Skip to content
Publicly Available Published by De Gruyter January 1, 2022

Percieved Stress in Emerging Adulthood: The Role of Sense of Control and the Mediation Effects of Religiosity and Materialistic Values

  • Muhammad Rehan Masoom EMAIL logo
From the journal Human Affairs

Abstract

The research addresses the effect of sense of control on perceived stress by controlling for the intervening effects of Religiosity and Materialism. A total of 609 emerging adults living in Dhaka city participated in the survey; surveyors used a 48-item structural closed-ended questionnaire to collect the responses. The elicited responses were quantified, and structural equation models were formulated to identify any associations among the variables of interest. The findings suggest that sense of control is a strong determinant of perceived stress; neither religiosity nor materialism mediate the association. The study advances the transactional model of stress and coping.

Introduction

Humans are social creatures by nature, with a fundamental need to sustain healthy social relationships (Slavich et al., 2010). Social relationships can provide a means of securing the necessities of life, promote feelings of social integration, and sometimes help the pursuit of reproductive potential (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Anything that interferes (or threatens to interfere) with social relationships can cause social stress. Stress indicates “a particular relationship between the person and the environment that is appraised by the person as taxing or exceeding his or her resources and endangering his or her well-being” (Lazarus & Folkman, 1987, p. 19). Low social status in the community or in society, delivering a speech at a public gathering, interviewing with prospective employers, caring for a child or partner with a long-term illness, meeting new people in a crowd, the threat or actual loss of a loved one, divorce, and prejudice are among some of the things that generate stress (Taylor, 2015). Recent research has identified three basic sources of social stresses that require a different degree of adaptation. First, there are significant life changes to which a person must quickly adapt, such as sudden physical injury or sexual assault; second, there is a series of events that the person has to adapt to over a long period, such as unemployment or divorce; third, there are daily annoyances or minor events, such as bad traffic or an uncooperative workenvironment, that necessitate regular adaptation (Carr & Umberson, 2013). The transactional model of stress and coping proposes that people usually find it beneficial to maintain positive social ties, but often feel powerless to sustain them (Lazarus & Folkman, 1987).

Humans need to develop the capacity to adapt to change in healthy and flexible ways in the face of adversity (Masten & Reed, 2002). Because the key changes in functional capacity, educational attainment, and social roles are extensive and varied, and have a considerable influence on life course outcomes, emerging adulthood is a particularly crucial stage in the human lifespan for understanding the responses to stressors (Wood et al., 2018). Emerging adulthood is the period from the late teens to the twenties, when individuals not only experience multiple biological, social, and psychological transitions but also have to face educational choices, manage multiple roles, discover the novel meaning of such roles, and navigate tasks in an uncertain climate (Arnett, 2000). These transformations require a new set of skills, values, attitudes, and social capital if they are to progress toward selfsufficiency and adult modes of interdependence (Eccles et al., 2003). Positive development in emerging adulthood is related to stronger family and peer interactions, improved school adjustment, higher family financial status, and improved emotional regulation (O’Connor et al., 2011). The present study places particular emphasis on the effect of the sense of control on perceived stress in emerging adulthood and considers religiosity and materialistic values as the coping mechanism for them.

The present research focuses on emerging adults’ primary appraisals to extend our understanding of social stresses in terms of sense of control, which is yet to be conclusively outlined, particularly in the urban context of a developing economy like Bangladesh. Given that perceived stress consists of stressors that have high prevalence in urban areas, where emerging adults often endure high levels of discrimination, stressful life events, neighborhood problems, and family stress, what role does sense of personal control play in relation to perceived stress? Does religiosity help to reduce stress? Perhaps religiosity enhances the stresses by creating dissonance if they become materialistic? Do religiosity and materialistic values mediate the effect of sense of control on perceived stress? If it is found that perceived stress and sense of control are constantly associated with each other regardless of the degree of religiosity and materialism, it can be argued that neither religion nor materialistic ideals is a useful coping mechanism for emerging adults.

Background and hypotheses

A sense of control is the presumption and impression that one can (or cannot) regulate one’s immediate social circumstances through one’s behavior (Ross et al., 2001). Personal mastery and perceived constraints are two ends of the sense of control continuum (Lachman & Weaver, 1998), where personal mastery indicates «one’s sense of efficacy or effectiveness in carrying out goals» and perceived constraints express the «extent one believes there are obstacles or factors beyond one’s control that interfere with reaching goals” (Lachman & Weaver, 1998, p. 765). In other words, a high sense of control manifests in the belief that one can shape the circumstances and events in one’s life, whereas a low sense of control expresses the belief that one’s actions cannot affect immediate events and circumstances (Ross & Mirowsky, 1989). Sense of personal control is the learned, generalized belief that one’s decisions and behaviors will determine one’s outcomes; the person believes they can master, control, and effectively change their surroundings (Ross et al., 1983; Ross & Mirowsky, 1989; Rotter, 1966; Seeman, 2001). Perceived constraints, on the other hand, is the cognitive understanding of a misalignment between one’s objectives and the resources available for achieving them (Ross & Mirowsky, 1989). Conversely, ‘something’ or perhaps some ‘important things’ or even ‘all the things’ that are happening unexpectedly or that may happen in the future determine the frequency with which the person becomes nervous, stressed or upset, and makes them feel they are unable to control their lives, cope with the circumstances, and overcome the difficulties and get so angry at the intractable nature of their circumstances—all of which leads to perceived stress (Cohen, 1994). Feeling helpless and lacking in self-efficacy is the cognitive perception through which the outcome of social experiences are attributed to external forces, influential others, and fate or destiny (Foulk et al., 2020)prior social power research has predominantly focused on consequences of “powerfulness.” This has led to contradictory predictions for how experienced powerlessness influences employees’ social perceptions and behaviors. To resolve this theoretical tension, we build on Social Distance Theory (Magee & Smith. Therefore, where the objective circumstances of the constraints trigger a lack of control over the life events, this can contribute to social stress (Cohen & Edwards, 1989). Thus, we can predict the following:

H1: Emerging adults with a low sense of control have high perceived stress.

Religiosity plays a role in dealing with stress and symptoms of depression (O. Harrison et al., 2001). Religiosity refers to “an individual’s beliefs and behaviors in relation to the supernatural and/or high-intensity values” (Roof, 1979, p. 18). Religious actions, such as prayer, congregational assistance, pastoral care, or even mere religious faith help people to cope with social stress by reducing their sense of powerlessness (Pargament et al., 2001). However, less is understood about how religious roles are linked to a sense of control. Previous studies have found that the belief in God’s intervention in earthly matters, or Godpower, is both negatively related (Spilka & Schmidt, 1983) and positively related (Silvestri, 1979), and at times has no association with personal control (Spilka et al., 1985). Believing in God is an active force in one’s life that necessitates a loss of personal or internal power (Schieman et al., 2006). Religion is often criticized for encouraging passivity and acceptance of the status quo (Marx & Engels, 1967). That is because individuals may have low expectations about their ability to manage their reinforcements after witnessing pronounced discriminatory action (Lefcourt & Ladwig, 1965). Belief in God’s control has also been linked to intrinsic religious devotion (Spilka et al., 1985), high religiosity (Pargament & Sullivan, 1981), and high church attendance (Spilka et al., 1985). Conversely, compared to those who believe more in chance, social powers, and internal unconscious dynamics, people believing everything is under God’s control feel they that can exert greater control over things by simply praying to God and doing good deeds (Furnham, 1982). Therefore, one significant point of contention concerns the impact of religious precepts and practices on one’s sense of personal control (Ellison & Burdette, 2012; Jackson & Coursey, 1988; Schieman et al., 2006)1988; Schieman et al., 2006. Hence, we can hypothesize:

H2: Emerging adults with high perceived constraints have high religiosity.

People with an acute sense of powerlessness may be more affected by others’ consumption attitudes toward buying high-end goods since they are more receptive to interpersonal input (Roberts et al., 2005). The term materialistic values refers to “the importance a person places on possessions and their acquisition as a necessary or desirable form of conduct to reach desired end states, including happiness” (Richins & Dawson, 1992, p. 307). People with higher materialistic ideals have more favorable attitudes toward the idea that luxury items and goods can aid the development of a positive self-image (Shrum et al., 2013). They may even choose to eat foods that others give positive feedback on and ignore those associated with negative comments (Netemeyer et al., 1995). Consequently, individuals who base their self-worth on external factors are often insecure in self-worth and negatively react to their own non-competitive personal appearance in social circumstances (Deci & Ryan, 1995; Roberts et al., 2014). Since certain possessions can provide some temporary comfort, individuals often proliferate their materialism levels in response to their feelings of threat and insecurity (Zeigler-Hill et al., 2011). Therefore, desired possessions can provide personal protection (Christopher et al., 2009), serve as a replacement for interpersonal dependence (Marcus et al., 2014), reinforce self-image (Dittmar & Drury, 2000), and symbolically provide a kind of recovery of self-identity (Wattanasuwan, 2005). Likewise, individuals with an acute sense of powerlessness achieve a sense of worth by cultivating a self-appealing appearance through their possessions and project dominance over others; thus, they believe that their materialistic values will satisfy their desire for social recognition (Crocker & Nuer, 2003), and enhance their social acceptance (Isaksen & Roper, 2012). While there are as yet no conclusive findings on whether a feeling of powerlessness is clearly connected to materialism across various cultures, we know that, to varying degrees, a dependence on external influences to sustain one’s self-worth makes people more likely to support materialistic ideals (Zhang et al., 2020). In addition, most previous research on materialism has consistently shown that challenges to self-esteem, such as poor task success, feelings of vulnerability, and social isolation, all lead to escalated materialistic values (J. Jiang et al., 2015). Therefore, we can predict the following:

H3: Emerging adults with a low self of control have high materialistic values.

H4: Emerging adults with high materialistic values have high perceived stress.

Based on this prediction, we would like to explore the following hypothesis:

H5: Religiosity and Materialistic values mediate the effect of the sense of control on perceived stress in emerging adulthood.

Methods

Sampling and data collection

The study followed the superpopulation model (Isaki & Fuller, 1982) to avoid selection bias in the sampling procedure. A superpopulation is the infinite population considered part of the enumeration of a finite population. Data were collected from thirty popular shopping malls in ten randomly selected postal zones from the fifty zones in Dhaka city. The shoppers were mostly socially affiliated affluent middle class; hence, it was more likely that their perceived stress was not affected by other social phenomena, such as poverty or social isolation. According to Index Mundi, about thirty million, or 18.56 percent of people in Bangladesh, belong to the target age group. The smallest possible representative sample size for performing a regression analysis is 667 respondents, at a 99 percent confidence level and 95 percent confidence interval.

It took about 14 weeks for five surveyors to collect enough data to reach the desired sample size. Data were collected from mid-December 2019 to the end of February 2020. Note that the first known case of COVID-19 in Bangladesh was reported on 8 March 2020. When collecting the data, the surveyors placed special emphasis on selecting respondents who were likely to be emerging adults. The surveyors collected the data by writing down respondents’ answers to the questionnaire items and did not stop even if the respondent indicated their age was not between 18 and 25. A total of 1,068 shoppers participated in the survey, and 690 (64.61%) were emerging adults.

Instruments and procedure

The 10-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) (Cohen et al., 1983), and 19 items from the Locus of Control of Behavior scale (LCB) (Craig et al., 1994)psychotherapy and healthy life-style programmes require subjects to take responsibility for the control of the old unwanted behaviours or to be responsible for maintaining new desired behaviours after therapy has ended. A scale to measure the locus of control of behaviour would be valuable if it could predict persons likely to relapse following apparently successful therapy. A 17-item Likert-type scale to measure this construct was developed and shown to have satisfactory internal reliability, to be test-retest reliable in the absence of treatment, to be independent of age, sex and social desirability, and to distinguish clinical disorder from normal non-clinical subjects. Furthermore, change towards internality (a reduced LCB score were used to measure the degree of sense of control. Nine items from the Religiousness Scale (Sethi & Seligman, 1993), and 10 items from the Materialistic Value-orientation Scale (Richins & Dawson, 1992) were used to quantify the variables. For further analysis, only the negatively-worded 9 items were elicited to measure LCB. Item selection depended on the validity and reliability addressed in previous studies. Sense of Control, Religiosity, and Materialism were measured on a six-point Likert scale, where 1 indicated responses such as Strongly Disagree, or Not at All Influential, or Strongly Disbelieve, whereas 6 indicated Strongly Agree, or Extremely Influential, or Strongly Believe. The higher values of these three constructs indicate a lower sense of control, higher religiosity, and higher materialism. Perceived stress was measured on a five-point Likert scale, where 0 indicated never and 4 indicated very often. Four PSS items (S4, S5, S7, S8) were reverse coded, and a higher value indicated higher perceived stress.

A structural equation model was formulated to test the hypotheses. In the equation, perceived stress was considered a second-order two-dimension (perceived control and perceived helplessness) formative construct because of the comparative high validity and reliability (cf. Lee, 2012), whereas the other three variables were unidimensional zero-order constructs. Sense of control was the exogenous variable, perceived stress was the endogenous variable, and both religiosity and materialism were treated as compounding variables in the equation. Before testing the mediation effects of religiosity and materialism, the effect of sense of control on perceived stress as well as on religiosity and materialism was tested; likewise, the effect of religion and materialism on perceived stress was also estimated. The construct validity and internal consistency of all four variables was measured, and where required some items were deleted before the hypotheses were tested. The indirect, direct, and total effects were measured, the number of bootstrap samples was set to 500, and the bias-corrected confidence intervals were set at a confidence level of 95 BC. IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 23) and IBM SPSS Amos graphics (Version 24) were used to perform the statistical analysis.

Results

Participants

The survey participants were 431 (63.8%) males and 259 females (36.2%) in emerging adulthood; their mean age was 22.10 (±1.96). About 51.7 percent of the participants were higher secondary school students, 46.5 were undergraduate students, and the rest of them were pursuing postgraduate degrees. Please see Table 1 for details of the sample.

Table 1

Demographic profile of the sample

Factor Category Gender Total
Male Female
Gender Male 431
62.5%
Female 259
37.5%
Level of Education Higher Secondary 218 139 357
50.6% 53.7% 51.7%
Undergrad 205 116 321
47.6% 44.8% 46.5%
Post-graduate 8 4 12
1.9% 1.5% 1.7%
Total 100.0% 431 259 690
100.0% 100.0%

Measurement Validity

The estimated construct validity of the four constructs – sense of control, perceived stress, religiosity, and materialism – showed good fit (CFI>0.9; CMIN/DF>1; RMSE<0.07 except religiosity; SRMR<0.05, except religiosity). The estimated internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of sense of control (α=0.600), perceived stress (α=0.566), religiosity (α=0.805), and materialism (α=0.728) indicates that perceived stress was less reliable but not unacceptably so (α < 0.5). Since numerous previous studies have confirmed the validity and reliability of the PSS, low internal consistency is of no concern for further analysis. Please see Table 2 for the standardized regression weights for the constructs; see Table 3 for more results on construct validity.

Table 2

Estimated Standardized Regression weights of the constructs

Sr. Factors and Corresponding Items β
Sense of Control (M=3.72;SD=1.42; Cronbach’s Alpha=0.600)
L3 Everyone knows that luck or chance determines ones’ future .27
L4 I can control my problem(s) only if I have outside support .27
L6 My problem(s) will dominate me all my life .59
L9 My life is controlled by outside actions and events .47
L10 People are victims of circumstance beyond their control .29
L11 To continue to manage my problems I need professional help .40
L12 When I am under stress, the tightness in muscles is due to things outside my control .35
L14 It is impossible to control my irregular and fast breathing when I am having difficulties .31
L17 In my case maintaining control over my problems is due mostly to luck .44
Perceived Stress (M=2.04;SD=1.12; Cronbach’s Alpha=0.566)
PSS1. Perceived Control .21
S1 how often have you been upset because of something that happened unexpectedly? .57
S2 how often have you felt that you were unable to control the important things in your life? .55
S3 how often have you felt nervous and «stressed»? .45
S6 how often have you found that you could not cope with all the things that you had to do? .27
S9 how often have you been angered because of things that were outside of your control? .46
S10 how often have you felt difficulties were piling up so high that you could not overcome them? .5
PSS2. Perceived Helplessness* 1.00
S4R how often have you felt confident about your ability to handle your personal problems? .65
S5R how often have you felt that things were going your way? .26
S7R how often have you been able to control irritations in your life? .30
S8R how often have you felt that you were on top of things? .29
Religiosity (M=5.26;SD=1.66; Cronbach’s Alpha=0.805)
RQ2 How important would you say religion is in your life? .52
RQ6 How much influences do your religious beliefs have on the important decisions of your life .61
Sr. Factors and Corresponding Items β
RQ7 Would you support to marry someone of another religion? .46
RQ8 How much influence do your religious beliefs have on what you wear? .75
RQ9 How much influences do your religious beliefs have on what you eat and drink? .73
RQ10 How much influence do your religious beliefs have whom you associate with? .56
RQ11 How much influence do your religious beliefs have on what social activities you undertake? .63
RQ12 Do you believe that there a is heaven? .38
RQ15 Do you believe your suffering will be rewarded? .37
Materialism (M=3.76;SD=1.44; Cronbach’s Alpha=0.728)
M1 I admire people who own expensive homes, cars, and clothes .40
M2 Some of the most important achievements in life include acquiring material possessions .47
M4 The things that I own say a lot about how well I’m doing in life. .48
M5 I like to own things that impress people. .31
M11 Buying things give me a lot of pleasure .53
M12 I like a lot of luxury in my life. .50
M15 My life would be better if I owned certain things I don’t have. .45
M16 I wouldn’t be any happier if I owned nicer things. .24
M17 I’d be happier if I could afford to buy more things. .50
M18 It sometimes bothers me quite a bit that I can’t afford to buy all the things I like. .54
Table 3

Goodness of fit indices for the formative constructs

Model Fit Index Locus of Control (LCB) PSS Religiosity Materialism
Minimum discrepancy/ degrees of freedom (CMIN/DF) 1.71 1.92 5.88 3.58
Comparative-fit index (CFI) .96 .94 .92 .91
Goodness-of-fit-index (GFI) .99 .99 .96 .97
Adjusted goodness of fit index (AGFI) .98 .97 .92 .94
Root mean square error of approximation (RMSE) [pclose] .03 [p=0.968] .04 [p=0.952] .08 [p=0.001] .06 [p=0.063]
Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) .03 .04 .05 .05

Standardized Regression Coefficients for bivariate relationships

The estimated Standardized Regression Coefficients for bivariate relationships show the predictive capacity of one variable to another. It was found that 1 score added to the low sense of control led to 0.86 high perceived stress (t=5.14 p<.001); likewise, 1 score added to the low sense of control led to 0.32 high religiosity (t=3.03, p<.01). It was also found that 1 score added to religiosity led to 0.12 high perceived stress (t=1.18, p<.05). However, the degree of sense of control could not predict the level of materialism (t=1.63, p=.103); likewise, the level of materialism could not predict the degree of perceived stress (t=0.137, p=.891). For the rest of the results, see Table 4.

Table 4

The Standardized regression coefficients in bivariate structural equation models

Predictor Outcome β b SE t p
LCB PSS .62 .86 .17 5.14 .001
LCB Religiosity .19 .32 .10 3.03 .001
LCB Materialism .09 .19 .14 1.63 .103
Religiosity PSS .13 .12 .06 2.12 .034
Materialism PSS .01 .01 .07 0.14 .891

Mediation effects of religiosity and materialism on the relationship between LCB and PSS

The standardized total effect of sense of control on perceived stress was 0.641, where the direct effect was 0.636 and the indirect effect was 0.005. There was no significant difference between the standardized total effects and the direct effects by religiosity in the equation.

Materialism did not affect the equation. Therefore, it can be said that neither religiosity nor materialism mediates the predictive capacity of the sense of control for perceived stress. For the rest of the results, see Figure 1 and Table 5.

Figure 1 
            Estimated Structured equation model (AMOS output)
Figure 1

Estimated Structured equation model (AMOS output)

Table 5

The Standardized mediation effects in multivariate structural equation model

Effects Variables LCB Materialism Religiosity
Standardized Total Effects Materialism .11
Religiosity .21* .00
PSS .64* -.04 .04
Standardized Direct Effects Materialism .11
Religiosity .21* .00
PSS .64* -.04 .04
Standardized Indirect Effects Materialism .00
Religiosity .00 .00
PSS .01 .00 .00
  1. Note. *significant, p<0.001

Discussions

Emerging adulthood, the transition from adolescence to adulthood, is a critical normative shift that typically entails extensive and frequent changes in social roles (Schulenberg et al., 2004). While late adolescence to the mid-twenties is no longer about settling into traditional adult roles, there is an increasing sense of family obligation and emerging adults are likely to contribute financially to their families (Arnett & Tanner, 2006). Since sense of responsibility was found to be connected with a higher level of emotional well-being, its effect is associated with level of education and employment opportunities (Fuligni & Pedersen, 2002). Hence, the emphasis has changed to higher education, self-discovery, and crafting a future that best fits the individual’s personal objectives and family preferences (Arnett & Tanner, 2006). Besides, the functioning and adjustment trajectories established during childhood and adolescence are likely to persist into early adulthood, interacting with (or opposing) the pervasive changes in roles and settings, where the stressors of the transition can contribute to the consolidation of transient mental health during adolescence (Schulenberg et al., 2004).

Hence, stress and depression – which are the most predictive indication of early mental disorders – emerged as a frequent temporal concern in emerging adulthood (Kessler & Walters, 1998)comorbidities, and cohort effects of DSM-III-R major depression (MD. Many youths continue to struggle significantly with transition outcomes (Berzin, 2010). Degree of exposure to violence is related to increased perceived stress in emerging adulthood (Heinze et al., 2017). Therefore, it is at this period of the transition that risky health behaviors such as substance abuse tend to peak, but it is also a period of opportunities for growth and development as young adults assume the responsibilities that come with becoming an adult (Hussong & Chassin, 2004). The current research addressed the perceived stress experienced by emerging adults living in Dhaka, the capital of Bangladesh.

Given that the urban social environment, such as Dhaka, contains stressors that are common in urban areas, and emerging adults often face prejudice, traumatic life events, community issues, and family stress, we asked what role a sense of control plays in perceived stress, and whether religiosity or materialistic values contribute to reducing stresses. The results show that while emerging adults with high perceived constraints or an external locus of control have high perceived stress (H1) and high religiosity (H2), their external sense of control does not affect their materialistic values (H3). There is no relationship between materialistic values and perceived stress (H4), and neither religiosity nor materialistic values mediate the effect of sense of control on perceived stress in emerging adulthood (H5). We can therefore say that sense of control is a strong determinant of perceived stress.

The transactional model of stress and coping suggested that the less control a person feels in a dangerous situation, the more stressful the situation is (Lazarus & Folkman, 1987). External locus of control has invariably been found to lead to a high degree of religiosity (Geist & Bangham, 1980), and emerging adults seem to be no different. However, while it is considered that religion can give comfort to deal with stress (Chen & Koenig, 2006), and high religiosity enhances people’s perceived ability to adapt to stressful life conditions (Pargament et al., 2001), the religiosity of emerging adults plays no role in reducing their perceived stress. A recent study indicates that materialism is a stable trait of emerging adults (W. Jiang et al., 2021), but the present research found otherwise, indicating that materialistic ideas vary from one context to another. In addition, it is often argued that external locus of control leads to high materialism, and high materialism leads to high stress (Burroughs & Rindfleisch, 2002; Burroughs et al., 1991)such as family values and religious values. This state of values conflict leads to psychological tension (disharmony, but the present study found no significant relationships as such.

The generalizability of the results of this research is limited by two significant conditions; first, in a developing economy, the proliferation of materialistic values is constrained by buying capacity (Güliz & Belk, 1996); second, most people in Bangladesh are religious (Bielefeldt, 2015). Hence religion is a ubiquitous personality trait shaped by cultural values (Masoom, 2020). However, our urban shopper sample is unlikely to be a complete representation of the emerging adults in Bangladesh. Hence, despite high religiosity being conclusive here, there is a need to validate in a further study the predictive capacity of a low sense of control in determining high religiosity, and particular consideration should be given to samples from rural contexts.

References

Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55(5), 469. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.46910.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469Search in Google Scholar

Arnett, J. J., & Tanner, J. L. (Eds.). (2006). Emerging adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century. American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/11381-00010.1037/11381-000Search in Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497–529. https://doi. org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.49710.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497Search in Google Scholar

Berzin, S. C. (2010). Vulnerability in the transition to adulthood: Defining risk based on youth profiles. Children and Youth Services Review, 72(4), 487–495. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. childyouth.2009.11.00110.1016/j.childyouth.2009.11.001Search in Google Scholar

Bielefeldt, H. (2015). Preliminary findings of Country Visit to Bangladesh by Heiner Bielefeldt, Special Rapporteur on freedom of religion or belief. United Nations Human Rights Press Statement. http://www.ohchr.org/EN/NewsEvents/Pages/DisplayNews.aspx?NewsID=16399Search in Google Scholar

Burroughs, J. E., & Rindfleisch, A. (2002). Materialism and well-being: A conflicting values perspective. Journal of Consumer Research, 29(3), 348–370. https://doi.org/10.1086/34442910.1086/344429Search in Google Scholar

Burroughs, J. W., Drews, D. R., & Hallman, W. K. (1991). Predicting personality from personal posessions: A self-presentational analysis. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6(6), 147–163..Search in Google Scholar

Carr, D., & Umberson, D. (2013). The social psychology of stress, health, and coping. In Handbook of social psychology (pp. 465–487). Springer.10.1007/978-94-007-6772-0_16Search in Google Scholar

Chen, Y. Y., & Koenig, H. G. (2006). Traumatic stress and religion: Is there a relationship? A review of empirical findings. Journal of Religion and Health, 45(3), 371–381. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-006-9040-y10.1007/s10943-006-9040-ySearch in Google Scholar

Christopher, A. N., Saliba, L., & Deadmarsh, E. J. (2009). Materialism and well-being: The mediating effect of locus of control. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(7), 682–686. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.00310.1016/j.paid.2009.01.003Search in Google Scholar

Cohen, S. (1994). Perceived Scale Stress. https://doi.org/10.1037/t02889-00010.1037/t02889-000Search in Google Scholar

Cohen, S., & Edwards, J. (1989). Personality characteristics as moderators of the relationship between stress and disorder. In R. W. J. Neufeld (Ed.), Advances in the investigation of psychological stress. (pp. 235–283). John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Search in Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385–396.10.2307/2136404Search in Google Scholar

Craig, A., Franklin, J., & Andrews, G. (1994). A scale to measure locus of control of behaviour. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 57(1984), 173–180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1984.tb01597.x10.1111/j.2044-8341.1984.tb01597.xSearch in Google Scholar

Crocker, J., & Nuer, N. (2003). The insatiable quest for self-worth. Psychological Inquiry, 14(1), 31–34.10.1207/S15327965PLI1403&4_9Search in Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Human autonomy. In Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem (pp. 31–49). Springer.10.1007/978-1-4899-1280-0_3Search in Google Scholar

Dittmar, H., & Drury, J. (2000). Self-image–is it in the bag? A qualitative comparison between “ordinary” and “excessive” consumers. Journal of Economic Psychology, 21(2), 109–142.10.1016/S0167-4870(99)00039-2Search in Google Scholar

Eccles, J., Templeton, J., Barber, B., & Stone, M. (2003). Adolescence and emerging adulthood: The critical passage ways to adulthood. In Well-being: Positive development across the life course. (pp. 383–406). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. https://doi.org/10.4324/978141060717110.4324/9781410607171Search in Google Scholar

Ellison, C. G., & Burdette, A. M. (2012). Religion and the sense of control among U.S. Adults. Sociology of Religion: A Quarterly Review, 73(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1093/socrel/srr03510.1093/socrel/srr035Search in Google Scholar

Foulk, T. A., Pater, I. E. D., Schaerer, M., Plessis, C. du, Lee, R., & Erez, A. (2020). It’s lonely at the bottom (too): The effects of experienced powerlessness on social closeness and disengagement. Personnel Psychology, 73(2), 363–394. https://doi.org/10.1111/peps.1235810.1111/peps.12358Search in Google Scholar

Fuligni, A. J., & Pedersen, S. (2002). Family obligation and the transition to young adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 38(5), 856–868. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.85610.1037/0012-1649.38.5.856Search in Google Scholar

Furnham, A. (1982). The Protestant work ethic and attitudes towards unemployment. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 55(4), 277–285.10.1111/j.2044-8325.1982.tb00101.xSearch in Google Scholar

Geist, C. R., & Bangham, W. R. (1980). Locus of control and religious affiliation. Psychological Reports, 47(3_suppl), 1281–1282. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1980.47.3f.128110.2466/pr0.1980.47.3f.1281Search in Google Scholar

Güliz, G., & Belk, R. W. (1996). Cross-cultural differences in materialism. Journal of Economic Psychology, 17(1), 55–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-4870(95)00035-610.1016/0167-4870(95)00035-6Search in Google Scholar

Heinze, J. E., Stoddard, S. A., Aiyer, S. M., Eisman, A. B., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2017). Exposure to violence during adolescence as a predictor of perceived stress trajectories in emerging adulthood. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 49, 31–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. appdev.2017.01.00510.1016/j.appdev.2017.01.005Search in Google Scholar

Hussong, A. M., & Chassin, L. (2004). Stress and coping among children of alcoholic parents through the young adult transition. Development and Psychopathology, 16(04). https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940404010610.1017/S0954579404040106Search in Google Scholar

Isaki, C. T., & Fuller, W. A. (1982). Survey design under the regression superpopulation model. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 77(377), 89–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459. 1982.1047777010.1080/01621459.1982.10477770Search in Google Scholar

Isaksen, K. J., & Roper, S. (2012). The commodification of self-esteem: branding and British teenagers. Psychology & Marketing, 29(3), 117–135.10.1002/mar.20509Search in Google Scholar

Jackson, L. E., & Coursey, R. D. (1988). The relationship of God control and internal locus of control to intrinsic religious motivation, coping and purpose in life. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 27(3), 399–410. https://doi.org/10.2307/138737810.2307/1387378Search in Google Scholar

Jiang, J., Zhang, Y., Ke, Y., Hawk, S. T., & Qiu, H. (2015). Can’t buy me friendship? Peer rejection and adolescent materialism: Implicit self-esteem as a mediator. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 58, 48–55.10.1016/j.jesp.2015.01.001Search in Google Scholar

Jiang, W., Liu, H., & Jiang, J. (2021). The development of materialism in emerging adulthood: Stability, change, and antecedents. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 47(2), 293–306. https://doi.org/10.1177/014616722092523410.1177/0146167220925234Search in Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., & Walters, E. E. (1998). Epidemiology of DSM-III-R major depression and minor depression among adolescents and young adults in the National Comorbidity Survey. Depression and Anxiety, 7(1), 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1520-6394(1998)7:1<3::aid-da2>3.0.co;2-f10.1002/(SICI)1520-6394(1998)7:1<3::AID-DA2>3.0.CO;2-FSearch in Google Scholar

Lachman, M. E., & Weaver, S. L. (1998). The sense of control as a moderator of social class differences in health and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(3), 763773. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.76310.1037/0022-3514.74.3.763Search in Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1987). Transactional theory and research on emotions and coping. European Journal of Personality, 1(3), 141–169. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.241001030410.1002/per.2410010304Search in Google Scholar

Lee, E.-H. (2012). Review of the psychometric evidence of the Perceived Stress Scale. Asian Nursing Research, 6(4), 121–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anr.2012.08.00410.1016/j.anr.2012.08.004Search in Google Scholar

Lefcourt, H. M., & Ladwig, G. W. (1965). The American Negro: A problem in expectancies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1(4), 377.10.1037/h0021867Search in Google Scholar

Marcus, D. K., Zeigler-Hill, V., Mercer, S. H., & Norris, A. L. (2014). The psychology of spite and the measurement of spitefulness. Psychological Assessment, 26(2), 563.10.1037/a0036039Search in Google Scholar

Marx, K., & Engels, F. (1967). The communist manifesto (1848). Trans. Samuel Moore. London: Penguin, 15.Search in Google Scholar

Masoom, M. R. (2020). Measuring Muslim Religiosity and Outlining Gender-Age Differences: The Case of Urban Middle Class in Bangladesh. Comparative Sociology, 19(1), 122–149. https://doi. org/10.1163/15691330-1234152010.1163/15691330-12341520Search in Google Scholar

Masten, A. S., & Reed, M.-G. J. (2002). Resilience in development. Handbook of Positive Psychology, 74, 88.Search in Google Scholar

Netemeyer, R. G., Burton, S., & Lichtenstein, D. R. (1995). Trait aspects of vanity: Measurement and relevance to consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(4), 612–626.10.1086/209422Search in Google Scholar

O. Harrison, M., Koenig, H. G., Hays, J. C., Eme-Akwari, A. G., & Pargament, K. I. (2001). The epidemiology of religious coping: A review of recent literature. International Review of Psychiatry, 13(2), 86–93.10.1080/09540260124356Search in Google Scholar

O’Connor, M., Sanson, A., Hawkins, M. T., Letcher, P., Toumbourou, J. W., Smart, D., Vassallo, S., & Olsson, C. A. (2011). Predictors of Positive Development in Emerging Adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(7), 860–874. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-010-9593-710.1007/s10964-010-9593-7Search in Google Scholar

Pargament, K. I., & Sullivan, M. (1981). Examining attributions of control across diverse personal situations: A psychosocial perspective. Unpublished Paper Presented at the American Psychological Association Meeting.Search in Google Scholar

Pargament, K. I., Tarakeshwar, N., Ellison, C. G., & Wulff, K. M. (2001). Religious coping among the religious: The relationships between religious coping and well-being in a national sample of Presbyterian clergy, elders, and members. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 40(3), 497–513.10.1111/0021-8294.00073Search in Google Scholar

Richins, M. L., & Dawson, S. (1992). A consumer values orientation for materialism and its measurement: Scale development and validation. Journal of Consumer Research, 19(3), 303–316. https://doi.org/10.1086/20930410.1086/209304Search in Google Scholar

Roberts, D., Hughes, M., & Kertbo, K. (2014). Exploring consumers’ motivations to engage in innovation through co-creation activities. European Journal of Marketing, 48(1/2), 147–169. https:// doi.org/10.1108/EJM-12-2010-063710.1108/EJM-12-2010-0637Search in Google Scholar

Roberts, J. A., Tanner Jr, J. F., & Manolis, C. (2005). Materialism and the family structure–stress relation. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15(2), 183–190.10.1207/s15327663jcp1502_10Search in Google Scholar

Roof, W. C. (1979). Concepts and indicators of religious commitment: A critical review. In W. C. Roof (Ed.), The Religious Dimension: New Directions in Quantitative Research (1st ed., pp. 17–45). Academic Press.Search in Google Scholar

Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (1989). Explaining the social patterns of depression: Control and problem solving–or support and talking? Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 30(2), 206–219.10.2307/2137014Search in Google Scholar

Ross, C. E., Mirowsky, J., & Cockerham, W. C. (1983). Social class, Mexican culture, and fatalism: Their effects on psychological distress. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11 (4), 383–399.10.1007/BF00894055Search in Google Scholar

Ross, C. E., Mirowsky, J., & Pribesh, S. (2001). Powerlessness and the amplification of threat: Neighborhood disadvantage, disorder, and mistrust. American Sociological Review, 66(4), 568–591. https://doi.org/10.2307/308892310.2307/3088923Search in Google Scholar

Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80(1), 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1037/h009297610.1037/h0092976Search in Google Scholar

Schieman, S., Pudrovska, T., Pearlin, L. I., & Ellison, C. G. (2006). The sense of divine control and psychological distress: Variations across race and socioeconomic status. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 45(4), 529–549.10.1111/j.1468-5906.2006.00326.xSearch in Google Scholar

Schulenberg, J. E., Sameroff, A. J., & Cicchetti, D. (2004). The transition to adulthood as a critical juncture in the course of psychopathology and mental health. Development and Psychopathology, 16(04). https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940404001510.1017/S0954579404040015Search in Google Scholar

Seeman, M. (2001). Alienation: Psychosociological tradition. In International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences (pp. 385–388). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B0-08-043076-7/01825-810.1016/B0-08-043076-7/01825-8Search in Google Scholar

Sethi, S., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1993). Optimism and fundamentalism. Psychological Science, 4(4), 256–259. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00271.x10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00271.xSearch in Google Scholar

Shrum, L. J., Wong, N., Arif, F., Chugani, S. K., Gunz, A., Lowrey, T. M., Nairn, A., Pandelaere, M., Ross, S. M., & Ruvio, A. (2013). Reconceptualizing materialism as identity goal pursuits: Functions, processes, and consequences. Journal of Business Research, 66(8), 1179–1185.10.1016/j.jbusres.2012.08.010Search in Google Scholar

Silvestri, P. J. (1979). Locus of control and God-dependence. Psychological Reports, 45(1), 89–90.10.2466/pr0.1979.45.1.89Search in Google Scholar

Slavich, G. M., O’Donovan, A., Epel, E. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2010). Black sheep get the blues: A psychobiological model of social rejection and depression. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(1), 39–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.00310.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.003Search in Google Scholar

Spilka, B., Kojetin, B., & McIntosh, D. (1985). Forms and measures of personal faith: Questions, correlates and distinctions. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 24(4), 437–442.10.2307/1385997Search in Google Scholar

Spilka, B., & Schmidt, G. (1983). General attribution theory for the psychology of religion: The influence of event-character on attributions to God. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 22(4), 326–339. https://doi.org/10.2307/138577110.2307/1385771Search in Google Scholar

Taylor, J. M. (2015). Psychometric analysis of the ten-item perceived stress scale. Psychological Assessment, 27(1), 90–101. https://doi.org/10.1037/a003810010.1037/a0038100Search in Google Scholar

Wattanasuwan, K. (2005). The self and symbolic consumption. Journal of American Academy of Business, 6(1), 179–184.Search in Google Scholar

Wood, D., Crapnell, T., Lau, L., Bennett, A., Lotstein, D., Ferris, M., & Kuo, A. (2018). Emerging adulthood as a critical stage in the life course. In N. Halfon (Eds.) et. al., Handbook of Life Course Health Development. (pp. 123–143). Springer.10.1007/978-3-319-47143-3_7Search in Google Scholar

Zeigler-Hill, V., Green, B. A., Arnau, R. C., Sisemore, T. B., & Myers, E. M. (2011). Trouble ahead, trouble behind: Narcissism and early maladaptive schemas. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42(1), 96–103.10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.07.004Search in Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Hawk, S. T., Opree, S. J., de Vries, D. A., & Branje, S. (2020). “Me”, “We”, and Materialism: Associations between Contingent Self-Worth and Materialistic Values across Cultures. The Journal of Psychology, 154(5), 386–410. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2020.175 949610.1080/00223980.2020.1759496Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2022-01-01
Published in Print: 2022-02-03

© 2021 Muhammad Rehan Masoom published by De Gruyter.

Downloaded on 1.6.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/humaff-2022-0005/html
Scroll to top button