Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter Mouton October 24, 2018

Standing up to the canoe: Competing cognitive biases in the encoding of stative spatial relations in a language with a single spatial preposition

  • Åshild Næss EMAIL logo
From the journal Cognitive Linguistics

Abstract

This paper discusses how verbal directional markers are used to encode stative spatial relations in the Oceanic language Äiwoo. It argues that the apparent reversal of directional meaning in stative expressions, where ‘up’ is used in expressions meaning ‘underneath’, ‘down’ in expressions meaning ‘above’, and ‘out’ in expressions meaning ‘inside’, can be explained by a fictive motion analysis where the figure is construed as metaphorically moving towards the ground. It moreover argues that in expressions where motion leads to a resulting spatial configuration, where ‘up’ means ‘on top of’ rather than ‘underneath’, this reading is overridden by the so-called goal bias, whereby the resultant configuration is more cognitively salient than the motion producing it. It suggests that the linguistic construal of stative spatial relations may to some extent be correlated with the formal means of expression, where marking by adpositions favours a ‘search domain’ construal whereas encoding within the verb favours a ‘fictive path’ construal. It thus provides a new angle on the linguistic encoding of spatial relations, an area which has been subject to much research within cognitive linguistics, but which so far has paid little attention to the possibility of encoding stative spatial relations within the verb.

Acknowledgements

The author would like to thank the participants at the 9th Austronesian and Papuan Languages and Linguistics conference and three anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on earlier versions. Moreover, I gratefully acknowledge the support of the Endangered Languages Documentation Programme through a Small Grant (grant no. SG0308) which funded the collection and analysis of part of the data on which this work builds. Earlier fieldwork was funded by the Norwegian Research Council, project no. 148717.

Abbreviations

A

actor voice

ART

article

ATTR

attributive

AUG

augmented number

BN

bound noun

CENTRIP

centripetal

COM.ART

common article

COM.LOC

common locative proclitic

CONJ

conjunction

CS

change of state

CV

circumstantial voice

DEF

default aspect

DEM

demonstrative

DIR

directional

DIST

distal

EVIT

evitative mood

FUT

future

IPFV

imperfective

IRR

irrealis

LD

locative-directional

LOC

locative

MIN

minimal number

NMLZ

nominaliser

O

undergoer voice

PFV

perfective

POSS

possessive

PREF

prefix (function unclear)

PRES

presentative

PRO:PP

pro-form for prepositional phrase

PROH

prohibitive

PROX

proximal

UA

unit-augmented number

References

Ameka, Felix, Carlien de Witte & David Wilkins. 1999. Picture series for positional verbs: Eliciting the verbal component in locative descriptions. In David Wilkins (ed.), Manual for the 1999 field season, 48–54. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Ameka, Felix & Stephen C. Levinson. 2007. The typology and semantics of locative predicates. [Special issue]. Linguistics 45(5–6). 847–871.10.1515/LING.2007.025Search in Google Scholar

Báez, Gabriela Pérez & Jürgen Bohnemeyer. 2008. Object to path in Mesoamerica: Semantic composition of locative and motion descriptions in Yucatec Maya and Juchitán Zapotec. In Rosa María Ortiz Ciscomani (ed.), Memorias del IX Encuentro Internacional de Lingüística en el Noroeste, Vol. 2. 269–284. Hermosillo, Sonora: Editorial UniSon.Search in Google Scholar

Bohnemeyer, Jürgen, Nicholas J. Enfield, James Essegbey, Iraide Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Sotaro Kita, Friederike Lüpke & Felix K. Ameka. 2007. Principles of event segmentation in language: The case of motion events. Language 83(3). 495–532.10.1353/lan.2007.0116Search in Google Scholar

Bowerman, Melissa, Marianne Gullberg, Asifa Majid & Bhuvana Narasimhan. 2004. Put project: The cross-linguistic encoding of placement events. In Asifa Majid (ed.), Field manual, vol. 9. 10–24. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Bowerman, Melissa & Asifa Majid. 2003. Kids’ cut & break. In Nicholas J. Enfield (ed.), Field research manual 2003, part I: Multimodal interaction, space, event representation, 70–71. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Brown, Penelope. 1994. The INs and ONs of Tzeltal locative expressions: The semantics of static descriptions of locations. Linguistics 32(4–5). 743–790.10.1515/ling.1994.32.4-5.743Search in Google Scholar

Brown, Penelope. 2006. A sketch of the grammar of space in Tzeltal. In Stephen C. Levinson & David Wilkins (eds.), Grammars of space: Explorations in cognitive diversity, 230–272. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511486753.008Search in Google Scholar

Cablitz, Gabriele H. 2006. Marquesan: A grammar of space. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110197754Search in Google Scholar

Cauchard, Aurélie. 2014. A study of space in Caac, an Oceanic language spoken in the north of New Caledonia. Manchester, UK: University of Manchester dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Cauchard, Aurélie. ms. Seawards towards us … and back. Fuzzy boundaries between factive and fictive motion in a New Caledonian language. Unpublished manuscript.Search in Google Scholar

Foley, William A. & Mike Olson. 1985. Clausehood and verb serialization. In Johanna Nichols & Anthony C. Woodbury (eds.), Grammar inside and outside the clause: Some approaches to theory from the field, 17–60. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

François, Alexandre. 2004. Reconstructing the geocentric system of Proto-Oceanic. Oceanic Linguistics 43(1). 1–31.10.1353/ol.2004.0009Search in Google Scholar

Freeman, N. H., C. G. Sinha & J. A. Stedmon. 1980. The allative bias in 3-year-olds is almost proof against task naturalness. Journal of Child Language 8(2). 283–296.10.1017/S0305000900003196Search in Google Scholar

Frostad, Benedicte H. 2012. A grammar of Ughele. Nijmegen: Radboud University dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Hammarström, Harald, Sebastian Bank, Robert Forkel & Martin Haspelmath. Glottolog 3.2. http://glottolog.org/ (accessed 14 February 2018).Search in Google Scholar

Hawkins, Bruce W. 1981. Variable temporal integration between motional verbs and locational prepositions. Linguistic Notes from La Jolla 10. 98–127.Search in Google Scholar

Hellwig, Birgit & Friederike Lüpke. 2001. Caused positions. In Stephen C. Levinson & Nicholas J. Enfield (eds.), Manual for the field season 2001, 126–128. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Lakusta, Laura & Barbara Landau. 2005. Starting at the end: The importance of goals in spatial language. Cognition 96(1). 1–33.10.1016/j.cognition.2004.03.009Search in Google Scholar

Langacker, Ronald W. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar. Vol. I: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Levinson, Stephen C. 2003. Space in language and cognition: Explorations in cognitive diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511613609Search in Google Scholar

Lynch, John, Malcolm Ross & Terry Crowley. 2002. The Oceanic languages. Richmond: Curzon.Search in Google Scholar

Malau, Catriona. 2016. A grammar of Vurës, Vanuatu. Boston: de Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9781501503641Search in Google Scholar

Matlock, Teenie. 2004. The conceptual motivation of fictive motion. In Günter Radden & Klaus-Uwe Panther (eds.), Studies in linguistic motivation, 221–248. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

Mosel, Ulrike & Even Hovdhaugen. 1992. Samoan reference grammar. Oslo: Scandinavian University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Næss, Åshild. 2015. Voice at the crossroads: Symmetrical clause alternations in Äiwoo, Reef Islands, Solomon Islands. Oceanic Linguistics 54(1). 270–307.10.1353/ol.2015.0000Search in Google Scholar

Næss, Åshild. 2017. A short dictionary of Äiwoo. Canberra: Asia-Pacific Linguistics. https://openresearch-repository.anu.edu.au/handle/1885/112469 (accessed 27 August 2018).Search in Google Scholar

Næss, Åshild & Mathias Jenny. 2011. Who changes language? Bilingualism and structural change in Burma and the Reef Islands. Journal of Language Contact 4. 217–249.10.1163/187740911X589253Search in Google Scholar

Newman, John (ed.). 2002. The linguistics of sitting, standing and lying. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.51Search in Google Scholar

Regier, Terry & Mingyu Zheng. 2007. Attention to endpoints: A cross-linguistic constraint on spatial meaning. Cognitive Science 31(4). 705–719.10.1080/15326900701399954Search in Google Scholar

Ross, Malcolm & Åshild Næss. 2007. An Oceanic origin for Äiwoo, the language of the Reef Islands? Oceanic Linguistics 46(2). 456–498.10.1353/ol.2008.0003Search in Google Scholar

Staden, Miriam van, Gunter Senft, Nicholas J. Enfield & Jürgen Bohnemeyer. 2001. Staged events. In Stephen C. Levinson & Nicholas. J. Enfield (eds.), Manual for the field season 2001, 115–125. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Talmy, Leonard. 1983. How language structures space. In Herbert L. J. Pick & Linda P. Acredolo (eds.), Spatial orientation: Theory, research, and application, 225–282. New York: Plenum Press.10.1007/978-1-4615-9325-6_11Search in Google Scholar

Talmy, Leonard. 1985. Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In Timothy Shopen (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description. Vol. III: Grammatical categories and the lexicon, 57–149. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Talmy, Leonard. 1991. Path to realization: A typology of event conflation. In Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: General session and parasession on the grammar of event structure, 480–519.10.3765/bls.v17i0.1620Search in Google Scholar

Talmy, Leonard. 2000. Toward a cognitive semantics. Vol. I: Concept structuring systems. Cambrige, MA: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/6847.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Thiering, Martin. 2011. Figure-ground reversals in language. Gestalt Theory 33(3/4). 245–276.Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2017-08-08
Revised: 2018-04-05
Accepted: 2018-04-08
Published Online: 2018-10-24
Published in Print: 2018-11-27

© 2018 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 11.6.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/cog-2017-0096/html
Scroll to top button