Skip to main content
Log in

Some Reflections on the Evolution of Conscious Agents: The Relevance of body Plans

  • Comment
  • Published:
Biosemiotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The aim of this commentary article is to discuss several problems in the distribution map for conscious organisms and suggest a different strategy to address their evolutionary development. I propose to complement the model of Unlimited Associative Learning (UAL) that Jablonka and Ginsburg present in their Target Article with the additional consideration of how body plans constrain the possibilities of evolution of the brain— in some cases blocking, in some others enabling its complexification and corporal integration. In my view, the specific body plan of vertebrates has played a key role in the complexification and diversification of consciousness because it has favored an increasing integration between brain and body.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Notes

  1. A review of the most recent literature shows how questionable these “lower cases” are and the wide acceptance that more research is needed when dealing with them.

  2. In addition, the capacity for centralization/integration will not only depend on the number of neurons, but also on factors like their packing density, the inter-neural distance and axonal conduction velocity.

  3. Here I have focused on neurological features (like the number of neurons in the brain, the presence of a specific brain structure or its functional equivalent, the degree of centralization, re-entrant processing, neural assemblies…). Yet, many authors construct their explanations from behavioral observations. I prefer to take the behavioral data with caution, unless supported by the latter. Ethograms have limited utility for determining the cognitive capacities of animals.

  4. Compared with an aquatic environment, motility in a terrestrial environment requires, for a big size animal, exerting a significantly stronger force.

  5. For the importance of the specific form of vertebrate’s BBB see Monahan-Earley (2013).

  6. This is an interesting point that connects my own view about the emergence of C with the bio-semiotic research on the role played by information and signals in the study of living systems, since the essence of the nervous system is informational. But this is not the place to extend myself on this point.

  7. “What I mean by “strange loop” is (…) an abstract loop in which, in the series of stages that constitute the cycling-around, there is a shift from one level of abstraction (or structure) to another, which feels like an upwards movement in a hierarchy, and yet somehow the successive “upward” shifts turn out to give rise to a closed cycle. That is, despite one’s sense of departing ever further from one’s origin, one winds up, to one’s shock, exactly where one had started out. In short, a strange loop is a paradoxical level-crossing feedback loop” (Hofstadter, 2007; pp. 101–102).

  8. However, of the 500 million neurons in an octopus’ nervous system, 300 are in the arms. The tentacles of an octopus can function somewhat independently, and severed tentacles have been shown to be controllable by mechanical or electrical stimulation, suggesting that the basic motor program for voluntary movement is embedded within the neural circuitry of the tentacle itself (Hochner et al., 2006).

  9. Octopuses have integration centers of 45 million neurons.

  10. Jablonka and Ginsburg affirm that the capacity for C in cephalopods evolved 250 million after that of the vertebrates and arthropods (p. 21).

  11. Just to be a bit more specific on some of these limitations. Fast motility (at the considerable size scale of these cephalopod bodies) implies a jet propulsion system that provides thrust, which in turn requires bioenergetic, physiological, and biochemical adaptations which maximize their metabolic rates (O’Dor & Webber, 2011). Predator-prey competition with rapid fishes generates strong energetic pressure on the basic molluscan physiology of coleoids cephalopods. These animals have evolved a complex closed circulatory system (i.e., enlargement of the mantle cavity was coupled with supplementation of the systemic heart through the addition of twin accessory branchial (gill) hearts, originating from the nautilus pericardial glands, allowing the increased oxygen demands required for greater mobility). However, as argued by several authors (O’Dor, 2002; Seibel, 2016) oxygen transport is lower in cephalopods compared to fish, due to their smaller hearts relative to body size, and because they use haemocyanin (Hcy), instead of the vertebrate’s haemoglobin. Yet, Hcy—which is found only in arthropods and in molluscs– is not cell-bound as is haemoglobin in the red blood cells of vertebrates, but remains free in the blood plasma. Hcy has only half of the oxygen carrying capacity, compared to haemoglobin. In addition, as Seibel (op. cit.) has pointed out, cephalopods also have little or no venous oxygen reserves. Thus, as Jaitly and colleagues have recently argued, “under conditions of high oxygen demand, cephalopods must make maximal use of blood-borne oxygen at every circulatory cycle, as facilitated by increased gill ventilation, heart rate and stroke volume. Jet propulsion is also highly inefficient relative to the caudal fin swimming of fish.” (Jaitly et al., 2022, p 14). So it seems quite obvious that these animals are pushing their limits to the extreme.

  12. Somehow, in brief, the view presented here could be considered as a much more radical version of the embodied approach to C that their work also supports.

References

  • Animal Ethics (2022). Invertebrate sentience: A review of the neuroscientific literature. Animal Ethics is a 501(c)(3) public charity. https://www.animal-ethics.org/invertebrate-sentience-a-review-of-the-neuroscientific-literature/

  • Arnellos, A., & Moreno, A. (2022). Cognitive functions are not reducible to biological ones: the case of minimal visual perception. Biology & Philosophy, 37(35), https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-022-09860-3.

  • Birch, J., Burn, C., Schnell, A., Browning, H., & Crump, A. (2021). Review of the evidence of sentience in cephalopod molluscs and decapod crustaceans. LSE Consulting. LSE Enterprise Ltd. The London School of Economics and Political Science.

  • Cross, F., Jackson, R., & Grace, R. (2020). Arthropod Intelligence? The Case for Portia. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.568049

  • Eberhard, W. G., & Wcislo, W. T. (2012). Plenty of room at the bottom? Scientific American, 100, 226–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, D., & Seth, A. (2009). Animal consciousness: a synthetic approach. Trends in Neurosciences, 32(9), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.008. 476 – 84.

  • Ginsburg, S., & Jablonka, E. (2019). The evolution of the sensitive soul: learning and the Origins of consciousness. MIT Press.

  • Godfrey-Smith, P. (2013). Cephalopods and the evolution of the mind. Pacific Conservation Biology, 19(1), 4–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith, P. (2016). Other minds: the Octopus, the Sea, and the Deep Origins of consciousness. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

  • Hickman, C., Roberts, L., & Larson, A. (2001). Integrated Principles of Zoology. MacGraw-Hill.

  • Hochner, B., Shomrat, T., & Fiorito, G. (2006). The octopus: a model for a comparative analysis of the evolution of learning and memory mechanisms. Biological Bulletin, 210, 308–317.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofstadter, D. (2007). I am a strange Loop. Basic Books.

  • Jablonka, E., & Ginsburg, S. (2022). Learning and the evolution of conscious agents. Biosemiotics, 15(3), 401–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaitly, R., Ehmsten, E., Hedlund, J., Cant, M., Lehmann, P., & Hayward, A. (2022). The evolution of predator avoidance in cephalopods: A case of brain over brawn?Frontiers. Marine. Science 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fmars.2022.909192.

  • Jenkins, P. M., Kim, N., Jones, S. L., Tseng, W. C., Svitkina, T. M., Yin, H. H., & Bennett, V. (2014). Giant ankyrin-G: a critical innovation in vertebrate evolution of fast and integrated neuronal signaling. PNAS, 112(4), 957–964.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kerkova, K., Marhounova, L., Polonyiova, A., & Nemec, P. (2022). The evolution of brain neuron numbers in amniotes. PNAS, 119(11), e2121624119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.212162411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levi-Montalcini, R. (1999). La Galassia Mente. Baldini and Castoldi.

  • Monahan-Earley, R., Dvorak, A. M., & Aird, W. C. (2013). Evolutionary origins of the blood vascular system and endothelium. Journal Of Thrombosis And Haemostasis, 11(1), 46–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/jth.12253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, A., & Lasa, A. (2003). From Basic Adaptivity to early mind. The origin and evolution of cognitive capacities. Evolution and Cognition, 9(1), 12–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nilsson, S., & Holmgren, S. (Eds.). (1994). Comparative Physiology and Evolution of the Autonomic Nervous System. Harwood Academic Publishers.

  • O’Dor, R. (2002). Telemetered cephalopod energetics: swimming, soaring, and blimping. Integrative and comparative biology, 42(5), 1065–1070. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/42.5.1065.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Dor, R. K., & Webber, D. M. (2011). The constraints on cephalopods: why squid aren’t fish. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 64(8), 1591–1605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seibel, B. A. (2016). Cephalopod susceptibility to asphyxiation via ocean incalescence, deoxygenation, and acidification. Physiology, 31, 418–429.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shigeno, S., Sasaki, T., Moritaki, T., Kasugai, T., Vecchione, M., & Agata, K. (2008). Evolution of the cephalopod head complex by assembly of multiple molluscan body parts: evidence from Nautilus embryonic development. Journal of morphology, 269, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1002/jmor.10564.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shimeld, S. M., & Holland, P. W. H. (2000). Vertebrate Innovations. PNAS, 97(9), 4449-4452.10.1073/pnas.97.9.4449.

  • Smith, M. R., & Caron, J. B. (2010). Primitive soft-bodied cephalopods from the Cambrian. Nature, 465, 427–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storer, T., Usinger, R., Stebbins, R., & Nybakken, J. (1979). General Zoology. McGraw-Hill.

  • Strausfeld, N. J., Ma, X., & Edgecombe, G. D. (2016). Fossils and the evolution of the Arthropod Brain. Current Biology, 26(20), 989–1000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2022). The evolution of Agency: behavioral organization from lizards to humans. MIT Press.

  • Van Niekerk, G., Davis, T., & Engelbrecht, A. M. (2015). Was the evolutionary road towards adaptive immunity paved with endothelium? Biology Direct, 10(47), https://doi.org/10.1186/s13062-015-0079-0.

  • Villabuena-Rueda, A., Erice, C., Pardo, C. A., & Stins, M. C. (2019). The Evolving Concept of the Blood Brain Barrier (BBB): From a Single Static Barrier to a Heterogeneous and Dynamic Relay Center. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience (13) https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2019.00405.

Download references

Funding

The author acknowledges Kepa Ruiz-Mirazo for critical remarks and suggestions after his careful reading of previous versions of this commentary, and funding from the John Templeton Foundation (research project ‘Agency, Directionality & Function’ - Science of Purpose).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

I am the unique author.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alvaro Moreno.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The author has not conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Moreno, A. Some Reflections on the Evolution of Conscious Agents: The Relevance of body Plans. Biosemiotics 16, 35–43 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-023-09525-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-023-09525-y

Keywords

Navigation