Skip to main content
Log in

Practice makes perfect: the effect of dance training on the aesthetic judge

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

According to Hume, experience in observing art is one of the prerequisites for being an ideal art critic. But although Hume extols the value of observing art for the art critic, he says little about the value, for the art critic, of executing art. That is, he does not discuss whether ideal aesthetic judges should have practiced creating the form of art they are judging. In this paper, I address this issue. Contrary to some contemporary philosophers who claim that experience in creating art is irrelevant to one’s ability to judge that art form, as well as to some dance critics who see dance training as possibly even detrimental to one’s aesthetic judgment, I suggest that having practiced dancing makes one a better observer of certain aesthetic qualities of dance. Dance training, I argue, can facilitate a kinesthetic experience upon watching dance without which some aesthetic aspects of a dance performance—such as grace, power, and precision, as perceived kinesthetically—may go unnoticed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Compare this to the experience of reading an article for enjoyment and reading that same article after you have written on the topic yourself. After practice, your reading becomes much more focused.

  2. Although it seems unlikely, one explanation of these results could be that ballet dancers and capoiera performers have innate neural differences that account for their different neural reactions. If so, their specific training is irrelevant to the observed different patterns of neural activity. However, the researchers guarded against this explanation of their results. Capoiera was chosen specifically because it is kinematically very similar to ballet. For example, they both involve turns, high kicks, and big jumps with the legs scissoring past each other. And a professional choreographer was recruited to choose the video clips that were the best kinematically matched. Most likely, our brains are not innately fine-tuned so that observing capoiera but not ballet produces a motor response in some of us while in others observing ballet but not capoiera produces a motor response.

  3. I am hesitant to say that it shows this (rather than it seems to show this) since male dancers still observe more male-specific movements and female dancers still observe more female-specific movements. This is in part because rehearsals and classes are sometimes held separately. For example, there will be a rehearsal for the men’s variation, and only the men who are doing this role will be there; and then there is men’s class and women’s pointe class. Moreover, even when in training together, women watch women more and men watch men more.

  4. A further reason for why females outperformed males may be that, in the ballet world, females generally have trained more (starting at a younger age and putting in more hours) than males.

  5. Moreover, it could be that for a trained ballet dancer, the motor–perceptual experience of watching ballet accounts, in part, for why ballet dancers generally prefer watching good ballet much more watching good performances of other physical activities, such as capoiera. See Cole and Montero (2007) for a discussion of the pleasure of bodily movement.

  6. For a discussion of expressive qualities in art, see Graham (2005).

  7. It may also be that part of the aesthetic value of ensemble work depends on having a motor–perceptual experience of the movement of the ensemble as a whole. In watching the string of dancers, hand in hand, tie themselves into a knot in Balanchine’s Concerto Barroco, for example, one appreciates not only the mathematical beauty and cleverness of the choreography but also the feeling of being tied into a knot oneself, of being, that is, the entire tangle. However, since dancers have never practiced being tied into individual knots themselves (or at least not literally), the role of dance training in facilitating this, if at all, is unclear.

  8. One further questions is, granted that dance training does improve one’s ability to aesthetically judge dance, does the rate of improvement in one’s ability to aesthetically judge dance decrease with more practice?

References

  • Barnes, C. (1976). The functions of a critic. In M. Crabb (Ed.), Visions. Proceedings of the Twenty-Fifth Anniversary Conference, The National Ballet of Canada, November 1976. Toronto: Simon & Pierre, 1979, p.24.

  • Best, D. (1974). Expression in movement and the arts. London: Lepus Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burke, E. (1990). A philosophical inquiry into the origin of our ideas of the sublime and the beautiful. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burke, E. (1757/1998). A philosophical enquiry into the origin of our ideas of the sublime and the beautiful. In A. Phillips (Ed.), Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1243–1249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology, 16(19), 1905–1910.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo-Merino, B., Ehrenberg, S., Leung, D., & Haggard, P. (2010). Experts see it all: configural effects in action observation. Psychological Research, 74(4), 400–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casile, A., & Giese, M. A. (2006). Non-visual motor learning influences the recognition of biological motion. Current Biology, 16(1), 69–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, T. (1973). A critique of Sibley's position. Theoria, 1973, 113–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, J., & Montero, B. (2007). Affective proprioception. Janus Head Journal of Interdisciplinary Studies in Literature, Continental Philosophy, Phenomenological Psychology, and the Arts, 9.2, 299–317. Special issue edited by Shaun Gallagher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denby, E. (1998). Dance writings and poetry. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, G. (2005). Expressivism. In B. Gaut & D. M. Lopes (Eds.), The Routledge companion to aesthetics (pp. 133–145). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (1757/1993). Of the standard of taste. In Andrew Edgar & S. Copley (Eds.), Selected essays. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Kivy, P. (1975). What makes ‘aesthetic’ terms aesthetic? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 36(2), 197–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McFee, G. (1992). Understanding dance. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mill, J. S. (2001/1861). Utilitarianism. Indianapolis: Hackett Publishing Company, Inc.

  • Sibley, F. (1965). Aesthetic and nonaesthetic. Philosophical Review, 74, 135–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zangwill, N. (2001). The metaphysics of beauty. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I thank Rachel Zuckert for her helpful comments on numerous aspects of this work as well as the Phenomenology and Cognitive Science anonymous referees for their extensive useful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Barbara Montero.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Montero, B. Practice makes perfect: the effect of dance training on the aesthetic judge. Phenom Cogn Sci 11, 59–68 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-011-9236-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-011-9236-9

Keywords

Navigation