Skip to main content
Log in

Susceptibility to the ‘Dark Side’ of Goal-Setting: Does Moral Justification Influence the Effect of Goals on Unethical Behavior?

  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Setting goals in the workplace can motivate improved performance but it might also compromise ethical behavior. In this paper, we propose that individual differences in the dispositional tendency to morally justify behavior moderate the effects of specific performance goals on unethical behavior. We conducted an experimental study in which working participants, who were randomly assigned to a specific goal condition or to a condition with a vague goal that lacked a specific target (i.e., ‘do your best’), completed two tasks in which they had the opportunity to act unethically. In an ethical dilemma task, participants in the specific goal condition were more likely to advocate using unethical methods. However, in an anagram task, only those with high moral justification overstated their performance to a greater degree in the specific goal condition. As such, individuals may not be equally susceptible to the ‘dark side’ of goal-setting.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In this paper, we view moral justification as a form of moral disengagement, i.e., as a form of motivated reasoning that enables individuals to justify or excuse unethical conduct (see, e.g., Bandura 1991, 1999). Others within the sensemaking and moral intuition literature (e.g., Haidt 2001) use the term in a slightly different way, to refer to post hoc rationalizations of one’s behavior.

  2. Sensemaking models argue that cognitive processes of moral reasoning are not necessarily causal factors in ethical decision-making and may be engaged at a later phase after unethical behavior has been enacted, as a means of explaining or justifying actions (e.g., Sonenshein 2007). However, they allow that it is possible for moral justifications to be engaged earlier and to override the instinctive judgments that drive unethical behavior.

  3. The exact response rates for the study cannot be stated with certainty. Around 20 employees from the car rental firm were invited to participate in the study (one business unit), while the number invited from the electricity provider was in the region of 100–150.

  4. A repeated measures ANOVA comparing the total number of words overstated across the three rounds of the anagram task (M = 1.32, SD = 3.32) to the total number of words understated (M = 0.06, SD = 0.27), was significant, F(1,105) = 18.35, p < 0.01, \(\eta_{\text{p}}^{2}\) = 0.15, indicating that overstatements of performance were likely to reflect intentional cheating as opposed to carelessness in responding.

  5. A possible explanation for the lack of main effect of goal condition on performance overstatements within the anagram task is that participants in the specific goal condition may not have been that committed to achieving the target they were set of creating nine words. However, in Schweitzer et al.’s (2004) study, there were no significant differences in goal commitment between participants who did and did not overstate their performance at least once in their goal conditions, suggesting that our lack of main effect is not simply an indicator of poor commitment.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Sage.

  • Aquino, K., & Reed, A., II (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1423–1440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashforth, B. E., & Anand, V. (2003). The normalization of corruption in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 25, 1–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Association of Certified Fraud Examiners (2012). 2012 Report to the Nation on Occupational Fraud and Abuse. Retrieved 12 May, 2014, from http://www.acfe.com/uploadedFiles/ACFE_Website/Content/rttn/2012-report-to-nations.pdf.

  • Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 248–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3(3), 193–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 364–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsky, A. (2008). Understanding the ethical cost of organizational goal-setting: A review and theory development. Journal of Business Ethics, 81(1), 63–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsky, A. (2011). Investigating the effects of moral disengagement and participation on unethical work behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 104(1), 59–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brief, A. P., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1986). Prosocial organizational behaviors. Academy of Management Review, 11(4), 710–725.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cadsby, C. B., Song, F., & Tapon, F. (2010). Are you paying your employees to cheat? An experimental investigation. The BE Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy, 10(1), 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catania, G. (2014). The unintended consequences of motivational techniques: Goal-setting and unethical behavior in the Maltese financial services industry. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 109(8), 1375–1385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colbert, A. E., Mount, M. K., Harter, J. K., Witt, L. A., & Barrick, M. R. (2004). Interactive effects of personality and perceptions of the work situation on workplace deviance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(4), 599–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ethics Resource Centre. (2014). 2013 National Business Ethics Survey of the US Workforce. Retrieved 12 May, 2014 from http://www.ethics.org/downloads/2013NBESFinalWeb.pdf.

  • Fritzsche. (2000). Ethical climates and the ethical dimension of decision making. Journal of Business Ethics, 24(2), 125–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F., Schweitzer, M. E., Mead, N. L., & Ariely, D. (2011). Unable to resist temptation: How self-control depletion promotes unethical behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115(2), 191–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, J., & Bromiley, P. (2007). Incentives to cheat: The influence of executive compensation and firm performance on financial misrepresentation. Organization Science, 18(3), 350–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, M. C. (2003). Paying people to lie: The truth about the budgeting process. European Financial Management, 9(3), 379–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, G. E., & Kavanagh, M. J. (1996). An experimental examination of the effects of individual and situational factors on unethical behavioral intentions in the workplace. Journal of Business Ethics, 15(5), 511–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kish-Gephart, J. J., Harrison, D. A., & Treviño, L. K. (2010). Bad apples, bad cases, and bad barrels: Meta-analytic evidence about sources of unethical decisions at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Latham, G. P., & Locke, E. A. (2006). Enhancing the benefits and overcoming the pitfalls of goal setting. Organizational Dynamics, 35(4), 332–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewicki, R. J. (1983). Lying and deception: A behavioural model. In M. H. Bazerman & R. J. Lewicki (Eds.), Negotiating in organizations (pp. 68–90). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, P. V. (1985). Defining ‘business ethics’: Like nailing jello to a wall. Journal of Business Ethics, 4(5), 377–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57(9), 705–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2013). New developments in goal setting theory. East Sussex, UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loe, T. W., Ferrell, L., & Mansfield, P. (2000). A review of empirical studies assessing ethical decision-making in business. Journal of Business Ethics, 25(3), 185–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, S. R., Kish-Gephart, J. J., & Detert, J. R. (2014). Blind forces: Ethical infrastructures and moral disengagement in organizations. Organizational Psychology Review, 4(4), 295–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murnighan, J. K., Cantelon, D. A., & Elyashiv, T. (2001). Bounded personal ethics and the tap dance of real estate agency. Advances in qualitative organizational research, 3, 1–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ordóñez, L. D., Schweitzer, M. E., Galinsky, A. D., & Bazerman, M. H. (2009). On good scholarship, goal-setting, and scholars gone wild. Academy of Management Perspectives, 23(3), 82–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and theory. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, S. L., & Bennett, R. J. (1995). A typology of deviant workplace behaviors: A multidimensional scaling study. Academy of Management Journal, 38(2), 555–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sackett, P. R., Burris, L. R., & Callahan, C. (1989). Integrity testing for personnel selection: An update. Personnel Psychology, 42(3), 491–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schweitzer, M., Ordóñez, L. D., & Douma, B. (2004). Goal setting as a motivator of unethical behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 47(3), 422–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shalvi, S., Dana, J., Handgraaf, M. J., & De Dreu, C. K. (2011). Justified ethicality: Observing desired counterfactuals modifies ethical perceptions and behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115(2), 181–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shantz, A., & Latham, G. (2011). The effect of primed goals on employee performance: Implications for human resource management. Human Resource Management, 50(2), 289–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sonenshein, S. (2007). The role of construction, intuition, and justification in responding to ethical issues at work: The sensemaking-intuition model. Academy of Management Review, 32(4), 1022–1040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staw, B. M., & Boettger, R. D. (1990). Task revision: A neglected form of work performance. Academy of Management Journal, 33(3), 534–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Street, M. D., Douglas, S. C., Geiger, S. W., & Martinko, M. J. (2001). The impact of cognitive expenditure on the ethical decision-making process: The cognitive elaboration model. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86(2), 256–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terpstra, D. E., Rozell, E. J., & Robinson, R. K. (1993). The influence of personality and demographic variables on ethical decisions related to insider trading. Journal of Psychology, 127(4), 375–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treviño, L. K., & Brown, M. E. (2004). Managing to be ethical: Debunking five business ethics myths. Academy of Management Executive, 18(2), 69–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treviño, L. K., & Youngblood, S. A. (1990). Bad apples in bad barrels: A causal analysis of ethical decision-making behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75(4), 378–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, D., & Ordóñez, L. (2014a). Conscience without cognition: The effects of subconscious priming on ethical behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 57(3), 723–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, D. T., & Ordóñez, L. D. (2014b). The dark side of consecutive high performance goals: Linking goal setting, depletion, and unethical behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 123(2), 79–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karen Niven.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Niven, K., Healy, C. Susceptibility to the ‘Dark Side’ of Goal-Setting: Does Moral Justification Influence the Effect of Goals on Unethical Behavior?. J Bus Ethics 137, 115–127 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-015-2545-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-015-2545-0

Keywords

Navigation