Skip to main content
Log in

An Examination of the Contribution of Dispositional Affect on Ethical Lapses

  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The popular press and academic research has focused primarily on the characteristics of corporate leaders. Subordinates have been studied much less frequently than leaders and yet they play a pivotal role in destructive leadership processes. An area holding significant potential to bring clarity to subordinates’ ability to withstand (or succumb) to pressures from superiors is dispositional affect. In our exploratory study, we examine how specific affective states influence subordinates’ unethical behavior. We performed an experiment with 63 mid-level managers having significant work experience. Participants were given ethical scenarios and asked to assess their intentions to comply with their superiors’ requests to engage in unethical conduct. The participants also completed the positive affect negative affect schedule (PANAS) which provides measures of affective states. Our results provide support for theory-based predictions. The findings of our study make important contributions and have implications to both practice and theory. First, we identify certain affective states that encourage subordinates to adopt the behavior of a conformer or colluder and thus be susceptible to their superiors’ unethical directives. Second, our results suggest the need for training programs to assist employees in managing affect in the work place and consideration of organizational changes that provide a culture of empowerment of its employees. Third, unlike a large majority of prior research, we measured naturally occurring affective states rather than providing a contrived (and potentially exaggerated) triggering event to elicit affective states. Fourth, we examined specific affective states rather than examining only general positive and negative valence categories.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Although the terms positive and negative affect suggest that these states are opposites (i.e., negatively correlated), they have been found to be independent states (Cacioppo et al. 1999; Connelly et al. 2004; Watson and Tellegen 1985).

  2. Our participants’ affect would be considered an outgrowth of various general life experiences and nearly 9 years experience in the business world, primarily as a subordinate.

  3. WorldCom management often asked their employees to misstate accounts. Given that these individuals risked their current salaries and bonuses by not succumbing to these demands, they acquiesced and stayed with the firm (Reinstein et al. 2006).

  4. Similar results were obtained using an unweighted scale.

  5. The raw mean scores are 25.70 for the high level of frustration and 20.12 for the low level of frustration.

References

  • Adophs, R., & Damasio, A. R. (2001). The interaction of affect and cognition: A neurobiological perspective. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), The handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 27–49). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amernic, J., & Craig, R. (2010). Accounting as a facilitator of extreme narcissism. Journal of Business Ethics, 1(1), 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. J. (2003). The psychology of doing nothing: Forms of decision avoidance result from reason and emotion. Psychological Bulletin, 129(1), 139–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Associated Press. (2005, September 6). Ex-WorldCom Accountant Gets Prison Term. The New York Times. http://query.nytimes.com/gst/fullpage.html?res=9C06E1DC163EF935A3575BC0A9639C8B63.

  • Au, K., Chan, F., Wang, D., & Vertinsky, I. (2003). Mood in foreign exchange trading: cognitive processes and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91, 322–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernard, M. (1998). Validation of the general attitude and belief scale. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 16, 183–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhattacharjee, S., & Moreno, K. (2002). The impact of affective information on the professional judgments of more experienced and less experienced auditors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15, 361–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birnberg, J. G. (2011). A proposed framework for behavioral accounting research. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 23(1), 1–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H. (2000). The interplay of affect and cognition: The mediating role of general knowledge structures. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 201–202). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boddy, C. R., Ladyshewsky, R., & Galvin, P. G. (2010). The influence of corporate psychopaths on corporate social responsibility and organizational commitment to employees. Journal of Business Ethics, 97(1), 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1999). The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 839–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carson, T. L. (2003). Self-interest and business ethics: Some lessons of the recent corporate scandals. Journal of Business Ethics, 43, 389–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caruso, E. M., & Shafir, E. (2006). Now that you mention it, I’m in the mood for laughs: Decisions focused on mood. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 155–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, J., Cohen, J. R., & Monroe, G. S. (2008). The effect of moods on auditors’ inventory valuation decisions. Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory, 27(2), 137–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, J, Cohen, J. R., & Monroe, G. S. (2011). The influence of ethical conflict and emotion on auditors’ inventory judgments. Working paper, Boston College.

  • Cianci, A. M., & Bierstaker, J. L. (2009). The impact of positive and negative mood on the hypothesis generation and ethical judgments of auditors. Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory, 28(2), 119–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cieslewicz, J. (2010). Earnings management through the management of employees. Working paper, University of Hawaii.

  • Cohen, J. R., Pant, L., & Sharpe, D. J. (1993). Culture-based ethical conflicts confronting multinational accounting firms. Accounting Horizons, 7(3), 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. R., Pant, L., & Sharpe, D. J. (1995). An exploratory examination of international differences in auditors’ ethical perceptions. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 7, 37–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connelly, S., Helton, W., Schultz, R., Van Doorn, J., Benavidez, J., Thompson, H., & Mumford, M. (2000). Assessment tool report: review of existing tools and development of new tools (Vols. 1 and 2). Technical report for the Department of Defense, University of Oklahoma.

  • Connelly, S., Helton-Fauth, W., & Mumford, M. D. (2004). A managerial in-basket study of the impact of trait emotions on ethical choice. Journal of Business Ethics, 51, 245–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, J. R., & Henry, J. D. (2004). The positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS): Construct validity, measurement properties, and normative data in a large clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 245–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Creyer, E. H., & Kozup, J. C. (2003). An examination of the relationships between coping styles, task-related affect, and the desire for decision assistance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 90, 37–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cyriac, K., & Dharmaraj, R. (1994). Machiavellianism in Indian management. Journal of Business Ethics, 13(4), 281–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dacin, T., & Murphy, P. R. (2009). Understanding and preventing unethical conduct in organizations: A situation and affect-based fraud framework. Working paper, Queen’s School of Business.

  • Davis, S., DeZoort, F. T., & Kopp, L. S. (2006). The effect of obedience pressure and perceived responsibility on management accountants’ creation of budgetary slack. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 18, 19–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeSteno, D., Petty, R., Rucker, D., & Wegener, D. (2000). Beyond valence in the perception of likelihood: The role of emotion specificity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 397–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ding, S. & Beaulieu, P. (2009). The balanced scorecard and mood congruency bias. Working paper, York University.

  • Donnelly, D. P., Quirin, J. J., & O’Bryan, D. (2003). Auditor acceptance of dysfunctional audit behavior: An explanatory model using auditors’ personal characteristics. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 15, 87–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & McDermott, R. (2008). Emotion and the framing of risky choice. Political Behavior, 30, 297–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duchon, D., & Drake, B. (2009). Organizational narcissism and virtuous behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 85(3), 301–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fedor, D. B., & Ramsay, R. J. (2007). Effects of supervisor power on preparers, responses to audit review: A field study. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 19(1), 91–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1992). Affect in social judgments and decisions: A multi-process model. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P., & George, J. M. (2001). Affective influence on judgments and behavior in organizations: An information processing perspective. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86(1), 3–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fraedrich, J., Ferrell, O. C., & Pride, W. M. (1989). An empirical examination of three Machiavellian concepts. Journal of Business Ethics, 8(9), 687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaudine, A., & Thorne, L. (2001). Emotion and ethical decision-making in organizations. Journal of Business Ethics, 31(2), 175–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • George, J. M., & Brief, A. P. (1992). Feeling good–doing good: A conceptual analysis of the mood at work–organizational spontaneity relationship. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 310–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • George, J. M., & Brief, A. P. (1996). Motivational agendas in the workplace: The effects of feelings on focus of attention and work motivation. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior. Green, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • George, J. M., & Jones, G. R. (1997). Experiencing work: Values, attitudes, and moods. Human Relations, 50, 393–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graen, G. B., & Uhi-Bien, M. (1995). Development of leader-member exchange (LMX) theory of leadership over 25 years: Applying a multi-level domain perspective. Leadership Quarterly, 6, 219–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J. (1998). Sharpening the focus: Emotion regulation, arousal, and social competence. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 287–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., Sigelman, J. D., Bohlig, A., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2003). Anger, coping, and frontal cortical activity: The effects of coping potential on anger-induced left frontal activity. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helton, W., Benavidez, J., & Connelly, M. (2000). Emotion and emotional processes: A review and some new theoretical considerations. Technical report for the Department of Defense, University of Oklahoma.

  • Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollander, E. P. (1992). The essential interdependence of leadership and followship. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 71–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollander, E. P., & Offermann, L. (1990). Power and leadership in organizations: Relationships in transition. American Psychologist, 45, 179–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iacobucci, D., & Duhachek, A. (2003). Advancing alpha: Measuring reliability with confidence. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13, 478–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. G., Deis, D. R., Bedard, J. C., & Curtis, M. B. (2008). Accounting firm culture and governance: A research synthesis. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 20(1), 45–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kadous, K. (2001). ‘Improving jurors’ evaluation of auditors in negligence cases. Contemporary Accounting Research, 18(3), 425–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kellerman, B. (2004). Bad leadership: What it is, how it happens, why it matters. Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion and personality. In C. Malatesta-Magai & S. S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult development, and aging (pp. 385–401). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kida, T. E., Moreno, K. K., & Smith, J. F. (2001). The influence of affect on managers, capital-budgeting decisions. Contemporary Accounting Research, 18(3), 477–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laros, F., & Steenkamp, J. (2005). Emotions in consumer behavior: A hierarchical approach. Journal of Business Research, 58, 1437–1445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Progress on a cognitive–motivational–relational theory of emotion. American Psychologist, 46(8), 819–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, K., & Allen, J. N. (2002). Organizational citizenship behavior and workplace deviance: The role of affect and cognitions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 131–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J., Gonzalez, R., Small, D., & Fischhoff, B. (2003). Effects of fear and anger on perceived risk of terrorism: A national experiment. Psychological Science, 14, 144–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgments and choice. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 473–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Portrait of the angry decision maker: How appraisal tendencies shape anger’s influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 115–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libby, R. R., Tan, H. T., & Seybert, N. (2008). Relationship incentives and the optimistic/pessimistic pattern in analysts, forecasts. Journal of Accounting Research, 46(1), 173–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. F., Hsee, C. K., Weber, E. U., & Welsh, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 267–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, R. G., & Brown, D. G. (2004). Leadership processes and follower self-identity. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, A. T., & DeZoort, F. T. (2001). The impact of commitment and moral reasoning on auditors’ responses to social influence pressure. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 26, 215–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Louwers, T., Ponemon, L., & Radtke, R. (1997). Examining accounting’ ethical behavior: A review and implications for future research. In V. Arnold & S. Sutton (Eds.), Behavioral foundations and frontiers (pp. 44–88). Sarasota, FL: American Accounting Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, A. P. (1970). Revised scale for ambiguity tolerance: Reliability and validity. Psychological Reports, 26, 791–798.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 602–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLean, B., & Elkind, P. (2005). The smartest guys in the room. New York: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, K., Kida, T., & Smith, J. (2002). The impact of affective reactions on risky decision making in accounting contexts. Journal of Accounting Research, 40(5), 1331–1349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, G., & Gilbertson, D. (1991). Machiavellianism revisited. Journal of Business Ethics, 10(8), 633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Otley, D. T., & Pierce, B. J. (1996). The operation of control systems in large audit firms. Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory, 15(2), 65–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Padilla, A., Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. (2007). The toxic triangle: destructive leaders, susceptible followers, and conducive environments. Leadership Quarterly, 18, 176–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, E., Vastfjall, D., Garling, T., & Slovic, P. (2006). Affect and decision making: A “hot” topic. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 79–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ponemon, L. A., & Gabhart, D. (1993). The influence of ethical reasoning on auditors’ perceptions of management’s competence and integrity. Advances in Accounting, 11, 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayburn, J. M., & Rayburn, L. G. (1996). Relationship between Machiavellianism and type A personality and ethical-orientation. Journal of Business Ethics, 15(11), 1209–1219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reinstein, A., Moehrle, S., & Reynolds-Moehrle, J. (2006). Crime and punishment in the marketplace; accountants and business executives repeating history. Managerial Auditing Journal, 21, 420–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125(1), 3–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sawers, K. M. (2005). Evidence of choice avoidance in capital-investment judgments. Contemporary Accounting Research, 22(4), 1063–1092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, D. N., & Kadous, K. (1997). The joint effects of task-related negative affect and task difficulty in multiattribute choice behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70(2), 159–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storm, C., & Storm, T. (1987). A taxonomic study of the taxonomy of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 805–816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweeney, J. T., & Summers, S. L. (2002). The effect of busy season workload on public accountants’ job burnout. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 14, 223–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, T., & Chen, Y. J. (2010). Intelligence vs. wisdom: The love of money, Machiavellianism, and unethical behavior across college major and gender. Journal of Business Ethics, 1(1), 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98(2), 219–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, J. T. (2001). Why employees commit fraud: Its either green or need. Journal of Accountancy, 191(2), 89–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yukl, G. A. (2005). Leadership in organizations (6th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge the helpful comments and suggestions from Anna Cianci and Amber Whisenhunt. We also appreciate comments from participants at the 2009 Accounting, Behavior, and Organizations Research Conference.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to D. Jordan Lowe.

Appendices

Appendix A

Ethical Scenarios

  1. 1.

    You are a part-time university student and a fulltime employee for a small start-up company which has significant career opportunities for you. One of the owner/partners asks you to use university software to which you have access through your courses to store and process firm data. Licensing agreements between the university and the software company restrict use to educational purposes.

  2. 2.

    You are asked by your manager to “intentionally misfile” interoffice memos supporting claims of gender based harassment…claims called a “trivial and distracting nuisance” by your manager.

  3. 3.

    Your boss and close friend asked you to temporarily stall any employee complaints by demanding excessive documentation and legalistic procedures. Your friend is up for consideration for a major promotion with transfer. His advancement could also be in your career interests.

  4. 4.

    Your company is fighting a hostile takeover bid by another company. To secure stockholder support the firm needs to show more profits. As head accountant for the firm, you are asked to do your part…you are asked to temporarily revise to lower levels than you believe appropriate estimated expenses related to product defects, warrantee costs, and product obsolescence.

  5. 5.

    Your direct supervisor is close to an important profit benchmark for your branch that would provide him with a large bonus. To help achieve that profit level, he asks you to defer certain costs into next year, specifically, as head of marketing to delay routine advertising layout production and radio and newsprint advertising for the last 3 weeks of the year. The requested reduced marketing activity will cost the company some sales as it enters the new year.

  6. 6.

    Given a limited pool of money for raises this year, you have been asked by the head of personnel to withhold and redirect merit pay raises from employees who cannot leave the firm to less deserving but younger and more-mobile employees.

  7. 7.

    In response to a call from corporate headquarters asking for more profits, the head of production (your direct boss) asks you to schedule overtime in shipping to push out extra product and hold the records open for an extra 12 h after year end (that is, back date shipping logs showing shipments for the first 12 h of the new year as shipments of the current year).

  8. 8.

    The head of division accounting directs you and a colleague to defer recognition of material obsolescence experienced related to selected assets. Your best estimate is that the assets have lost 50% of their value. Your colleague stresses “We need to go along with this. Instead of recognizing this loss this year, let’s wait it out and take another look at it next year when we have more information.”

  9. 9.

    A current employee has been very difficult to work from day one. This employee has been argumentative with you on several occasions and wastes everyone’s time with lots of complaints. In response to a direct request from your department manager, would you agree to undertake an effort to get rid of this employee by making assignments to inferior clients with lots of travel and overtime.

  10. 10.

    In response to a direct request from your supervisor seeking enhanced revenues, and for purposes of keeping your job, would you assist in development of a marketing campaign known to be deceptive regarding product capabilities and durability (not safety)?

Participants responded to each scenario using a five-point scale: (1) “I absolutely would not agree to do this”, (2) “I probably would not agree to do this”, (3) “Uncertain”, (4) I probably would agree to do this”, and (5) “I absolutely would agree to do this.” Thus, higher scores reflect an intention to carry out arguably unethical behavior.

Appendix B

  Positive affect negative affect schedule (PANAS)

Appendix C

Tolerance of Ambiguity Scale

  1. 1.

    An expert who doesn’t come up with a definite answer probably doesn’t know too much.

  2. 2.

    In the long run, it is possible to get more done by tackling small, simple problems than by tackling large and complicated ones.

  3. 3.

    Many of our most important decisions in life are based upon insufficient information.

  4. 4.

    People who fit their lives to a schedule probably miss most of the joy of living.

  5. 5.

    There is really no such thing as a problem that can’t be solved.

  6. 6.

    It is more fun to tackle a complicated problem than a simple one.

  7. 7.

    People who insist on a “yes” or “no” answer just don’t know how complicated things really are.

  8. 8.

    A good job is one where what is to be done and how it is to be done are always clear.

  9. 9.

    The sooner we all acquire similar values and ideas, the better.

  10. 10.

    A person who leads an even, regular life in which few surprises or unexpected happenings arise, really have a lot to be grateful for.

  11. 11.

    I would like to live in a foreign country for awhile.

  12. 12.

    Teachers or supervisors who hand out vague assignments give a chance for one to show initiative and originality.

  13. 13.

    What we are used to is always preferable to what is unfamiliar.

  14. 14.

    I like parties where I know most of the people more than ones where all or most of the people are complete strangers.

  15. 15.

    A good teacher is one who makes you wonder about your way of looking at things.

  16. 16.

    Often, the most interesting and stimulating people are those who don’t mind being different or original.

Participants indicated their agreement with these questions on a seven-point scale: (1) “Strongly Disagree”, (2) “Moderately Agreement”, (3) “Slightly Disagree”, (4) “Neutral”, (5) “Slightly Agree”, (6) “Moderately Agree”, and (7) “Strongly Agree”.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lowe, D.J., Reckers, P.M.J. An Examination of the Contribution of Dispositional Affect on Ethical Lapses. J Bus Ethics 111, 179–193 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-011-1200-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-011-1200-7

Keywords

Navigation