Skip to content
Publicly Available Published by De Gruyter August 7, 2020

Consensual Qualitative Research on Free Associations for Criticism and Self-Criticism

  • Júlia Halamová , Petra Langová , Martina Baránková , Bronislava Strnádelová and Jana Koróniová
From the journal Human Affairs

Abstract

Criticism and self-criticism have far reaching impacts on wellbeing and emotional balance. In order to create better interventions for criticism and self-criticism, more in-depth knowledge about these two constructs is required. The goal of our study was to examine three associations for criticism and self-criticism. The data were collected from a sample of 151 psychology students: 114 women and 37 men (Mean age 22.2; SD 4.4). We were interested in the associations participants would produce in relation to criticism and self-criticism, whether participants conceptualized these two concepts in the same categories and whether the categories were equally important for each concept. The data were analyzed using Consensual Qualitative research (CQR). The team consisted of four core members and one auditor. Separately all four members analyzed the data multiple times and then discussed it until all the researchers including the auditor reached a consensus. We identified four domains common to both criticism and self-criticism. These were – Emotional Aspects, Cognitive Aspects, Behavioral Aspects, and Preconditions. For both stimulus words, the most saturated domain was Behavioral Aspects. These findings suggest that both concepts – criticism and self-criticism – are multidimensional constructs consisting of Behavioral Aspects, Cognitive aspects, Emotional Aspects, and Preconditions. Further research on this topic would be beneficial.

Introduction

Criticism is defined as “an opinion given about something or someone, esp. a negative opinion, or the activity of making such judgments” (Cambridge Advanced Learner’s Dictionary & Thesaurus, 2019, pg. 327) and has generally negative connotations. Criticism was widely studied by Gottman (1994) in relation to couples and couple therapy. He labeled criticism one of the four “horsemen of the apocalypse” (Gottman, 1994, p. 72). It is important to distinguish between criticism and constructive complaints – criticism is an attack on a person’s character or personality and often involves blame as well, while constructive complaints focus on a specific behavior (Gottman, 1994). Similarly, Baron (1988) differentiates between two types of criticism—constructive and destructive.

Constructive criticism is feedback that is specific, not harsh and is not about a character trait or personality. Destructive criticism does not follow any of these principles (Baron, 1988). In the work environment, destructive criticism tends to occur because the person delivering the criticism is angry and upset and cannot hold back their emotions any longer. At this point, the manner in which the criticism is delivered is influenced by emotions that have built up over a longer period of time (Baron, 1988). In such cases it is not unusual for destructive criticism to be sarcastic in tone. This kind of criticism triggers more negative interactions (Liden & Mitchell, 1985). Destructive criticism can also be used instead of personal conflict and hidden motives, such as different values or personal dislike (Neuman & Baron, 1998). When harsh criticism is given, the recipient considers his or her own actions, along with the motives behind the criticism and the future implications for the relationship with the person delivering it (Ilgen & Davis, 2000). We also know that criticism from others is a very powerful trigger of a cortisol stress response (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004).

Some people are not only criticized by others, but often engage in self-criticism or almost constant self-criticism, which becomes part of their personality. These people are more prone to depression (Blatt & Zuroff, 1992; Greenberg & Watson, 2005), social anxiety (Cox, Fleet, & Stein, 2004), drug abuse (Blatt, 1991), and many other forms of psychopathology. In addition, self-criticism affects the incidence and persistence of psychopathology (Falconer, King, & Brewin, 2016). Self-criticism can be described as a general sensitivity to negative perceptions about ourselves (Kitayama, Markus, Matsumoto, & Norasakkunkit, 1997) or as harsh and constant examination and judgement of ourselves, accompanied with feelings such as worthlessness, guilt, inadequacy, and failure (Blatt & Zuroff, 1992). Shahar (2015) developed an even more detailed description of this phenomenon: self-criticism is a constant intensive relationship with the self. This relationship involves high expectations of a high performance, which is usually unachievable. If these conditions are not met, individuals become hostile and contemptuous of themselves (Shahar, 2015). According to Falconer, King, and Brewin (2016), self-criticism develops in early childhood and is caused by a lack of supportive relationships. Self-criticism is considered to be a relatively stable personality characteristic (Hermanto et al., 2016) and the most severe forms are very hard to change (Werner, Tibubos, Rohrmann, & Reiss, 2019). For some people self-criticism emerges only in some situations or at certain stages of their lives (Whelton, Paulson, & Marusiak, 2007). But research has indicated that various compassionate interventions or psychotherapy can improve clients’ conditions and reduce self-criticism (Kirby, Tellegen, & Steindl, 2017). Self-criticism has a significant impact on clinical practice and we therefore require better knowledge and understanding of this area of study (Halamová, 2018).

Previous research findings related to criticism and self-criticism

Whelton and Henkelman (2002) conducted a verbal analysis of forms of self-criticism. The aim of the study was to document different types of verbal behaviors participants used when criticizing and to compare healthy participants with participants vulnerable to depression. During the first part, the data was subjected to a phenomenological analysis and eight categorical clusters were created from the self-criticism transcripts. The categories were: “demands and orders; exhorting and preaching; explanations and excuses; inducing fear and anxiety; concern, protection, and support; description; explore/puzzle/existential; and self-attack and condemnation” (Whelton & Henkelman, 2002, p. 89).

Destructive criticism, task outcomes and appraisals, and relation to competitiveness was studied by Raver, Jensen, Lee, & O’Reilly (2012). They suggested that people who received destructive criticism were more prone to think the feedback giver had harmful intentions, distrusted and blamed the person giving the feedback, and were more likely to feel angry. Their findings also indicate that competitiveness plays a big part in task outcome. They found that if highly competitive people receive destructive feedback, their motivation to work harder increases. But they also observed a difference among people with low competitiveness. However, task outcome was not reflected in increased intent to work harder. After receiving destructive criticism, highly competitive people performed worse, but following constructive criticism they outperformed low competitive individuals. In contrast people with low competitiveness performed better at tasks after receiving destructive criticism (Raver, Jensen, Lee, & O’Reilly, 2012).

A study by Fong et al. (2018) focused on constructive criticism or constructive feedback. They pointed out that many studies concentrate on the destructive part of criticism, and that only a few studies focus on the other part. They also stressed that constructive feedback can help individuals recover from failure and improve learning. In order to better understand perceptions of constructive feedback they conducted several focus groups. The researchers developed a process model with three main aspects so as to gain a better understanding of the underlying mechanisms of constructive criticism. These three main aspects of constructive criticism are: 1. the person who gives feedback must be deserving of respect and the reason for giving the criticism needs to be a caring one; 2. it is important that the message reflects good intentions, focuses on the problem and provides guidance for improvement; 3. understanding of the feedback needs to be reflected in change of emotions and motivation and therefore change in work can occur (Fong et al., 2018).

As we mentioned above, self-criticism plays an important role in various mental disorders. Wernere et al. (2019) analyzed the literature from 2012 and 2018 to gain an understanding of self-criticism and psychopathology. They found a positive connection between self-criticism and “symptoms of eating disorders, social anxiety disorder, and personality disorders as well as to psychotic symptoms or interpersonal problems” (Werner et al., 2019, p. 530). Two main treatments were identified—emotion focused therapy and compassion focused therapy. Both treatments reduced self-criticism in the non-clinical and clinical samples. However, some forms of self-criticism were hard to change (Werner et al., 2019).

Similarly to self-criticism, the research on perceiving compassion is growing. Some researchers see self-compassion as the antidote to self-criticism (e.g. Gilbert & Procter, 2006; Greenberg, 2011) and therefore a better means of treating self-criticism, but it is also important to understand self-compassion. Baránková, Halamová and Koróniová (2019) explored compassion from a non-expert perspective. The results support a multidimensional definition of compassion with compassion being described as a mixture of non-specified positive emotions and specified negative emotions (mainly fear, remorse, and sadness). In addition, compassion was related to empathy and, in terms of behaviors, compassion was found to be beneficial and support mental closeness and interest.

A study of associations for compassion and self-compassion among psychology students indicated the terms contained further dimensions: Emotional, Cognitive, Behavioral and Evaluative Aspects of compassion/self-compassion (Halamová, Baránková, Strnádelová, & Koróniová, 2018). The most frequently represented domain for both compassion and self-compassion was the Emotional Aspect. Participants perceived compassion as mainly consisting of empathy; the emotions of love, sadness, and remorse; cognitive understanding; and behavioral displays of help and physical or mental closeness. Self-compassion was viewed primarily in terms of the positive emotions of love and calmness; the negative emotions of unhappiness, sadness, and remorse; cognitive understanding; and behavioral displays of self-help through the provision of self-support and self-assurance.

To the best of our knowledge, there has been no research analyzing first associations for criticism and self-criticism. Also, there is very little qualitative research on criticism and self-criticism which would help us gain a deeper understanding of what people associate these concepts with and how people understand these concepts. On top of that, self-criticism plays a key role in psychopathology and therefore it is important to learn how people perceive criticism and self-criticism.

Aim of the study

Our aim was to categorize the first three association words students of psychology produced in relation to the stimulus words criticism and self-criticism. We were interested in which associations participants related to criticism and self-criticism, whether participants conceptualized these two concepts in the same categories and whether the categories would be equally important for each concept.

Methods

Sample and materials

Our sample consisted of 151 psychology students at Comenius University in Bratislava. Mean age was 22.2 years (SD=4.4), 37 participants were men and 114 were women. All participants were given the same instructions: to complete an open-ended sentence “… without censoring, please write down the first three associations for the word criticism” and “…without censoring, please write down the first three associations for the word self-criticism”.

Procedure and data analysis

The free association task was administered as part of a battery of questionnaires. The association tasks came immediately after the informed consent and participant’s sociodemographic information form. For the data analysis, we used a qualitative method—Consensual Qualitative Research (CQR) (Hill, 2012).

Consensual qualitative research

Our core CQR team consisted of four researchers and one auditor. In order to eliminate possible bias, before seeing the data, the participating researchers noted their expectations of how the participants would complete the three association tasks. In the second step, each member of the core team obtained the data and working individually created their own categories. The members of the core team created their own domains and allocated the free associations to them depending on how they understood them. A consensus was sought through discussions as a team. At this point, the team members individually made changes to their original categorizations. This process was repeated several times until a consensus was reached, which meant agreeing on the domains, subdomains, categories, and characteristics. They then moved on to the next step—the auditor check.

The auditor had two roles: to check if there was consensus among the core team and to give feedback on the data analysis. All the auditor’s comments were taken into consideration and the changes were incorporated into the final categorization. At the end, a consensus was reached by the core team members and approved by the auditor.

Results

In total 688 free associations, from 151 participants, were coded and assigned to one of four domains. Criticism consisted of 348 coded phrases and self-criticism of 342phrases. During the analysis all the researchers agreed to exclude associations not related to the subject matter. Ten associations were excluded from the self-criticism construct (e.g. general knowledge) and seven were excluded from criticism (e.g. grey). For both constructs four domains were created. Criticism was assigned 8 subdomains, 18 categories, and 6 characteristics. Self-criticism was assigned 7 subdomains, 14 categories, and 6 characteristics. Details of the data categorization with specific examples for criticism can be found in Appendix 1, and for self-criticism in Appendix 2.

The behavioral aspects domain consisted of all content relating to behavior, such as behavior manifestation, motivation to improve, and how to deal with criticism and self-criticism. This domain included progress, change, constructive and negative expressions, specific acts, and changing mindset. Associations found in both concepts for this domain were progress, change, motivation, help, and advice. The cognitive aspects domain consisted of all thought-related associations. All of these were linked to evaluation and so only one subdomain emerged. Afterwards two main categories were created: content and form. Content consisted of all associations describing the product of the evaluation process. Forms included all the associations describing all types of evaluation. A third category emerged in relation to criticism: Evaluation of Importance. This category consisted of all the associations that evaluated criticism as important. Examples of Content associations allocated to this category were negative feedback, evaluation, and excessive criticism. While for Form they included perception, biased, excessive and justified. For Evaluation of Importance, they were essential and important associations. The Precondition domain contained anything leading to and causing criticism and self-criticism. Both internal and external sources were identified as categories. Internal sources included all associations connected to the person and his/her characteristics, while external sources consisted of all the sources of criticism that are dependent on the environment, other people, and things. Examples of internal sources were weakness, me, laziness, and low self-esteem. Examples of external sources were family, work, relationships, and enemy. The Emotional Aspects domain included all feeling and emotion associations or images. Criticism consisted only of negative associations, while self-criticism had a small number of positive emotions such as love, self-love, and happiness. Examples of negative associations were hate, anger, fear, and sorrow.

Behavioral Aspects was the most represented domain in relation to criticism (f =125; 36%). It consisted of four subdomains, Motivational Function (e.g. being better), Aggression Expressions (e.g. attack), Behavioral Expressions (e.g. lecturing), and How to Handle Criticism (e.g. advice, ability to accept). Cognitive Aspects was the second most represented domain (f =104; 30%). Only one subdomain was created—Evaluation, which was divided into three categories—Content, Form, and Evaluation of Importance. Both content and form had three characteristics—negative, neutral, and constructive. Examples of content were: negative (e.g. underlining mistakes, negative feedback), neutral (e.g. evaluation, analysis) and constructive (e.g. another point of view), while examples of form were: negative (e.g. harsh, excessive), neutral (e.g. truth, adequate) and constructive (e.g. justified, constructive). A third category was created for criticism—Evaluation of Importance. This category consisted of a small number of associations (e.g. important, essential). The third most represented domain was Preconditions (f =69; 20%). This domain had two subdomains— Internal (e.g. self-worth) and External (e.g. rival, enemy) sources. The least frequent domain relating to criticism was emotional aspects (f =48; 14%), which had only one subdomain— Emotions. This subdomain contained two categories: general negative emotions (e.g. bad feelings) and specific negative emotions (e.g. fear). For a comparison of criticism in percentages see Chart 1.

Behavioral Aspects was the most frequently represented domain relating to self-criticism (f =133; 39%) and contained three subdomains. These subdomains were Motivational Function (e.g. improvement, new beginnings), Behavioral Expressions (e.g. lecturing), and How to Handle Criticism (e.g. discipline, understanding). The second most frequent domain was Preconditions (f =87; 25%). The subdomains contained within Preconditions were Internal (e.g. laziness, low self-esteem) and External (e.g. family, relationships) sources. The third most frequent domain relating to self-criticism was Cognitive Aspects (f =74; 22%). Cognitive Aspects include only one subdomain – Evaluation, which was divided into two categories: Content and Form. Both included three characteristics: neutral (e.g. content – self-analysis, form – being objective), negative (e.g. content – too much criticism, form – biased), and constructive (e.g. content – reality, form – constructive). The least represented domain relating to self-criticism was Emotional Aspects (f =48; 14%), containing only one subdomain: Emotions. In contrast to criticism, some positive emotions emerged in this concept, producing two categories: Positive Emotions (e.g. self-love) and Negative Emotions (e.g. hate, pity).

A comparison of the percentages for self-criticism can be seen in Figure 2. A more detailed comparison of each domain is given in Table 1. To improve clarity, only the domain, subdomains, and categories are included in table 1.

Figure 1 
          Share of each criticism domain.
Figure 1

Share of each criticism domain.

Figure 2 
          Share of each self-criticism domain.
Figure 2

Share of each self-criticism domain.

Table 1

Comparison of Criticism and Self-criticism

Criticism 346 Self-Criticism 342
Behavioral aspects 125 Behavioral aspects 133
Behavioral expressions (57) Behavioral expressions (50)
Negative expression (32) Negative expression (42)
Constructive expressions (25) Constructive expressions (8)
Motivational function (30) Motivational function (42)
Progress (19) Progress (30)
Change (11) Change (12)
How to handle criticism (29) How to handle criticism (41)
Specific acts (22) Specific acts (16)
Changing mindset (7) Changing mindset (25)
Aggression expressions (9)

Cognitive aspects 104 Cognitive aspects 74
Evaluation (104) Evaluation (74)
Content (36) Content (42)
Form (65) Form (32)
Evaluation of importance (4)

Preconditions 69 Preconditions 87
Internal source (6) Internal source (67)
Characteristics (4) Characteristics (18)
Self-regard (2) Self-regard (49)
External sources (63) External sources (20)
People (16) People, work, things (10)
Work related (10) Situations, Relationships (10)
Things (24)
Media (11)
Characteristics (2)

Emotional aspects 48 Emotional aspects 48
Emotions (48) Emotions (48)
Specific negative emotions (36) Negative emotions (38)
General negative emotions (12)
Positive emotions (10)

Discussion

The goal of our study was to explore and categorize the first three associations for two constructs—criticism and self-criticism. Both constructs are highly significant for well-being as well as psychopathology and so obtaining more in-depth knowledge about these concepts is very desirable. We were interested in whether any patterns would emerge within these constructs, and whether they could be conceptualized in the same terms.

The same domains emerged for both concepts during coding—Behavioral Aspects, Cognitive Aspects, Preconditions, and Emotional Aspects. However, differences can be found in the saturation of each domain. Behavioral Aspects was the most frequent domain and Emotional Aspects was the least frequent domain for both concepts, and the second and third domains differed for each concept. Differences were also found at the subdomain and category level.

Behavioral aspects

The most saturated domain for both concepts was Behavioral Aspects. In both cases behavioral aspects represented over a third of the answers. This suggests that criticism and self-criticism are mainly connected with the way people behave, rather than how they feel. The Emotional Aspects domain was the least frequent for both concepts. The subdomains of Behavioral Aspects differed for criticism and self-criticism. The free associations relating to Behavioral Aspects for criticism consisted of four subdomains, but only three for self-criticism. In both cases Behavioral Expressions were the most represented subdomains However, they differed at category level. Both concepts had identical subdomains— constructive and negative expressions, but the distribution of the associations was different. This could suggest that people perceive self-criticism more negatively. This is also supported by the fact that self-criticism has a significant impact on mental health (Falconer, King, & Brewin, 2016). The second most represented subdomain for both concepts was motivational function. This may indicate that even though participants think self-criticism is negative, they also understand that self-criticism has the power to motivate. Support for this is found in Gilbert et al. (2017): self-criticism leads to feelings such as being inadequate, and that motivates the person to improve and avoid past mistakes. Criticism from others also motivates individuals to improve. This is supported by research by Kluger and DeNisi (1996), who found that recipients of negative feedback are eager to minimize the feedback-standard gap. They do that by motivating themselves to work harder. Raver, Jensen, Lee, & O’Reilly (2012) suggested that there is a difference between highly competitive and low competitive individuals. Criticism motivates highly competitive people to work harder but has no effect on people with low competitiveness. The third subdomain that was the same was how to handle criticism/self-criticism. This suggests that participants felt the need to find a way of dealing with self-criticism in particular. As mentioned before, self-criticism has a big impact on mental health and plays a big part in the occurrence of psychopathology (Falconer, King, & Brewin, 2016). Participants probably felt the need to create a strategy for avoiding the negative effects of self-criticism. It is also important to have strategies for handling criticism as well as self-criticism. Results by Raver, Jensen, Lee, & O’Reilly (2012) indicate that competitiveness plays a significant role in handling criticism. Competitive individuals perform worse after receiving destructive criticism. On the other hand, low competitive individuals perform better after receiving destructive criticism. But under normal circumstances, highly competitive individuals would perform better. One more domain was identified in relation to criticism—aggressive expressions. This was a very small domain but surprisingly it was the only subdomain found in criticism. Self-criticism is harsh, hostile and contemptuous (Shahar, 2015). All these can be perceived as aggression. But it may have been too personal for participants to mention aggression towards themselves.

Cognitive aspects

In our data the order of the cognitive aspects domain was different for criticism and self-criticism. It was the second biggest domain for criticism, but was third for self-criticism. Although the order of the domains differed, the structure of the subdomains and categories was very similar. Both consisted of only one subdomain – evaluation. Three categories emerged out of the criticism data—Content, Form, and Evaluation of Importance, while self-criticism had two—Content and Form. The emergence of this domain supports the idea that negative cognition is considered a core element of self-criticism (Greenberg et al., 1998). Further support is found in feedback intervention theory (FIT) proposed by Kluger and DeNisi (1996). After receiving criticism, the first thing people do is to evaluate it in relation to their goals. Kluger and DeNisi explained this by pointing out that the attention span is limited. In order to change our behavior we require cognitive resources. These resources are only used when people identify a gap between their desired goal and their performance. In other words, cognitive processes are essential when deciding whether or not to act upon criticism. Raver et al. (2012) state that their results indicate that destructive criticism consistently generates negative evaluations.

Preconditions

Much of the data we collected seemed to represent the causes of criticism and self-criticism. We decided to name this domain Preconditions because we saw the associations that make up this domain as conditions that result either in self-criticism or criticism of others. Interestingly, when the participants were asked what associations came to mind, they were triggers for these conditions. In order to cope with criticism and self-criticism, it is very important to identify what causes such behavior, so it can then be changed and people do not end up overwhelmed. As Greenberg, Watson, & Goldman (1998) have suggested depressive helplessness is caused by the negative effects of self-criticism. The reasons we criticize ourselves are more important than the reasons others criticize us. This can be seen in our data, where the Preconditions domain is the second most frequent domain in Self-criticism and third for Criticism.

Emotional aspects

The least represented domain for both concepts was Emotional Aspects (criticism (48), self-criticism (48)). Interestingly, the emotions related to self-criticism were both positive and negative, while the emotions associated with criticism were only negative. See Appendix 1 and 2. This might suggest that people view self-criticism more positively than criticism. Surprisingly there is little research on the repercussions of emotions in self-criticism (Whelton & Greenberg, 2005). Nonetheless, we do know that emotions play an important role in both criticism and self-criticism. Anger is often the result of criticism, especially destructive criticism, because anger is a natural and dominant reaction to the harmful events individuals experience (Quigley & Tedeshi, 1996). Halamová et al. (2018) confirmed this is the case with similar words—compassion and self-compassion. In their research, the most frequent domain was Emotional Aspects. Self-compassion has very good potential as a means of improving one’s relationship with oneself; it is a promising therapy for tackling self-criticism and the psychopathologies that feed off it (Halamová, 2018). It is possible that emotions, especially the negative emotions in criticism and self-criticism, are downplayed and suppressed. We recommend further research on this.

Limitations and future work

As the participants provided their associations without giving more detailed context, it was not possible to dig deeper into the meanings. We tried to eliminate this disadvantage by using Consensual Qualitative Research for the data analysis. In future work, we would like to analyze short texts, where the participants write a short essay about what criticism and self-criticism means to them or to hold focus groups on the same topic. Using a short text or holding longer group discussions could provide context and further insights into what goes on in the participant’s mind.

The second limitation of this study concerns the sample composition. The sample was comprised of psychology students and so may not reflect the general population in terms of education. However, as this was the first study on this subject we chose to study participants with potentially greater insights because of their education. We recommend doing the same research with a general population sample in the future.

Moreover, the analyses could have been more detailed and beneficial if we had divided the sample into low and highly self-critical respondents. Previous research (e.g. Whelton & Greenberg, 2005) has led to the assumption that associations and the overall perception of criticism and self-criticism are related to the individual’s level of self-criticism.

This research is just the first step of longer research into these complex multidimensional concepts. We suggest that further research relating to compassion and self-compassion should be conducted in order to improve interventions for self-criticism and self-compassion as these improve quality of life in general.

Conclusion

Our research supports the finding that criticism and self-criticism are multidimensional concepts. The data were categorized into four domains, which were the same, for both concepts. The most frequent domain—Behavioral Aspects—and the least frequent domain – Emotional Aspects—was the first domain for both criticism and self-criticism. However, the second and third most frequent domains differed according to concept. Cognitive Aspects came second in relation to criticism and Preconditions came third. But for self-criticism the second and third domains were in the reverse order. Criticism and self-criticism were mainly viewed in terms of behavior. These behaviors were both negative and positive. Behavioral Aspects also had the most subdomains. Cognitive Aspects and Preconditions were a significant part of criticism and self-criticism. That means that if we are to better understand these concepts, we require more research into form, content, and the triggers of criticism and self-criticism.


1This work has been written with the support of research grant VEGA 1/0075/19.


References

Baránková, M., Halamová, J., & Koróniová, J. (2019). Non-expert views of compassion: Consensual qualitative research using focus groups. Human Affairs 29(1), 6–19. doi: 10.1515/humaff-2019-000210.1515/ humaff-2019-0002Search in Google Scholar

Baron, R. A. (1988). Negative effects of destructive criticism: Impact on conflict, self-efficacy, and task performance. Journal of Applied Psychology 73(2), 199–207. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.73.2.19910.1037/0021-9010.73.2.199Search in Google Scholar

Blatt, S. J. (1991). Depression and destructive risk-taking behavior in adolescence. In L. P. Lipsitt & L. L. Mitnick (Eds.), Self-regulatory behavior and risk-taking: Causes and consequences (pp. 285–305). Norwood: Ablex.Search in Google Scholar

Blatt, S. J., & Zuroff. D. C. (1992). Interpersonal relatedness and self- definition: Two prototypes for depression. Clinical Psychology Review 12, 527–562. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(92)90070-o10.1016/0272-7358(92)90070-oSearch in Google Scholar

Cambridge Advanced Learner’s Dictionary & Thesaurus (2019) Retrieved from https://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/english/criticismSearch in Google Scholar

Cox, B., Fleet, C., & Stein, M. B. (2004). Self-criticism and social phobia in the US national comorbidity survey. Journal of Affective Disorders 82(2), 227–234. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2003.12.01210.1016/j.jad.2003.12.012Search in Google Scholar

Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M.E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol response: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin 130, 335–391. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.35510.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355Search in Google Scholar

Falconer, C. J., King, J. A., & Brewin, C. R. (2015). Demonstrating mood repair with a situation-based measure of self-compassion and self-criticism. Psychology and Psychotherapy 88(4), 351–365. doi:10.1111/papt.1205610.1111/papt.12056Search in Google Scholar

Fong, C. J., Schallert, D. L., Williams, K. M., Williamson, Z. H., Warner, J. R., Lin, S., & Kim, Y. W. (2018). When feedback signals failure but offers hope for improvement: A process model of constructive criticism. Thinking Skills and Creativity 30, 42–53. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2018.02.01410.1016/j.tsc.2018.02.014Search in Google Scholar

Gilbert, P., & Procter, S. (2006). Compassionate mind training for people with high shame and self-criticism: Overview and pilot study of a group therapy approach Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy 13(6), 353–379. doi:10.1002/cpp.50710.1002/cpp.507Search in Google Scholar

Gilbert, P., Catarino, F., Duarte, C., Matos, M., Kolts, R., Stubbs, J., Ceresatto, L., Duarte, J., Pinto-Gouveia, J., & Basran, J. (2017). The development of compassionate engagement and action scales for self and others. Journal of Compassionate Health Care 4(4). doi:10.1186/s40639-017-0033-310.1186/s40639-017-0033-3Search in Google Scholar

Greenberg, L. S., Watson, J. C., & Goldman, R. (1998). Process-experiential therapy of depression. In L. S. Greenberg, J. C. Watson, & G. Lietaer (Eds.), Handbook of experiential psychotherapy (pp. 227–248). New York: Guilford.Search in Google Scholar

Greenberg, L., & Watson, J. (2005). Emotion-focused therapy of depression Washington D.C.: American Association Press.10.1037/11286-000Search in Google Scholar

Greenberg, L. (2011). Emotion-focused therapy: Theories of psychotherapy series. Washington: American Psychological Association.Search in Google Scholar

Gottman. J. M. (1994). Why marriages succeed or fail New York: Fireside.Search in Google Scholar

Halamová, J. (2018). Sebasúcit a sebakritickosť. Tvorba a meranie efektu intervencie [Self-compassion and self-criticism: Creating and measuring the effect of an intervention] Bratislava: Univerzita Komenského.Search in Google Scholar

Halamová, J., & Kanovský, M. (2017). Sebasúcit a sebakritickosť psychometrická analýza meracích nástrojov [Self-compassion and self-criticism: Psychometric analysis of measuring instruments]. Bratislava: Univerzita Komenského.Search in Google Scholar

Halamová, J., Baránková, M., Strnádelová, B., & Koróniová, J. (2018). Consensual qualitative research on free associations for compassion and self-compassion. Human Affairs 28(3), 253–270. doi:10.1515/humaff-2018-002110.1515/humaff-2018-0021Search in Google Scholar

Hermanto, N., Zuroff, D.C., Kopala-Sibley, D. C., Kelly, A.C., Matos, M., Gilbert, P., & Koestner, R. (2016). Ability to receive compassion from others buffers the depressogenic effect of self-criticism: A cross-cultural multi-study analysis. Personality and Individual Differences 98, 324– 332. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.05510.1016/j.paid.2016.04.055Search in Google Scholar

Hill, C. E. (2012). Consensual qualitative research: A practical resource for investigating social science phenomena Washington, DC: American Psychological AssociationSearch in Google Scholar

Ilgen, D.R., & Davis, C. A. (2000). Bearing bad news: Reactions to negative performance feedback. Applied Psychology: An International Review 49, 550–565.10.1111/1464-0597.00031Search in Google Scholar

Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72, 1245–1267. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.124510.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1245Search in Google Scholar

Kirby, J. N., Tellegen, C. L., & Steindl, S. R. (2017). A MetaAnalysis of Compassion-based Interventions: Current State of Knowledge and Future Directions. Behavior Therapy, 48, 778-792.10.1016/j.beth.2017.06.003Search in Google Scholar

Kluger, A.N., & DeNisi, A. (1996). The effect of feedback interventions on performance: A historical review, a meta-analysis, and a preliminary feedback intervention theory. Psychological Bulletin 119, 254–284.10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.254Search in Google Scholar

Liden, R. C., & Mitchell, T. R. (1985). Reactions to feedback: The role of attributions. Academy of Management Journal 28(2), 291–308. doi:10.5465/25620210.5465/256202Search in Google Scholar

Neuman, J. H., & Baron, R.A. (1998). Workplace violence and workplace aggression: Evidence concerning specific forms, potential causes, and preferred targets. Journal of Management 2, 391–419.10.1177/014920639802400305Search in Google Scholar

Quigley, B. M., & Tedeschi, J. T. (1996). Mediating effects of blame attributions on feelings of anger. Personal and Social Psychology Bulletin 22, 1280–1288.10.1177/01461672962212008Search in Google Scholar

Raver, J.L., Jensen, J.M., Lee, J., & O’Reilly, J. (2012). Destructive criticism revisited: Appraisals, ask outcomes, and the moderating role of competitiveness. Applied Psychology 61(2), 177–203.10.1111/j.1464-0597.2011.00462.xSearch in Google Scholar

Shahar, G. (2015). Erosion: The Psychopathology of Self-Criticism New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/med:psych/9780199929368.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Werner, A. M., Tibubos, A. N., Rohrmann, S., & Reiss, N. (2019). The clinical trait self-criticism and its relation to psychopathology: A systematic review – Update. Journal of Affective Disorders 246, 530–547. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2018.12.06910.1016/j.jad.2018.12.069Search in Google Scholar

Whelton, W. J., & Greenberg, L. S. (2005). Emotion in self-criticism. Personality and Individual Differences 38, 1583–1595. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.02410.1016/j.paid.2004.09.024Search in Google Scholar

Whelton, W. J., & Henkelman, J. J. (2002). A verbal analysis of forms of self-criticism. Alberta Journal of Educational Research 48(1), 88–90.Search in Google Scholar

Whelton, W. J., Paulson, B., & Marusiak, C. W. (2007). Self-criticism and the therapeutic relationship Counselling Psychology Quarterly 20, 135–148. doi:10.1080/0951507070141242310.1080/09515070701412423Search in Google Scholar

Appendix 1

Detailed categorization of data with specific examples for stimulus word criticism.

Domain Subdomain Category Characteristic Example
Motivational function (30) Change (11) change, new possibilities, motivation
Progress (19) being better, progress attack, conflict, cursing
Aggression expressions (9) honesty, bravery, expressing opinion
Behavioral aspects (125) Behavioral expressions (57) Constructive expression (25) lecturing someone, gossiping, pointing only mistakes
Negative expressions (32) advice, help, learning
How to handle criticism (29) Specific acts (22) Changing mindset (7) not taking it personally, being able to accept it, knowing how to handle it

Content (36) Neutral content (12) Negative content (14) evaluation negative feedback, underlining mistakes
Constructive content (10) feedback, another point of view
Cognitive aspects (104) Evaluation (104) Form (65) Evaluation of importance (4) Neutral form (3) Negative form (25) Constructive form (37) truth, adequate excessive, harsh constructive, justified important, essential
Internal source (6) Characteristics (4) Self-regard (2) weaknesses me, self-worth
People (16) family, enemy, rival
Work related (10) work, school, carrier
Preconditions (69) External sources (63) Things (24) problem, game, mistakes
Media (11) newspaper, film, review
Characteristics (2) fake, rigidity

Emotional aspects (48) Emotions (48) General negative emotions (12) Specific negative emotions (36) bad feelings, hate it anger, fear, sorrow
Appendix 2

Detailed categorization of data with specific examples for stimulus word self-criticism.

Domain Subdomain Category Characteristic Example
Change (12) change, new beginning, motivation
Motivational function (42) Progress (30) improvement, progress
Behavioral aspects (133) Behavioral expressions (50) Constructive expression (8) Negative expressions (42) honesty, maturity, openness lecturing someone, crisis, crying
How to handle criticism (41) Specific acts (16) Changing mindset (25) discipline, help, learning realization, understanding, take it easy

Internal source (67) Characteristics (18) Self-regard (49) laziness, perfectionism, arrogance low self-esteem, underestimation
Preconditions (87) External sources (20) People, work, things (10) Situations. Relationships (10) family, work, mistakes relationships, situation, fail
Cognitive aspects (74) Evaluation (74) Content (42) Neutral content (30) Negative content (5) Constructive content (7) Neutral form (6) Evaluation, self-evaluation, analysis too much criticism, criticism reality, analysis of my mistakes
Form (32) Negative form (8) Constructive form (18) perception, being objective, self-reflection excessive, biased, negative
constructive, right, adequate
Positive emotions (10) love, self-love, happiness

Emotional aspects (48) Emotions (48)
Negative emotions (38) anger, sorrow, pity, hate
Published Online: 2020-08-07
Published in Print: 2020-07-28

© 2020 Institute for Research in Social Communication, Slovak Academy of Sciences

Downloaded on 25.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/humaff-2020-0032/html
Scroll to top button