Skip to main content
Log in

Does the Neuroscience Research on Early Stress Justify Responsive Childcare? Examining Interwoven Epistemological and Ethical Challenges

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Neuroethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper examines interwoven ethical and epistemological issues raised by attempts to promote responsive childcare practices based on neuroscience evidence on the developmental effects of early stress. The first section presents this “neuroscience argument for responsive early childcare”. The second section introduces some evidential challenges posed by the use of evidence from developmental neuroscience as grounds for parental practice recommendations and then advances a set of observations about the limitations of the evidence typically cited. Section three highlights the ethical implications of the neuroscience argument for responsive early childcare. It argues that the neuroscience argument, first, fuels unwarranted parental anxiety by unduly raising the stakes of families’ early childcare choices and, second, threatens public confidence in developmental science’s potential to inform childcare practices and policy that enhance children’s health and well being.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Cacioppo, J.T., and B.G. G., eds. 2005. Social neuroscience: key readings in social psychology. Key readings in social psychology. New York: Psychology Press, 296.

  2. Frazzetto, G., and S. Anker. 2009. Neuroculture. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 10(11): 815–821.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Madriga, A. 2010. Brain scan lie-detection deemed far from ready for courtroom, in Wired.

  4. Madriga, A. 2010. Judge issues legal opinion in Brooklyn fMRI case, in Wired.

  5. McClure, S.M., et al. 2004. Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks. Neuron 44(2): 379–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Grey, T., et al. 2003. Emory university asked to halt neuromarketing experiments, 1–5. ActionScript.

  7. Ruskin, G. 2004. Commercial alert asks senate commerce committee to investigate neuromarketing, 1–4. Commercial Altert.

  8. Racine, E., O. Bar-Ilan, and J. Illes. 2005. fMRI in the public eye. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 6(2): 159–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Nature Neuroscience, 2004. Editorial. Brain scam? 7(7): 683.

  10. Kulynych, J. 2002. Legal and ethical issues in neuroimaging research: human subjects protection, medical privacy, and the public communication of research results. Brain and Cognition 50(3): 345–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Dumit, J. 2004. Picturing personhood: brain scans and biomedical identity. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Illes, J., and T.A. Raffin. 2005. No child left without a brain scan? Toward a pediatric neuroethics. Cerebrum 7(3): 33–46.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Illes, J., E. Racine, and M. Kirschen. 2006. A picture is worth a thousand words, but which one thousand? In Neuroethics: defining the issues in research, practice and policy, ed. J. Illes, 149–168. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Racine, E., E. Bell, and J. Illes. 2010. Can we read minds? Ethical challenges and responsibilities in the use of neuroimaging research, In Neuroethics: scientific, philosophical, and ethical perspectives, ed. J. Giordano and B. Gordijn, 240–266. Cambridge University Press.

  15. OECD. 2002. Understanding the brain: toward a new learning science, vol 1, 115. Paris: OECD Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Gura, T. 2005. Educational research: big plans for little brains. Nature 435(7046): 1156–1158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Racine, E., O. Bar-Ilan, and J. Illes. 2006. Brain imaging: a decade of coverage in the print media. Science Communication 28(1): 122–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Meltzoff, A.N., et al. 2009. Foundations for a new science of learning. Science 325(5938): 284–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Racine, E., and J. Illes. 2009. “Emergentism” at the crossroads of philosophy, neurotechnology, and the enhancement debate. In Handbook of philosophy and neuroscience, ed. J. Bickle. Oxford University Press.

  20. Eschel, N., et al. 2006. Responsive parenting: interventions and outcomes. World Health Organization Bulletin 83(12): 991–998.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Barrett, H. 2006. Attachment and the perils of parenting. London: National Family and Parenting Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Simpson, J., and J. Belsky. 2008. Attachment theory within a modern evolutionary framework. In Handbook of attachment, ed. J. Cassidy and P.R. Shaver, 131–157. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Rutter, M. 2008. Implications of attachment theory and research for child care policies. In Handbook of attachment, ed. J. Cassidy and P.R. Shaver, 958–974. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Kagan, J. 1998. Three seductive ideas. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Hyman, S.E. 2009. How diversity gets under the skin. Nature Neuroscience 12(3): 241–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Thompson, R.A., and C.A. Nelson. 2001. Developmental science and the media. The American Psychologist 56(1): 5–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Pontius, A.A. 1973. Neuro-ethics of “walking” in the newborn. Perceptual and Motor Skills 37(1): 235–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Sunderland, M. 2006. The science of parenting. New York: DK Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Schore, A. 2003. Affect dysregulation and disorders of the self. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Gerhardt, S. 2004. Why love matters. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Newton, R.P. 2008. The attachment connection. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Leach, P. 2010. The essential first year. New York: DK Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Lupien, S., B.S. McEwen, M.R. Gunnmar, and C. Heim. 2009. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature 10: 434–445.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Fonagy, P., G. Gergely, E. Jurist, and M. Target. 2002. Affect-regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. New York: Other Press.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Dawson, G., S.B. Ashman, and L.J. Carver. 2000. The role of early experience in shaping behavioral and brain development and its implications for social policy. Development and Psychopathology 12: 695–712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Shonkoff, J.P. 2000. Science, policy, and practice. Child Development 71(1): 181–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Rosen, A.C., et al. 2002. Ethical, and practical issues in applying functional imaging to the clinical management of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition 50(3): 498–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Wolpe, P.R., K.R. Foster, and D.D. Langleben. 2005. Emerging neurotechnologies for lie-detection: promises and perils. The American Journal of Bioethics 5(2): 39–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Illes, J., and E. Racine. 2005. Imaging or imagining? A neuroethics challenge informed by genetics. The American Journal of Bioethics 5(2): 5–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Fenton, A., L. Meynell, and F. Baylis. 2009. Responsibility and speculation: on possible applications of pediatric fMRI. The American Journal of Bioethics 9(1): W1–W2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. McEwen, B. 2002. The end of stress as we know it. Washington, DC: Dana Press.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Racine, E., et al. 2010. Contemporary neuroscience in the media. Social Science & Medicine 71(4): 725–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Weisberg, D.S., et al. 2008. The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20(3): 470–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. McCabe, D.P., and A.D. Castel. 2008. Seeing is believing: the effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning. Cognition 107(1): 343–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Bruer, J.T. 1998. The brain and child development: time for some critical thinking. Public Health Reports 113(5): 388–398.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Chugani, H.T. 1998. Neuroscience and public policy. Public Health Reports 113(i6): 480.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Hinton, V.J. 2002. Ethics of neuroimaging in pediatric development. Brain and Cognition 50(3): 455–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Rauscher, F., G. Shaw, and K. Ky. 1993. Music and spatial task performance. Nature 365: 611.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Beatty, B. E., D. Cahan, and J. Grant, eds. 2006. When science encounters the child: perspectives on education, parenting, and child welfare in twentieth century America. New York: Teachers College Press.

  50. Karen, R. 1998. Becoming attached. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Illes, J., et al. 2010. Neurotalk: improving the communication of neuroscience research. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 11(1): 61–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Insel, T.R., and L.J. Young. 2001. The neurobiology of attachment. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 2: 129–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Narvaez, [contribution to this issue].

  54. Contratto, S. 2002. A feminist critique of attachment theory and evolutionary psychology. In Rethinking mental health and disorder: feminist perspectives, vol xxii, ed. M. Ballou and L.S. Brown, 29–47. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Blasi, A. 1990. How should psychologists define morality? Or, the negative side effects of philosophy’s influence on psychology. In The moral domain: essays in the ongoing discussion between philosophy and psychology, ed. T. Wren, 38–70. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Racine, E., E. Bell, and J. Illes. 2010. Can we read minds? Ethical challenges and responsibilities in the use of neuroimaging research. In Neuroethics: scientific, philosophical, and ethical perspectives, ed. J. Giordano and B. Gordijn, 240–266. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Vohs, K.D., and J.W. Schooler. 2008. The value of believing in free will: encouraging a belief in determinism increases cheating. Psychological Science 19(1): 49–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Racine, E., and J. Illes. 2006. Neuroethical responsibilities. The Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences 33: 269–277.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Hall, S.S. 1998. The scientific method: test-tube moms. The New York times April 5, 1998; Sect. Section 6, Column 3, page 22.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bruce Maxwell.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Maxwell, B., Racine, E. Does the Neuroscience Research on Early Stress Justify Responsive Childcare? Examining Interwoven Epistemological and Ethical Challenges. Neuroethics 5, 159–172 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-011-9110-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-011-9110-z

Keywords

Navigation