and wolves. Music Sci 13(2 suppl):291–320

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harmand S, Lewis JE, Feibel CS et al (2015) 3.3-million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature 521:310–315

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hattori Y, Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T (2013) Spontaneous synchronized tapping to an auditory rhythm in a chimpanzee. Sci Rep 3:1566

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes K (2003) Grandmothers and the evolution of human longevity. Am J Hum Biol 15:380–400

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hiscock P (2014) Learning in lithic landscapes: a reconstruction of the hominid ‘toolmaking’ niche. Biol Theory 9:27–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobaiter C, Byrne RW (2014) The meanings of chimpanzee gestures. Curr Biol 24:1596–1600

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard JH (1984) The Canadian Sioux. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln

    Google Scholar 

  • Hrdy S (2007) Evolutionary context of human development: the cooperative breeding model. In: Salmon CA, Shackelford TK (eds) Family relationships: an evolutionary perspective, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 39–68

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hrdy S (2009) Mothers and others: the evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeffares B (2010) The co-evolution of tools and minds: cognition and material culture in the hominin lineage. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 9(4):503–520

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson S (2015) Language abilities in Neanderthals. Annu Rev of Linguist 1:311–332

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D (2011) Thinking, fast and slow. Allen Lane, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe AB (1999) Blackfoot and other hunters on the North American Plains. In: Lee R, Daly R (eds) The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 36–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Keverne EB, Martensz ND, Tuite B (1989) Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology 14:155–161

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killin A (2013) The arts and human nature: evolutionary aesthetics and the evolutionary status of art behaviours. Biol Philos 28(4):703–718

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killin A (2016a) Rethinking music’s status as an adaptation versus technology: a niche construction perspective. Ethnomusicol Forum 25(2):210–233

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killin A (2016b) Musicality and the evolution of mind, mimesis, and entrainment. Biol Philos 31(3):421–434

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killin A (in press) Music and human evolution: philosophical aspects. In: Joyce R (ed) Routledge companion to evolution and philosophy, Routledge, New York

  • Kohn M, Mithen S (1999) Handaxes: products of sexual selection? Antiquity 73:518–526

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer KL (2011) The evolution of human parental care and recruitment of juvenile help. Trends Ecol Evol 26(10):533–540

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsson M (2014) Self-generated sounds of locomotion and ventilation and the evolution of human rhythmic abilities. Anim Cogn 17:1–14

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsson M (2015) Tool-use-associated sound in the evolution of language. Anim Cogn 18:993–1005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leaky MD, Harris JM (1987) Laetoli: a Pliocene site in northern Tanzania. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis J (2009) As well as words: Congo Pygmy hunting, mimicry, and play. In: Botha R, Knight C (eds) Cradle of Language, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 236–256

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis-Williams D (2002) The mind in the cave: consciousness and the origins of art. Thames & Hudson, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Liebenberg L (1990) The art of tracking: the origin of science. D. Philip, Claremont

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLarnon AM, Hewitt GP (1999) The evolution of human speech: the role of enhanced breathing control. Am J Phys Anthropol 109:341–363

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marwick B (2003) Pleistocene exchange networks as evidence for the evolution of language. Cambridge Archaeol J 13(1):67–81

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marzke MW (2013) Tool making, hand morphology, and fossil humans. Philos Trans R Soc B. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0414

    Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty S, Brooks AS (2000) The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. J Human Evol 39:453–563

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNabb J, Binyon F, Hazelwood L (2004) The large cutting tools from the South African Acheulean and the question of social traditions. Curr Anthropol 45(5):653–677

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McPherron SP, Alemseged Z, Marean C et al (2010) Evidence for stone-tool-assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at Dikkia, Ethiopia. Nature 466:857–860

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merchant H, Honing H (2014) Are non-human primates capable of rhythmic entrainment? Evidence for the gradual audiomotor evolution hypothesis. Front Neurosci 7:274

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merker B (2012) The vocal learning constellation: imitation, ritual culture, encephalization. In: Bannan N (ed) Music, language, and human evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 215–260

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Merker B, Morley I, Zuidema W (2015) Five fundamental constraints of theories of the origins of music. Philos Trans R Soc B. doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0095

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller G (2000) Evolution of human music through sexual selection. In: Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S (eds) The origins of music. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 329–360

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen S (2005) The singing Neanderthals: the origins of music, language, mind and body. Weidenfeld & Nicolson, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Montelle Y-P (2004) Paleoperformance: investigating the human use of caves in the Upper Paleolithic. In: Bergaus G (ed) New perspectives on prehistoric art. Praeger, Westport, pp 131–152

    Google Scholar 

  • Morley I (2013) The prehistory of music: human evolution, archaeology, and the origins of musicality. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nettl B (2000) An ethnomusicologist contemplates universals in musical sound and culture. In: Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S (eds) The origins of music. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 463–472

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowell A, Chang ML (2009) The case against sexual selection as an explanation of handaxe morphology. PaleoAnthropology 2009:77–88

    Google Scholar 

  • Orians GH (2014) Snakes, sunrises, and Shakespeare: how evolution shapes our loves and fears. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Patel AD (2008) Music, language, and the brain. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Patel AD (2010) Music, biological evolution, and the brain. In: Bailar M, Field C, Henry CJ (eds) Emerging disciplines. Rice University Press, Houston, pp 91–144

    Google Scholar 

  • Patel AD, Iversen JR, Bregman MR, Schulz I (2009) Experimental evidence for synchronization to a musical beat in a nonhuman animal. Curr Biol 19(10):827–830

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petraglia MD, Shipton C (2008) Large cutting tool variation west and east of the Movius Line. J Human Evol 55(6):962–966

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petraglia MD, Shipton C, Paddayya K (2005) Life and mind in the Acheulean. In: Gamble C, Porr M (eds) The individual hominid in context: archaeological investigations of Lower and Middle Palaeolithic landscapes, locales and artefacts. Routledge, Oxon, pp. 197–219

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S (1997) How the mind works. Allen Lane, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Raum RL, Sterner KN, Noviello CM et al (2005) Catarrhine primate divergence dates estimated from complete mitochondrial genomes: concordance with fossil and nuclear DNA evidence. J Human Evol 48(3):237–257

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richman B (1987) Rhythm and melody in gelada vocal exchanges. Primates 28(2):199–223

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richman B (2000) How music fixed ‘nonsense’ into significant formulas: on rhythm, repetition, and meaning. In: Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S (eds) The origins of music. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 301–314

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachner A (2013) The origins of human and avian auditory-motor entrainment. Nova Acta Leopold 111:243–253

    Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth R, Cheney D (1990) The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and another species’ alarm calls. Anim Behav 40(4):754–764

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw-Williams K (2014) The social trackways theory of the evolution of human cognition. Biol Theory 9(1):16–26

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shenfield T, Trehub SE, Nakata T (2003) Maternal singing modulates infant arousal. Psychol Music 31(4):365–375

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shipton C (2013) A million years of hominin sociality and cognition: Acheulean bifaces in the Hunsgi-Baichbal Valley, India. British Archaeological Reports International Series 2468. Archaeopress, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Shipton C, Nielsen M (2015) Before cumulative culture: the evolutionary origins of overimitation and shared intentionality. Hum Nat 26:331–345

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D (1996) Explaining culture: a naturalistic approach. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2011) From hominins to humans: how sapiens became behaviourally modern. Philos Trans R Soc B 366:809–822

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2012) Language, gesture, skill: the co-evolutionary foundations of language. Philos Trans R Soc B 367:2141–2151

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2016) Cooperation, culture, and conflict. Br J Philos Sci 67(1):31–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2017) Religion re-explained. Relig Brain Behav. doi:10.1080/2153599X.2017.1323779

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J et al (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav Brain Sci 28(5):675–691

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomlinson G (2015) A million years of music: the emergence of human modernity. Zone, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Trehub SE (2001) Musical predispositions in infancy. Ann N Y Acad Sci 930:1–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trehub SE (2003) The developmental origins of musicality. Nat Neurosci 6(7):669–673

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Twomey T (2013) The cognitive implications of controlled fire use by early humans. Cambridge Archaeol J 23(1):113–128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan CD (2001) A million years of style and function: regional and temporal variation in Acheulean handaxes. In: Hurt TD, Rakita GFM (eds) Style and function: conceptual issues in evolutionary archaeology. Bergin & Garvey, Westport, pp 141–163

    Google Scholar 

  • Villmoare B, Kimbel WH, Seyoum C et al (2015) Early Homo at 2.8 ma from Ledi-Geraru, Afar, Ethiopia. Science 347:1352–1355

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WoldeGabriel G, Heiken G, White TD et al (2000) Volcanism, tectonism, sedimentation, and the paleoanthropological record in the Ethiopian Rift System. In: McCoy FW, Heiken G (eds) Volcanic hazards and disasters in human antiquity, Geological Society of America, Colorado, pp 83–99

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • WoldeGabriel G, Haile-Selassie Y, Renne P et al (2001) Geology and paleontology of the Late Miocene Middle Awash valley, Afar rift, Ethiopia. Nature 412:175–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham R (2009) Catching fire: how cooking made us human. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Zarco W, Merchant H, Prado L, Mendez JC (2009) Subsecond timing in primates: comparison of interval production between human subjects and rhesus monkeys. J Neurophysiol 102:3191–3202

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thanks to all the participants of “Symbols II” (the “Symbols and Communicative Behaviour in Pleistocene Hominins” workshop, University of Sydney) and the 2016 Empirical Philosophy workshop at Victoria University of Wellington, as well as audiences at the 2015 and 2016 New Zealand Association of Philosophers conferences. Kim Sterelny and Peter Hiscock provided valuable comments on previous versions of the manuscript, and I have benefitted greatly from discussions with Kim Shaw-Williams and Johanna Guest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anton Killin.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Killin, A. Plio-Pleistocene Foundations of Hominin Musicality: Coevolution of Cognition, Sociality, and Music. Biol Theory 12, 222–235 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-017-0274-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-017-0274-6

Keywords

Navigation